Fernando - Burton2009
Fernando - Burton2009
Fernando - Burton2009
Key Words older adults with no cognitive impairment on the EPT and
Alzheimer’s disease ⴢ Amnesia ⴢ Cognitive function ⴢ the SIB-R (both self- and informant-report versions). The
Everyday problem solving ⴢ Instrumental activities of daily multiple-domain MCI participants also demonstrated poor-
living ⴢ Mild cognitive impairment ⴢ Multiple-domain er IADLs than MCI participants with impairments in a single
impairment cognitive domain on the self-reported SIB-R and EPT. The
single-domain MCI groups demonstrated poorer IADLs than
older adults without cognitive impairment on the infor-
Abstract mant-reported SIB-R and EPT. No significant group differ-
Background: A classification scheme and general set of cri- ences were found on the Lawton and Brody IADL Scale. Us-
teria for diagnosing mild cognitive impairment (MCI) were ing the EPT and SIB-R as predictors in a multinomial regres-
recently proposed by a multidisciplinary group of experts sion analysis, MCI group status was reliably predicted, but
who met at an international symposium on MCI. One of the the classification rate was poor. Conclusion: Individuals
proposed criteria included preserved basic activities of daily with MCI demonstrated poorer IADL functioning compared
living and minimal impairment in complex instrumental to cognitively intact older adults. However, the changes in
activities of daily living (IADLs). Objective: To investigate IADL functioning observed in MCI may be too subtle to be
whether older adults with MCI classified according to the detected by certain measures, such as the Lawton and Brody
subtypes identified by the Working Group (i.e. amnestic, sin- IADL Scale. Copyright © 2009 S. Karger AG, Basel
gle non-memory domain, and multiple domain with or with-
out a memory component) differed from cognitively intact
older adults on a variety of measures indexing IADLs and to
examine how well measures of IADL predict concurrent MCI Instrumental Activities of Daily Living and Everyday
status. Methods: Two hundred and fifty community-dwell- Problem Solving in Older Adults with Mild Cognitive
ing older adults, ranging in age from 66 to 92, completed Impairment
self-report measures of IADLs (Lawton and Brody IADL Scale,
Scales of Independent Behaviour-Revised – SIB-R) and a Although interest in studying the intermediate state
measure of everyday problem solving indexing IADLs (Ev- between normal, healthy cognition and dementia has in-
eryday Problems Test – EPT). Ratings of participants’ IADL creased substantially over the past decade, much contro-
functioning were also obtained from informants (e.g. spouse,
adult child and friend). Results: Older adults with multiple-
domain MCI demonstrated poorer IADL functioning than Esther Strauss passed away on June 17, 2009.
198.143.54.65 - 1/27/2016 7:16:10 AM
www.karger.com www.karger.com/ger Tel. +1 250 721 6108, Fax +1 250 721 8929, E-Mail [email protected]
versy surrounds this area of research. Interest in this state, executive function and visuospatial skills (multiple-
often referred to as mild cognitive impairment (MCI), domain MCI), and (3) MCI with impairment in a sin-
originated from a desire to identify individuals at risk for gle non-memory domain (single non-memory-domain
developing Alzheimer’s disease (AD). Thus, conceptually, MCI). The multiple-domain MCI is further subdivided
the term MCI is often used to refer to a transitional zone into those with a memory impairment (multiple-domain
between normal cognitive function and early AD, or a MCI with amnesia) and those without (multiple-domain
preclinical state of AD [1, 2]. However, studies examining MCI without amnesia). The different subtypes likely re-
conversion rates from MCI to AD have shown that, al- flect different aetiologies, including degenerative, vascu-
though individuals with MCI have a greater risk of pro- lar, psychiatric or trauma-related causes [2, 8, 20]. For
gressing to AD than normal older adults, not all individ- example, the amnestic subtype of MCI is thought to pri-
uals with MCI go on to develop AD [3–5]. For example, marily reflect a prodromal phase of AD [21], whereas in-
in a population-based sample of older adults, Tuokko et dividuals with single non-memory domain MCI may
al. [6] reported that 32% of individuals initially diagnosed have a higher likelihood of progression to non-AD forms
with cognitive impairment but no dementia were diag- of dementia, such as dementia with Lewy bodies or fron-
nosed with AD 5 years later while 15% were diagnosed totemporal dementia.
with other forms of dementia (e.g. vascular dementia or In September 2003, a multidisciplinary, international
Parkinson’s disease). In fact, some individuals with MCI group of experts on MCI met to discuss the current status
appear to recover with time. Fisk and Rockwood [7] found and future directions of MCI research [1]. The Working
that 20–30% of individuals with MCI and objective mem- Group also endorsed a conceptualization of MCI that ac-
ory impairment showed no cognitive impairment at fol- knowledged the heterogeneity of the clinical presentation
low-up 5 years later. These results have led some research- and potential aetiologies associated with MCI. They pro-
ers to suggest that MCI represents a heterogeneous disor- posed a general set of criteria for diagnosing MCI, which
der with multiple possible outcomes [8–10]. include (1) absence of dementia (defined by DSM-IV or
Differences in the conceptualization of this transi- ICD 10); (2) cognitive decline [defined by (a) self and/or
tional state between normal aging and dementia are re- informant report in addition to evidence of objective cog-
flected in the varying terms and criteria used to opera- nitive impairment and/or (b) decline on objective mea-
tionalize it. Some of the most commonly used terms have sures of cognitive functioning], and (3) preserved basic
included age-associated cognitive decline [11], cognitive activities of daily living (ADLs) and minimal impairment
impairment, no dementia [12, 13], MCI [14] and mild in complex instrumental functions. Once a diagnosis of
neurocognitive disorder [15], all of which are associated MCI is achieved, the clinical presentation can then be
with varying inclusion and exclusion criteria. Although classified according to one of three subtypes: amnestic,
all of these concepts share a common focus (i.e. identify- multiple domain (with or without a memory component)
ing cognitive impairment in individuals who do not meet and single non-memory domain.
criteria for a diagnosis of dementia) [16], differences in As the criteria for MCI have evolved from the original
criteria are important because they impact estimates of criteria of Petersen et al. [14] to the current criteria estab-
prevalence, incidence and conversion rates [17–19]. lished by the Working Group [1], a subtle change in word-
One particularly well-known classification system ing is evident in the criterion pertaining to functional
was developed by Petersen et al. [14], referred to as MCI. abilities. Their original criteria for MCI specified ‘normal
It was originally proposed to identify individuals at risk activities of daily living’ [p. 304, 14], whereas the revised
for developing AD and the criteria included (1) the pres- version is somewhat less stringent and specifies ‘essen-
ence of a memory complaint, (2) normal activities of dai- tially’ or ‘largely’ intact functional activities, allowing for
ly living, (3) normal general cognitive functioning, (4) ‘minor inconveniences in daily function’ due to a cogni-
memory impairment relative to age peers and (5) the ab- tive impairment [p. 189, 20]. The Winblad et al. [1] criteria
sence of dementia. Later, in recognition of the heteroge- require preserved basic ADLs but allow for minimal im-
neous clinical presentation of MCI, the original criteria pairment in complex instrumental ADLs (IADLs). The
were revised to include other types of cognitive impair- report provided by the Working Group also suggests that
ment beyond memory [2]. The most recent classification future research is required to determine which specific
[8, 20] identifies three MCI subtypes: (1) MCI with a domains of IADLs might be impaired in MCI, thus ac-
memory impairment (MCI amnestic); MCI with impair- knowledging the possibility that individuals with MCI
ment in multiple cognitive domains, such as language, may show mild impairments in IADLs.
198.143.54.65 - 1/27/2016 7:16:10 AM
Number 158 6 39 19 28
Age, years 73.5784.72 79.5085.65 77.5485.61 82.0085.04 79.5784.86
Years of education 16.2982.70 13.6783.93 14.2382.67 12.0082.71 12.7982.71
MMSE 28.9281.17 26.8382.48 28.6781.26 28.1681.26 28.6881.09
WAIS-III Block Designa 13.5982.85 13.8381.60 11.8782.78 11.7481.82 11.1482.66
WAIS-III Estimated FSIQ 125.10811.22 125.0086.75 117.51811.42 111.7989.01 113.3689.51
Estimated NAART IQ 118.5685.00 118.0583.48 115.2687.11 110.1588.81 112.3487.31
NCI = Not cognitively impaired; A-MCI = amnestic MCI; non-A MCI = single non-memory domain MCI; MD MCI + A = mul-
tiple-domain MCI with amnesia; MD MCI – A = multiple-domain MCI without amnesia.
a WAIS-III Age-Scaled Scores.
Measures likely that the omission of these earlier items had little impact on
Self-Reported IADLs overall estimates of functional independence. The internal consis-
Participants completed two self-report questionnaires assess- tency (Cronbach’s ␣) for the modified SIB-R for year 1 of the cur-
ing their capacity to perform a variety of IADLs. The Lawton and rent study was 0.92. One-year test-retest data were available for 210
Brody IADL scale [26] is a self-report measure indexing eight participants; the test-retest reliability was r = 0.80.
IADL domains: telephone use, food preparation, housekeeping,
laundry, transportation, shopping, financial management and Informant-Reported IADLs
medication use. Participants rated their level of independence in The Lawton and Brody IADL scale and the SIB-R were also
each of the eight IADL domains according to Likert scales rang- administered to individuals identified by each participant as a
ing from 0 to 2–4. The items were summed to provide a total score significant other. Informants willing to complete the measures
(total possible score = 31), with lower scores indicating higher lev- were available for 230 of the participants and included spouses
els of functional independence. (36.8%), friends (19.4%), adult children (15.8%), siblings (0.7%)
The Scales for Independent Behavior-Revised (SIB-R) [42] is a and other relationships (3.0%). The informants ranged in age
measure of functional independence, and adaptive and problem from 29 to 90 (M = 66.71, SD = 13.11) and had an average of 15.34
behaviours. The functional independence and adaptive behav- years of education (SD = 3.13, range 7–27). Most of the informants
iour component is comprised of 13 subscales organized into four had known their participant for 120 years (75.7%), while 8.7%
adaptive behaviour clusters: Motor (gross- and fine-motor skills), had known them for 10–20 years, 8.7% for 5–10 years, 5.2% for
Social Interaction and Communication (social interaction, lan- 3–5 years and 1.7% for ^2 years. The frequency of contact varied:
guage comprehension and language expression), Personal Living 38.2% daily and living in same household, 12.5% daily or almost
(eating and meal preparation, toileting, dressing, personal self- daily but living in separate households, 17.8% one to two times a
care and domestic skills) and Community Living (time and punc- week and 5.6% one to two times a month (1.6% unknown fre-
tuality, money and value, work skills and home/community ori- quency of contact).
entation). Each subscale contains items that span the entire devel- The internal consistency (Cronbach’s ␣) and test-retest reli-
opmental range, from early infancy to 80 years and older. Items ability coefficient for the informant-report SIB-R were 0.95 and
within each subscale are arranged in approximate developmental r = 0.84, respectively.
order, from the easiest to the hardest. For each item, individuals
are asked how well they are able to complete the task (or would be Paper and Pencil Measure
able to complete the task if they do not typically perform it), with- The Everyday Problems Test (EPT) [43] is a paper and pencil
out help or supervision, using a 4-point rating scale, ranging from measure of everyday cognitive competence that requires partici-
0 (never or rarely performs the task, even if asked) to 3 (almost pants to solve problems associated with daily living. Participants
always does the task very well without being asked). Raw scores are provided with 21 printed stimulus materials (e.g. medication
were calculated for each adaptive behaviour subscale by summing label and pay phone information) and asked to solve two problems
the ratings of all items, with higher scores indicating higher levels pertaining to each stimulus. All stimuli are reproductions of real-
of functional independence. life materials with four to six items representing one of the follow-
The SIB-R was originally designed as an informant-reported ing IADL domains: medication use, meal preparation, telephone
measure. However, for the purposes of the present study, it was ad- use, shopping, financial management, household management and
ministered directly to participants to provide a self-reported mea- transportation. For example, with respect to meal preparation, par-
sure of functional abilities. In addition, items pertaining to early ticipants are provided with a nutritional information chart for cere-
infancy and early childhood were omitted in order to shorten the als and are asked to determine how many calories are in a serving
length of time required for administration. As all of the partici- of cereal if whole milk is used instead of skim milk. For a question
pants were still relatively highly functioning (i.e. no dementia), it is in the transportation domain, participants are provided with a
198.143.54.65 - 1/27/2016 7:16:10 AM
2.0
Total score
1.5
1.0
0.5
0
NCI Amnestic MCI Multiple- Multiple- Single
domain MCI domain MCI non-memory
with amnesia without domain MCI
amnesia
2.0
Total score
1.5
1.0
0.5
0
NCI Amnestic MCI Multiple- Multiple- Single
domain MCI domain MCI non-memory
with amnesia without domain MCI
Fig. 1, 2. Self-reported (1) and informant- amnesia
reported Lawton and Brody IADLs (2) as a
function of cognitive status. 2
SIB-R, the NCI group was rated by their significant oth- higher intercorrelations with one another than with the
ers as having significantly better functional abilities than EPT.
the single-domain MCI groups [F (1, 219) = 7.40, p =
0.007] and multiple-domain MCI groups [F (1, 219) = Prediction of Group Membership
9.93, p = 0.002]. On the EPT, the NCI group obtained sig- A multinomial logistic regression analysis was per-
nificantly better scores than the single-domain MCI formed in order to examine whether the IADL variables
groups [F (1, 241) = 8.58, p = 0.004], who in turn obtained could predict concurrent MCI group status (i.e. MCI
significantly better scores than the multiple-domain MCI group status at year 3). Only those measures that showed
groups [F (1, 241) = 53.4, p ! 0.001]. significant group differences in the previous analyses
Given that the MCI groups also differed with respect (i.e. self-reported SIB-R, informant-reported SIB-R and
to NAART Estimated IQ, the analyses were also conduct- EPT) were used as predictor variables. A total of 224 par-
ed using NAART Estimated IQ as a covariate, along with ticipants had data for all three IADL measures. The sin-
age and education, and the same pattern of results was gle-domain MCI groups were collapsed into one group
obtained. (n = 43) and the multiple-domain impairment groups
Table 3 shows the correlations between the various were collapsed into another (n = 39).
IADL measures, using Pearson correlation coefficients. A test of the full model (i.e. with all 3 predictors) showed
The Lawton and Brody IADL and SIB-R measures show that the predictors, as a group, reliably predicted group
198.143.54.65 - 1/27/2016 7:16:10 AM
Total score
120
100
80
60
40
20
0
NCI Amnestic MCI Multiple- Multiple- Single
domain MCI domain MCI non-memory
with amnesia without domain MCI
amnesia
3
120
100
80
60
40
20
0
NCI Amnestic MCI Multiple- Multiple- Single
domain MCI domain MCI non-memory
Fig. 3, 4. Self-reported (3) and informant- with amnesia without domain MCI
reported SIB-R (4) as a function of cogni- amnesia
tive status. 4
25
20
15
10
5
0
NCI Amnestic MCI Multiple- Multiple- Single
domain MCI domain MCI non-memory
with amnesia without domain MCI
amnesia
Fig. 5. Performance on the EPT as a func-
tion of cognitive status.
198.143.54.65 - 1/27/2016 7:16:10 AM
membership [2 (6) = 96.40, p ! 0.001]. In addition, using Table 4. Adequacy of classification based on the logistic regres-
a deviance criterion, the model was not reliably different sion analysis model
from a perfect model, indicating that the predictors pro-
Observed Predicted
vided a good fit [2 (78) = 41.15, p = 1.00]. According to
the Nagelkerke R 2, the set of predictors accounted for NCI single- multiple-
domain MCI domain MCI
41.7% of the variance in group membership. The model
was significantly degraded by the removal of the self-re- NCI 137 0 5
ported SIB-R [2 (2) = 6.23, p = 0.044] and EPT [2 (2) = Single-domain MCI 30 5 8
56.91, p ! 0.001], suggesting that only these two pre- Multiple-domain MCI 12 4 23
dictors reliably distinguished between the MCI status
groups. The Wald statistic indicated that both the self-re-
ported SIB-R [Wald (1) = 37.74, p ! 0.001] and the EPT
[Wald (1) = 6.07, p = 0.014] reliably separated the multiple- types. Thus, this study represents the first study to exam-
domain MCI group from the NCI group, but that only the ine functional abilities in the different MCI subtypes.
EPT [Wald (1) = 18.81, p ! 0.001] reliably separated the In the present community-dwelling sample, approxi-
single-domain MCI group from the NCI group. mately one third of the participants (36.8%) were classi-
As shown in table 4, classification was excellent for the fied into one of the MCI subtypes. Amongst the MCI par-
NCI group (96.5% correct) but much poorer for the mul- ticipants, the most common classification was the single
tiple-domain MCI (59.0% correct) and the single-domain non-memory domain MCI subtype, with 42.4% of the
MCI groups (11.6%), for an overall correct classification MCI participants falling into this category. Approximate-
rate of 73.7%. Clearly, participants were overclassified in ly one half of the MCI participants were classified as one
the largest group: the NCI group. of the multiple-domain MCI subtypes (20.7% with mem-
ory impairment and 30.4% without memory impairment).
Only 6.5% of the total MCI sample was classified as the
Discussion amnestic subtype. Although the overall prevalence rate of
MCI in the present study is substantially higher than
The primary purpose of the present study was to ex- prevalence rates reported by previous studies [17, 19, 45],
tend previous research examining the relationship be- it is important to bear in mind that most studies have ex-
tween MCI and functional abilities by examining the re- amined the amnestic type of MCI. Thus, the current prev-
lationship between the Winblad et al. [1] MCI classifica- alence rate of 2.4% obtained for the amnestic MCI subtype
tion scheme and ability to perform IADLs in a group of is similar to estimated prevalence rates of 3.2 and 3.7%
community-dwelling older adults. The original MCI cri- reported by Ritchie et al. [19] and Kumar et al. [17], respec-
teria of Petersen et al. [14] focused on memory impair- tively, for amnestic MCI. Fisk et al. [45] found that the
ment with relative preservation of other cognitive do- prevalence rate for the amnestic subtype varied depend-
mains, and, thus, most research to date is based on this ing on the restrictiveness of the criteria used; they found
more narrow conceptualization of MCI. A broader con- that the prevalence rate increased from 1.03 to 3.02%
ceptualization of MCI has been adopted in recent years, when the requirements for subjective memory complaints
recognizing other areas of cognitive impairment [1, 20], and intact IADLs were eliminated. In contrast to the pres-
but, as of yet, little is known about these other MCI sub- ent study, which used psychometric cut-off points and a
198.143.54.65 - 1/27/2016 7:16:10 AM