Perceived Cognitive Load of Extended Reality Serious Educational Games About Climate Change

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 19

Docens Series in Education

ISSN: 2583-1054

Annetta & Newton, 2024


Volume 7, pp. 25-43
Received: 27th August 2023
Revised: 9th November 2023, 28th November 2023, 1st December 2023
Accepted: 14th September 2023
Date of Publication: 15th June 2024
This paper can be cited as: Annetta, L. & Newton, M. (2024). Perceived Cognitive Load of Extended Reality
Serious Educational Games about Climate Change. Docens Series in Education, 7, 25-43
This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-Noncommercial 4.0 International License.
To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/ or send a letter to
Creative Commons, PO Box 1866, Mountain View, CA 94042, USA.

PERCEIVED COGNITIVE LOAD OF EXTENDED REALITY


SERIOUS EDUCATIONAL GAMES ABOUT CLIMATE
CHANGE

Leonard Annetta, Ph.D.


Taft Distinguished Professor of Science Education, East Carolina University, Flanagan Bldg.,
Greenville, NC, 27858 USA
[email protected]

Mark Newton, Ph.D.


Assistant Professor of Science Education, East Carolina University, Flanagan Bldg., Greenville,
NC, 27858 USA
[email protected]

Abstract
Integrating extended reality (XR) into undergraduate classrooms is not a new concept. However,
comparing identical content in subdomains of XR is unique. This study compared two
undergraduate courses with objectives about climate change on the Outer Banks of North Carolina
coast at a large university in the Mid-Atlantic region of the United States. The purpose of these
courses was to examine human and environmental impacts of global climate change in a local
context. Investigating the challenges facing North Carolina barrier islands, the class took a 5-day
field trip to the Outer Banks of North Carolina and visited four sites where they used augmented
reality (MR) to learn about the impact on climate change at those respected locations. The

25
Docens Series in Education
ISSN: 2583-1054

comparison class immersed in virtual reality (iVR) of the four sites using the same information
provided in the MR. 24 (6 MR and 18 iVR) participants completed the National Aeronautics and
Space Agency Task Load Index (TLX) immediately after completion of either the respective MR or
iVR based game. Independent samples Mann-Whitney U testing rejected the null hypotheses for
temporal, effort, and performance only. An explanation for possible reasons for these results are
discussed.
Keywords
Extended Reality, Climate Change, Undergraduate, Science

1. Introduction
Climate change is a global phenomenon grounded in many social and scientific topics.
This is a topic that often conjures contentious arguments around its origin and human influence to
stop or at least slow its impact on the environment. Climate change’s complexity creates challenges
for providing authentic experiences for learners while making it a difficult concept to teach. As a
result, many do not get to see the impact of climate change beyond overly sensationalized videos
online leading to misconceptions about the causes and validity of climate change. International
science educators have identified the need to explicitly teach about climate change in K-12 science,
which was identified by the United Nations Educational, Scientific, and Cultural Organization
(UNESCO). UNESCO seeks to create innovative teaching approaches through interdisciplinary
practices (Mermer, 2010).
Controversial topics, like climate change, meld into a research paradigm known as
Socioscientific Issues (SSI). Implementing an SSI pedagogical approach addresses the goals set
forth by UNESCO. SSI is a pedagogical framework for which educators can introduce such
seemingly antagonistic topics (e.g., climate change, evolution, cloning, etc.) while affording the
learner a safe experience to access or collect data to make conclusions on their own and create
origins for an argument favoring a perspective within the issue. This framework incorporates
personally relevant and complex issues that are interdisciplinary and develop functional scientific
literacy where individuals consider various lines of evidence, weigh a range of arguments, and
consider the moral and ethical implications of potential resolutions to an issue (Zeidler & Newton,
2017). A further challenge is integrating SSI into a personalized, place-based learning experience.
It has been argued that embedding SSI instruction in a place-based course allows students to
become immersed in the community impacted by the contentious issue, which leads to greater

26
Docens Series in Education
ISSN: 2583-1054

personal relevance, perspective-taking, and development of empathy (Newton & Zeidler, 2020;
Sadler, 2009). Ubiquitous technologies have the potential to supplement SSI; especially in place-
based SSI.
Place-based SSI is not only challenging, but these experiences are expensive and often
logistically demanding. These experiences require an abundance of planning, resources, and can
be impacted by the weather (Dolphin et al., 2019; Zhao et al., 2020). To overcome these
difficulties, Extended Reality (XR), a term that overarches such technologies as Virtual Reality
(VR), Augmented Reality (AR), or Mixed Reality (MR), have potential to complement place-
based learning. It is also important to operationally define what we call, immersive Virtual Reality
(iVR). Many inexpensive VR applications provide the user a 360-degree image only. Although
interesting and fun, it is not completely immersive or often engaging. In iVR, there are 6-degrees
of viewpoint freedom in the virtual surroundings with audio and interactive hotspots creating a
interesting, fun, and engaging immersive user experience. In this study, we use the term iVR.
Serious Educational Games (SEG) (Annetta, 2008) are digital game-based experienced
designed for K-20 educational learners. The term is a derivative of Serious Games, which are
defined as games designed for purposes other than entertainment. The "serious" adjective refers to
video games used by defense, education, medicine, first responders, etc. industry sectors (Abt,
1970).
1.1. Research Questions
With respect to the immersive nature of a technology-enable and place-based SSI,
researchers must begin to question cognitive demands on the learners as they attempt to assimilate
new knowledge while learning how to navigate a technology. To this end, the research question of
this study became:
What is the perceived cognitive load of participants while engaged in place-based
Serous Educational Game delivered through either Mixed Reality or immersive Virtual Reality?

2. Rationale
2.1. Extended Reality (XR)

27
Docens Series in Education
ISSN: 2583-1054

The technologies that fall under XR are quickly becoming a more relevant topic than
ever because of the ubiquitous nature of the hardware used to deploy the experiences. In recent
years, smart device enabled AR and MR educational activities have been developed. Additionally,
low-cost VR experiences have also been adopted on smart phones or inexpensive VR headsets.
The use of XR has the potential to either immerse learners in a technology-rich environment
(AR/MR) at a physical location (place-based) and/or virtually take students to a physical location
(VR). Environmental issues are topics that easily assimilate into XR applications because these
issues often occur on large scales of both time and physical space, while also being potential
harmful to humans. The personal relevance and embodied cognitive of this combination of SSI
and XR is an untapped research paradigm for science teaching and learning.
In a study on VR immersion and interactivity, Petersen et. al., (2022) reported effects of
interactivity and/or immersion on cognitive load, situational interest, and a sense of physical
presence along with effects between immersion and interactivity on agency and embodied
learning. Other studies have suggested that XR has impacted student learning and indicators of
learning, such as self-efficacy while not imposing unnecessary cognitive demands during the
learning process (X. Huang et al., 2023).
As in this study, comparing cognitive impact of VR to AR or MR is becoming an
essential research agenda. To date, studies have shown VR to provide a greater sense of presence
when compared and is also more immersive and engaging when it comes to a sense of user
presence.to MR (Allcoat et al., 2021). AR, however, has shown to be a more effective medium for
delivering auditory information through spatial presence. A possible explanation could be due to
the increased cognitive demands associated with immersive experiences in VR (Huang et al.,
2019). In a 2019 study, AR produced more individual excitement and activation than VR even
though users felt a greater sense of presence in the VR condition (Giglioli et al., 2019). In an
exploratory study comparing VR to AR, (Newton, Annetta & Bressler, 2023) found the VR group
made better connections between the physical impacts of climate change and the social, political,
economic ramifications of climate change. This study began to shed light on the importance of
both instructional design and the design of the technology conditions and how cognitive load might
affect those conditions.

2.2. Cognitive Load

28
Docens Series in Education
ISSN: 2583-1054

Cognitive load research is well-established. Cognitive load is said to account for the key
reason for the ineffectiveness of problem solving because cognitive processes insufficiently
overlap and causes a great deal of cognitive processing during problem which is unavailable for
schema acquisition (Sweller, 1988). As technology and multimedia increasingly become popular
tools for teaching required school age content, cognitive load needs to be accounted for in the
instructional design.
Investigating cognitive load on XR users is necessary to inform how the user
experiences should be designed to access learners to acquire schema most effectively and
efficiently. Wenk (2023) found cognitive load did not differ when comparing VR to AR. The
instructional design in this study did, however, suggest VR was more motivating and usable than
AR. A potential resolution to this conundrum is stratifying learners based on their learning style.
Students who identify as visual learners tend to have better learning effectiveness than the verbal
type students when learning through XR conditions. Sequential and global learners showed no
learning differences by XR condition (Chen & Huang, 2023). AR also did not show learning gains
in conceptual knowledge; however, it nonetheless suggested a significant lower extraneous
cognitive load than traditional teaching and learning (Ibİlİ & Bİllİnghurst, 2019; Thees et al.,
2020).
Gender is another consideration as it pertains to instructional design using XR. Males
tend to perceive a relationship between ease of use and extraneous cognitive load while females
show a perceived ease of use with intrinsic cognitive load. Females also show a perceived natural
interaction strongly correlated with perceived usefulness. Both genders perceived ease of use and
germane cognitive load similarly, however (Ibİlİ & Bİllİnghurst, 2019). Removing gender as a
variable, VR has a marked effect on extraneous and germane cognitive load, but no influence on
intrinsic cognitive load (Y. C. Chen et al., 2022). Overall, motor learning has a direct effect on
cognitive load in VR that often result in learning and long-term motor memory (Juliano et al.,
2022).

3. Methodology
3.1. Setting

29
Docens Series in Education
ISSN: 2583-1054

This study took place during the spring semester of 2023 at a large southeastern
university in the United States. Two groups of students engaged with four locations of the Outer
Banks (OBX) barrier islands in North Carolina (Figure 1) through either an iVR condition or an
MR condition. Each condition was a game-based scenario where students received in world points
or artifact collection based on interaction with science content designed to teach about coastal
resiliency including unique geological and historical sites from the chronological impacts of
climate change in each location. The OBX physical locations were Jockey’s Ridge State Park,
Jennette’s Pier, Oregon Inlet, and Cape Hatteras. These locations highlighted the fragility of these
islands from human and storm impacts. A reoccurring theme for the experience was also discussing
the role resiliency strategies like bridge construction near impacted areas and on-going road
maintenance caused by sea level rise and increased storm frequency as a result of climate change.
As students experienced the delicate ecosystems, they made cross-curricular connections and
informed decisions about the complex issues facing North Carolina coastal communities.

Figure 1: The North Carolina Outer Banks Barrier Island Chain


(Source: https://www.visitob.com/plan-your-trip/outer-banks-nc-map/)

While on the islands, students first used a mobile MR SEG created in Adobe Aero for each of the
four locations. Each MR SEG had embedded events (e.g., images, videos, etc.) to teach about

30
Docens Series in Education
ISSN: 2583-1054

specific aspects of climate change impact on the given location (figure 2). These embedded events
provided historical data as well as future projections for the barrier islands based on current climate
change data. Each student engaged with the MR SEG through their personal mobile phone that
had enabled cellular data. A second group of students were not able to make the 2-hour trip to the
place-based locations. A second group of students were not able to make the 2-hour trip to the
place-based locations. This group interacted with the exact same content but in an iVR SEG
condition created in WondaVR. The iVR SEGs were created using 360-degree images with the
same events embedded at the same four locations in the OBX as the MR SEGs and deployed
through a Meta Quest 2 headset. Ambient sound of wind and sea birds were included in the iVR
game design to build a sense of presence and this auditory addition would complement the nature
sounds the MR group experienced. Ambient sound of wind and sea birds were included in the iVR
game design to build a sense of presence and this auditory addition would complement the nature
sounds the MR group experienced.

Figure 2: Screen Capture from the iVR SEG Depicting Embedded Science Content
(Source: Self from iVR Game)

The MR game challenged players to walk the four locations in the OBX and unlock science content
with the goal of collecting gold coins left by pirates. Each location had a minimum of three content
artifacts in the form of a still image, video, or combination of the two. Players knew they had
unlocked all content when all the coins were collected in the given location (figure 3).

31
Docens Series in Education
ISSN: 2583-1054

Figure 3: Gold coin collection at Cape Hatteras light house.


(Source: Self from MR Game)

Because the WondaVR software had an interface that greatly differed from Adobe Aero, coin
collecting was not an option to embed in the game logic. Instead, WondaVR allows the designer
to create a scoreboard for the player and thus, gold coin collection in MR became a simple point
total in the iVR game (figure 4).

Figure 4: iVR SEG scoreboard at the Jennette’s Pier Location


(Source: Self from iVR Game)

32
Docens Series in Education
ISSN: 2583-1054

3.2. Participants
Six MR condition student participants (4 female and 2 male) enrolled in an
undergraduate honors seminar course traveled to experience the onsite locations and interacted in
the MR SEG at each location. The focus of the course was on coastal resiliency to climate change
on the Outer Banks of North Carolina. This topic was intentionally selected by the researchers to
align the SSI’s requirement that issues should be personally relevant to students, who in this case,
live in proximity to the OBX. The course met weekly on campus for the first seven weeks of the
semester for 75 minutes per meeting and then spent 5 days in the Outer Banks. The second group
of 18 iVR condition students (12 female and 6 male) from an undergraduate earth science for
teachers’ class was not able to travel to the OBX and thus, met for two 75-minute classes per week
in a traditional classroom environment. These students engaged in the iVR SEG that mirrored the
content and interactions in MR SEGs. It is worth noting that there was no mention of either
technology condition in the course description, meaning students’ decision to enroll in either
course was not influenced by the presence of technology.
3.3. Data Collection
Cognitive load data was collected from each study participant upon completing each XR
condition using the National Aeronautics and Space Administration (NASA) Task Load Index
(TLX) (Hart & Staveland, 1988). The NASA TLX is a multi-dimensional rating procedure
providing an overall workload score based on weighted average of ratings on 6 subscales (Table
1). Participants used a sliding scale within a Qualtrics online survey instrument to illustrate their
perceived workload magnitude from 0-100/low-high for each subscale.
Ratings of factors participants deem most important in creating workload for them
during their experience of a task are given more weight in computing overall workload. This two-
part evaluation consists of both self-reported weights and magnitude ratings for each subscale.
These weights account for two potential sources of inter-rater variability in the sources of the
workload between tasks. To calculate the 15 possible pair-wise comparisons of the 6 subscales,
each pair must designate the subscale of each pair provided as part of the index that contributed
more to their workload of that task.

33
Docens Series in Education
ISSN: 2583-1054

Table 1: NASA TLX Subscale Definitions


Subscale Definition
Mental Demand How much mental/perceptual (thinking, deciding, calculating,
remembering, looking, searching) activity was required? Was the task
easy or demanding; simple or complex; exacting or forgiving?
Physical Demand How much physical activity (pushing, pulling, turning, controlling,
activating) was required? Was the task easy or demanding; slow or brisk;
slack or strenuous; restful or laborious?
Temporal Demand How much time pressure did you feel due to the rate or pace at which the
tasks occurred? Was the pace slow and leisurely; rapid and frantic?
Self-Performance How successful do you think you were in accomplishing the goals of the
task set by the experience? How satisfied were you with your
performance in accomplishing these goals?
Self-Effort How hard did you have to work (mentally and physically) to accomplish
your level of performance?
Frustration Level How insecure, discouraged, irritated, stressed and/or annoyed are you
versus feeling secure, gratified, content, relaxed, and complacent during
the task?
(Source: Hart & Staveland 1988)
3.4. Data Analysis
The overall workload score is calculated by multiplying each rating by the weight given
to the factor by that participant. The sum of the weighted ratings for each task is divided by the
15 possible pair-wise comparisons (the sum of the weights). The NASA TLX requires a tally sheet
to be constructed to collect each time a participant indicates information on the subscales for each
task to compute the weighting. Because we asked participants in this study to complete the NASA
TLX immediately upon completing the entire XR condition, each subscale was given an equal
weight in the overall workload calculation.
The mean average of the load ratings was taken from the XR condition group on each
subscale. The weighted mean averages were compared per the NASA TLX manual and due to the
low N, non-parametric statistical analyses were performed. Independent samples Mann-Whitney
U testing was conducted to test the null hypothesis for subscale differences between each XR
condition.

4. Results
Result of the independent samples Mann-Whitney U testing rejected the null hypotheses
for temporal, effort, and performance only (table 1) as compared across each XR condition whereas
temporal, effort, and performance were statistically significant (p<.05). A comparison for each

34
Docens Series in Education
ISSN: 2583-1054

statistically significant subscale follows describing individual Mann-Whitney U testing on each


XR condition for each subscale.
Table 2: Independent Samples Mann-Whitney U Hypothesis Test Summary
S. No. Null Hypothesis Sig.a,b Decision
c
1 The distribution of Mental Demand .310 Retain the null hypothesis.
is the same across XR condition.
2 The distribution of Physical .119c Retain the null hypothesis.
Demand is the same across XR
condition.
3 The distribution of Temporal <.001c Reject the null hypothesis.
Demand is the same across XR
condition.
4 The distribution of Performance is .015c Reject the null hypothesis.
the same across XR condition.
5 The distribution of Effort is the <.001c Reject the null hypothesis.
same across XR condition.
6 The distribution of Frustration is .056c Retain the null hypothesis.
the same XR condition.
a. The significance level is .050.
b. Asymptotic significance is displayed.
c. Exact significance is displayed for this test.
(Source: Self from SPSS Output)

Drilling down into each subscale, we can see the comparison between the XR conditions by
subscale. As Table 2 shows, statistical significance of the Mann-Whitney U testing was seen in
the temporal, performance, and effort subscale. At an alpha level of .05, the p-value in the Mann-
Whitney U test displays both the 2-tailed Asymptotic significance and the exact significance level.
Table 3 indicates the summary for the Mann-Whitney U and Wilcoxon W results for each
condition on temporal demand. Comparing the N=24 across both conditions, the Mann-Whitney
U was 104.500 with an exact p-value of <.001 with a Wilcoxon W value of 125.50.

35
Docens Series in Education
ISSN: 2583-1054

Table 3: Independent-Samples Mann-Whitney U Test Summary on Temporal Demand

Total N 24
Mann-Whitney U 104.500
Wilcoxon W 125.500
Asymptotic Sig. (2-tailed test) <.001
Exact Sig. (2-tailed test) .000
(Source: Self from SPSS Output)
The mean ranks for each temporal demand condition is 9.69 (iVR) and 20.92 (MR). This suggests
a higher cognitive load for the MR group on temporal demand.
Table 4 shows the summary for the Mann-Whitney U and Wilcoxon W results on
performance demand. Comparing the N=24 across both conditions, the Mann-Whitney U was
90.00 with an exact p-value of .015 with a Wilcoxon W value of 111.00.
Table 4: Independent-Samples Mann-Whitney U Test Summary for Performance Demand

Total N 24
Mann-Whitney U 90.000
Wilcoxon W 111.000
Asymptotic Sig. (2-tailed test) .016
Exact Sig. (2-tailed test) .015
(Source: Self from SPSS Output)
The mean ranks for each performance demand condition is 10.50 (iVR) and 18.50 (MR). This also
suggest a higher cognitive load for the MR group on performance.
Table 5 shows the summary for the Mann-Whitney U and Wilcoxon W results on
performance demand. Comparing the N=24 across both conditions, the Mann-Whitney U was
102.50 with an exact p-value of <.001 with a Wilcoxon W value of 123.50.
Table 5: Independent-Samples Mann-Whitney U Test Summary

Total N 24
Mann-Whitney U 102.500
Wilcoxon W 123.500
Asymptotic Sig. (2-tailed test) .001
Exact Sig. (2-tailed test) .000
(Source: Self from SPSS Output)

36
Docens Series in Education
ISSN: 2583-1054

The mean ranks for each effort demand condition is 9.81 (iVR) and 20.58 (MR). Again, this result
suggests a higher cognitive load for the MR group on effort.

5. Discussion
In this study with undergraduate students, many of whom aspire to be teachers, results
are beginning to show the cognitive load impact when using integrated XR SEGs as a part of the
science teaching and learning process. The researchers recognize the sample size is small and not
evenly distributed but the findings are significant as a baseline for future studies. We also consider
that the weight given by the users were evenly distributed across the six subscales, which has a
potential impact on the NASA TLX calculations. When hypothesizing that there will be no
difference in cognitive load between Mixed Reality SEGs and Virtual Reality SEGs, this study
suggests that a self-reported measure, such as the NASA TLX, insinuates that Mixed Reality SEGs
create a larger cognitive demand than Virtual Reality SEGs with respect to temporal, performance,
and effort. This study also indicates there is no statistically significant differences in cognitive
load on mental demand, physical demand, or frustration level.
These findings were a bit surprising to the researchers. We anticipated that the physical
demand of walking a physical location while assimilating the nature surrounds and looking at a
MR SEG on one’s mobile device was surely going to show a higher cognitive workload on mental
and physical demand for the user as opposed to user of the iVR SEG where the user either sat or
stood in a location and simply looked around. The results of this study suggested otherwise. It was,
however, exciting to see the frustration demand was not statistically significant across the XR
conditions. In previous studies, we did see high frustration in users of an Augmented Reality-
based SEG but results of that study suggest the design of the SEGs in each XR condition was
insufficient and this producing a high frustration level.
Referring to Table 1 and the definitions the developers of the NASA TLX provided for
each cognitive demand subscale, we can begin to make inferences on possibly why there was
statistical significance for the MR SEG group. The Temporal demand definition was: How much
time pressure did you feel due to the rate or pace at which the tasks occurred? Was the pace slow
and leisurely; rapid and frantic? The design of each course may have played a role in the temporal
demand. Students on location in the Outer Banks that activated the MR SEGs were on a time
schedule. The first SEG, at Jockey’s Ridge State Park was the last stop of day 1 of the 5-day

37
Docens Series in Education
ISSN: 2583-1054

experience. The class had ample time to play the MR SEG being the end of a long day could have
caused some internal pressure to complete the game so they could move on to dinner and checking
into their hotel. Day 2 began with stops at the subsequent three locations and playing the MR SEG
at each. There could have been a sense of, “What’s next?”, and student felt pressure to complete
the game in a timely manner so they could board the bus and move to the next stop. In contrast,
the students using the iVR did not have to worry about the logistics of a field experience and had
the allocated class time to complete the tasks set forth in the iVR SEG.
With respect to Performance, the NASA TLX definition stated: How successful do you
think you were in accomplishing the goals of the task set by the experience? How satisfied were
you with your performance in accomplishing these goals? Again, results showed statistical
significance for the MR condition over the iVR condition. It can be inferred that students who
completed the SEG and collected all the gold coins in a timely manner felt a sense of
accomplishment and thus, performed well in the game/on the task. It can also be suspected that
students playing the MR SEG used a familiar device to activate the games. Most everyone now
has a smart phone and knows how to use it effectively and efficiently. Conversely, students playing
the iVR SEGs likely engaged with the Meta Quest 2 headset for the first time. Although they were
given instructions and guided through a tutorial before playing the first game, it was likely still a
foreign experience for most and thus, they likely did not feel a sense of high performance in this
task.
Finally, the NASA TLX defined Effort as: How hard did you have to work (mentally
and physically) to accomplish your level of performance? The result on this subscale speaks to our
original hypothesis that the MR condition group would have a higher physical and mental
workload. The NASA TLX instrument does have separate subscales for mental and physical
demand and does define each differently than under the Effort subscale, combining mental and
physical demand into one subscale likely encouraged students to report this combination as higher
for the MR condition group since they did walk and focus on their mobile device during game
play. The iVR group did not have such a physical demand during these games, which is why the
iVR condition group scored Effort lower than those in the MR condition group.

38
Docens Series in Education
ISSN: 2583-1054

6. Conclusion
Infusing technology into any educational setting has varying effects on both the
instructor and the learner. Technology can engage, allow students to explore environments in a
different way or locations they would never get to explore in person. Technology applications can
even serve as a formative assessment, but technology in and of itself is not a panacea. Technology
cannot, and should not, replace good teaching.
This study indicated the need for thoughtfully designed Serious Educational Games.
Juxtaposing instructional design with game design is a challenge unto itself, but considering games
in XR conditions adds another layer to the complexity. Understanding the cognitive demand each
SEG has on the learner is important considerations in both the instructional design and SEG design.
Clearly MR tasks the user to explore their physical surroundings while focusing their attention on
a smart device while iVR users do not have such a physical demand and are arguably more
immersed in the game world. This supports work done by Nur’amalia, Supriatna, & Ilfiandra
(2023) that higher task loads increase cognitive and mental fatigue. In doing so, learners lose
motivation to learn and increased boredom, stress, and anxiety.
Another important implication for practice is how we consider XR technology
embedded within place-based SSI versus remote SSI. First, based on the findings, we postulate
that familiarity with the technology is an important consideration when implementing SSI
instruction with XR technology. Participants in this study who used their smart phones felt more
successful and satisfied with their performance than did those who used a new piece of equipment
(i.e., Meta Quest). Teachers should provide opportunities for students to become familiar with the
technology in a low stake setting prior to engaging in content. Second, educators embedding MR
into place-based experiences must be judicious in the use of MR. The findings from this study
indicate that participants using the MR felt negatively impacted by time, which may have been the
result of designing too many MR experiences coupled with the perceived increased self-effort
needed to move from one MR experience to another. For example, in this study, reaching some of
the MR content artifacts required participants to walk some distance and up inclines. While this
was done to maximize the pedagogical impact of the location, it may have been a zero-sum game
in that any potential benefits of the location and/or technology were canceled out by the increased
effort required to reach the artifact. These findings are in potential conflict with those reported by
Wenk (2023) and Cheng and Huang (2023) where there were no cognitive load differences

39
Docens Series in Education
ISSN: 2583-1054

reported between AR or VR. There is much more research to be done into the synthesis of science
instruction, SSI, XR, and SEGs and this study should provide a baseline for future work.
The research limitations of this study are few but important. First, the cognitive demand
instrument used is dated and possibly did capture the true cognitive loads since they were self-
reported. Because cognitive load is difficult to measure, especially in low-cost technologies, a
self-report measure such as the NASA TLX used here is appropriate. However, self-reported data
is often exaggerated or embellished (i.e., the Hawthorne effect). Self-reported data should be
paired with other sources of data to ensure reliability.
This study will set the stage for a scope of future research. Most notably, the results of
this study suggested that users of these games must first be familiar with the technology before
engaging in the educational practices embedded in the games. Future studies will also include
bioinformatics as data sources. Such data points as eye and hand tracking, galvanic skin response,
heart rate, and fNIR brain blood flow information will support self-reported cognitive load
information. We would also see a future direction working with learners with special needs
(Mekacher, 2019).

REFERENCES
Abt, C. (1970). Serious Games. New York: The Viking Press.
Allcoat, D., Hatchard, T., Azmat, F., Stansfield, K., Watson, D., & Von Mü Hlenen, A. (2021).
Education in the Digital Age: Learning Experience in Virtual and Mixed Realities.
Journal of Educational Computing Research, 59(5), 795–816.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0735633120985120
Annetta, L. A., (2008). Serious Educational Games: From Theory to Practice. Amsterdam, The
Netherlands: Sense Publishers. pp. 83. https://doi.org/10.1163/9789087903817
Chen, C. C., & Huang, P. H. (2023). The effects of STEAM-based mobile learning on learning
achievement and cognitive load. Interactive Learning Environments, 31(1).
https://doi.org/10.1080/10494820.2020.1761838
Chen, Y. C., Chang, Y. S., & Chuang, M. J. (2022). Virtual reality application influences
cognitive load-mediated creativity components and creative performance in
engineering design. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 38(1).
https://doi.org/10.1111/jcal.12588

40
Docens Series in Education
ISSN: 2583-1054

Climate Change Education. In Shepardson, D.P., Roychoudhury, A., & Hirsch, A.S. (Eds.).
(2017). Teaching and Learning about Climate Change: A Framework for Educators
(1st ed.). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315629841
Dolphin, G., Dutchak, A., Karchewski, B., & Cooper, J. (2019). Virtual field experiences in
introductory geology: Addressing a capacity problem, but finding a pedagogical one.
Journal of Geoscience Education, 67(2), 114–130.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10899995.2018.1547034
Hart, S. G., & Staveland, L. E. (1988). Development of NASA-TLX (Task Load Index): Results
of empirical and theoretical research. In P. A. Hancock & N. Meshkati (Eds.), Human
mental workload (pp. 139–183). North-Holland. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-
4115(08)62386-9
Huang, K. T., Ball, C., Francis, J., Ratan, R., Boumis, J., & Fordham, J. (2019). Augmented
versus virtual reality in education: An exploratory study examining science knowledge
retention when using augmented reality/virtual reality mobile applications.
Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 22(2).
https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2018.0150
Huang, X., Huss, J., North, L., Williams, K., & Boyd-Devine, A. (2023). Cognitive and
motivational benefits of a theory-based immersive virtual reality design in science
learning. Computers and Education Open, 4, 100124.
https://doi.org/10.1016/J.CAEO.2023.100124
Ibili, E., & Billinghurst, M. (2019). Assessing the Relationship between Cognitive Load and the
Usability of a Mobile Augmented Reality Tutorial System: A Study of Gender Effects.
International Journal of Assessment Tools in Education, 6(3).
https://doi.org/10.21449/ijate.594749
Juliano, J. M., Schweighofer, N., & Liew, S. L. (2022). Increased cognitive load in immersive
virtual reality during visuomotor adaptation is associated with decreased long-term
retention and context transfer. Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation, 19(1).
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12984-022-01084-6
Mekacher, L. (2019). Augmented Reality (AR) and Virtual Reality (VR): The Future of
Interactive Vocational Education And Training For People With Handicap. PUPIL:

41
Docens Series in Education
ISSN: 2583-1054

International Journal of Teaching, Education and Learning, 3(1), 118–129.


https://doi.org/10.20319/pijtel.2019.31.118129
Mermer, T. (2010). The UNESCO climate change initiative. UNESCO.
Newton, M. H., & Zeidler, D. L. (2020). Developing socioscientific perspective taking.
International Journal of Science Education, 42(8), 1302–1319.
https://doi.org/10.1080/09500693.2020.1756515
Newton, M.H., Annetta, L.A., & Bressler, D. (2023). Using extended reality technology in
traditional and place-based environments to study climate change. Journal of Science
Education and Technology. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10956-023-10057-w
Nur’amalia, Y., Supriatna, M., & Ilfiandra, (2023). ONLINE LEARNING DIFFICULTIES AS
IMPACT OF COVID-19 IN INDONESIA: Received: 09th August 2021; Revised:
29th November 2022, 18th January 2023; Accepted: 20th January 2023. PUPIL:
International Journal of Teaching, Education and Learning, 6(3), 48–56.
https://doi.org/10.20319/pijtel.2023.63.4856
Petersen, G. B., Petkakis, G., & Makransky, G. (2022). A study of how immersion and
interactivity drive VR learning. Computers and Education, 179.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2021.104429
Sadler, T. D. (2009). Situated learning in science education: Socio-scientific issues as contexts
for practice. In Studies in Science Education (Vol. 45).
https://doi.org/10.1080/03057260802681839
Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on learning. Cognitive
Science, 12(2). https://doi.org/10.1016/0364-0213(88)90023-7
Thees, M., Kapp, S., Strzys, M. P., Beil, F., Lukowicz, P., & Kuhn, J. (2020). Effects of
augmented reality on learning and cognitive load in university physics laboratory
courses. Computers in Human Behavior, 108.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2020.106316
Wenk, N., Penalver-Andres, J., Buetler, K. A., Nef, T., Müri, R. M., & Marchal-Crespo, L.
(2023). Effect of immersive visualization technologies on cognitive load, motivation,
usability, and embodiment. Virtual Reality, 27(1). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10055-021-
00565-8
Zeidler, D. L., & Newton, M. H. (2017). Using a socioscientific issues framework for climate

42
Docens Series in Education
ISSN: 2583-1054

change education: An ecojustice approach. In Teaching and learning about climate


change (pp. 56-65). Routledge.
https://www.taylorfrancis.com/chapters/edit/10.4324/9781315629841-5/using-
socioscientific-issues-framework-climate-change-education-dana-zeidler-mark-newton
Zhao, J., LaFemina, P., Carr, J., Sajjadi, P., Wallgrun, J. O., & Klippel, A. (2020). Learning in
the Field: Comparison of Desktop, Immersive Virtual Reality, and Actual Field Trips
for Place-Based STEM Education. 893–902.
https://doi.org/10.1109/vr46266.2020.00012

43

You might also like