Patient-Practitioner Relationship
Patient-Practitioner Relationship
Patient-Practitioner Relationship
PRACTITIONER
RELATIONSHIP
Asim Masood
[email protected]
+923002626209
Practitioner and patient
interpersonal skills
NON-VERBAL COMMUNICATION (McKinstry and Wang, 1991)
Aim: to investigate whether patients think the way their doctor dresses is
important, what their preference ism and whether patients think the way
the doctor dresses influences their effectiveness as a doctor
The doctor in the cardigan and casual trousers scored higher than the doctor in jeans
The informally dressed female doctor scored lower than the other two female doctors
Older patients gave high scores to the male doctor in a white coat or formal suit
Older patients were more likely to give higher scores to the female doctor in a white
coat
41% of the sample answered “yes” to the question “Do you think you
would have more confidence in the ability of one of these doctors
based on their appearance?”
For the above, the main choices were suit and white coat for the male
doctor, followed by the tweed jacket, jeans and cardigan
For the female doctor, the main choice was white coat, followed by skirt
and then trousers
For the closed ended questions about general doctor dress code,
majority participants believed jeans would be too informal for a male or
female doctor
They also believed a male doctor should wear a tie and a female doctor
should wear a skirt
Many participants also stated their own doctor matched the smartly
dressed one in the photograph
Data was also analysed in terms of social class and gender with higher
social class objecting more to male doctors wearing an earring and to
female doctors wearing lots of jewellery
More male patients than female patients believed that women doctors
should wear white coats
In general it can be seen that patients prefer formally dressed doctors,
but this is affected by age, social class and the particular medical
practice
Patients also preferred doctors dressed in a manner similar to their own
doctor
Demand characteristics were reduced by analysing the data and
understanding that even though initially patients stated that the dressing
of their doctor does not matter or was not important, they still gave
discriminatory scores in assessing the photographs
To conclude, the appearance of a doctor and the way they dress can
affect the way a patient thinks about them in terms of their confidence
in their ability and whether they would be comfortable going to such a
doctor
Controls: The expression of the male doctor was the same for all 5 photographs, as well
as for the female doctor for the 3 photographs
The background and posture was standardized as well with the only difference being
the clothing
All participants answered the same close ended questions on the same rating scale
VERBAL COMMUNICATIONS (McKinlay, 1975)
Aim: to investigate the perceived and actual comprehension of medical
terms by a sample of lower working-class families in Scotland using
maternity services
The fourth interview is the one that collected data about the patients’
comprehension of medical terminology used by doctors that could be
misunderstood by them
57 words were initially selected that were classed as ones which either
patients would use; which patients would not understand; or those that
fell in the middle
For the study, 13 of these 57 words were selected as they were the ones
that fell in the middle category (those that may or may not be
understood by patients)
These words would be read out individually to the patient in the form of a
sentence and they would be asked what the word meant
The patient was reassured this was not a test, and was being conducted
to understand if doctors use words that are commonly not understood
by patients
Examples of words on which the patients were tested were –
ANTIOBIOTIC: If a doctor told a patient that she’s going to put her on
antibiotics, what’s she going to do?
BREECH: If a doctor told a patient that she’s going to have a breech,
what do you think is going to happen?
Score C: the patient recognized the word but did not understand it
Score D: The patient had a good idea what the word meant
Patients were split into two groups – Utilizers (those who avail the
maternity services regularly) and Underutilizers (those who do not avail
services regularly)
A year later, a separate group of doctors rated each of the words on
what they expected the patients to understand
Results: For nearly all 13 words, the utilizer group had a higher Score D
(understanding of the word) than the under-utilizer group, suggesting
that those patients who avail maternity services regularly are more likely
to comprehend medical terminology used by doctors
Furthermore, the study also showed that the doctors underestimated the
level of medical terminology comprehension in their patients
Controls: The same questions were asked to all the patients, making it a
structured interview
The same 13 words were used making it a standardized procedure
The scores were rated on the same scale
Two independent doctors scored each participant for inter rater
reliability
Only those words were selected which had a possibility of both being
understood and not understood (rather than selecting those words
which definitely would not be understood, or those which definitely
would be understood)
Other factors that can affect verbal communication in interactions as
noted by Ley (1988) include:
Volume of information provided by the doctor, as it is believed that just over
50% of the information provided by doctors to patients is remembered
Primacy effect plays a part as patients tend to remember more of the
information provided to them at the start of a consultation
Patients with some medical knowledge tend to understand better, and
therefore remember more information
Instructions provided verbally tend to be forgotten and therefore, written
instructions should also be provided
PATIENT AND PRACTITIONER
DIAGNOSIS AND STYLE
PRACTITIONER DIAGNOSIS
There are three main aspects to a practitioner diagnosis which include:
making a diagnosis, presenting a diagnosis, and coping with diagnosis
The results showed that they were more likely to disclose information via
a computerized interview as opposed to a paper questionnaire, and
both these methods gathered more information than the doctor was
able to when speaking to them directly during the consultation
Talking further about diagnosis, as mentioned earlier, errors and mistakes can
be made while diagnosing patients
There are two main types of misdiagnoses that can be made which are false
positive (aka Type 1 error) or false negative (aka Type 2 error)
They were explained the pros and cons of each option mentioned in a
leaflet provided to them
One month after receiving the biopsy result, a questionnaire was sent
This questionnaire asked various closed questions about how they
received their diagnosis, whether they had been given the information
leaflet, and if they were satisfied with the method at the time and in
hindsight
From the 77 patients, 48% chose to receive the diagnosis by letter, 37%
opted for telephone, 11% face to face, and 5% chose a combination of
two methods
89% received their diagnosis via their chosen method, with 11% being
sent a letter because they could not be contacted by phone
94% patients were happy with the way they received their diagnosis ,
and when asked if they would change in hindsight, 80% said no
Despite these findings, it is reported that most skin care centers do not
believe it is necessary to offer alternative methods to the traditional face
to face approach
Results showed that 32% of the patients received diagnosis via telephone
as opposed to face to face, and there was no significant difference in
satisfaction, anxiety or depression levels between patients who received
diagnosis by phone or face to face
However, some factors which could influence this include points such as
patients who were prepared for the diagnosis, had been presented
information clearly, and told everything about what to expect with
respect to the disease, experienced higher satisfaction, and lower
depression and anxiety as well
KEY STUDY: DIRECTING AND SHARING STYLES (Savage and Armstrong, 1990)
Procedure: The general practitioner did not know which style to adopt
for each patient until they had entered the consultation room
The practitioner would then turn over a card allocating one of the two
styles (directing or sharing)
An independent observer listened to a selection of 40 recordings of the
consultation and confirmed that all aspects of each consultation
conformed to the style allocated on the card
Results: Overall satisfaction levels were higher for the directed group who
reported more satisfaction with explanation provided by the doctor
compared to the sharing group
The directed group also reported greater satisfaction with regards to the
perception that the GP had a complete understanding
For the aspects of ‘I was able to discuss my problem well’, ‘I felt greatly helped’,
and ‘I felt much better’, there was no real significance between the directed
group and the sharing group
The comparison showed that 45% of patients in the directing group, but only
24% in the sharing group felt they received an excellent explanation, and one
week later after they took the assessment again, these figures dropped to 33%
and 17% respectively
A total of 62% patients in the directing style, but only 37% in the sharing style felt
the GP had a complete understanding of their health issue, and one week
later, these figures dropped to 39% an 18% respectively
It can therefore be concluded that a more directing style of consultation brings
about more satisfaction in patients
MISUSING HEALTH SERVICES
DELAY IN SEEKING TREATMENT
For many patients, there is a delay between noticing a
symptom and seeking treatment, and this can have serious
implications
Safer found there were three stages that make up the delay in seeking
treatment which include:
1. Appraisal delay: the time the patient takes to appraise or judge a symptom, an is
commonly the duration from when they first notice a new symptom, up to the time
they realize they are unwell
2. Illness delay: this is the duration of time from when the patient realizes they are
unwell, up to the time they decide to seek medical care
3. Utilisation delay: this is the duration of time from when the patient has decided to
seek medical care, up to the time that they actually access the services
Safer investigated the effect of the following predicting factors on each
of the stages of delay:
Sensory or perceptual experience of the symptom, e.g. pain or bleeding
Self-appraisal processes, e.g. observing the symptom for signs of change
Coping response to symptoms, e.g. home remedies
Emotional reactions to the health threat, e.g. fear and distress
The imagined consequences of the symptoms, e.g. imagining that surgery is
required
The situational barriers to receiving care, e.g. cost of services
The first three factors were considered to be the most probable
predictors of appraisal delay
The patients of the study were attending one of four clinics and had
come to present a new illness they developed, and were approached
by the researchers to take part in a 45 minute questionnaire and
interview
93 patients completed the interview, all who were presenting a new
symptom for an illness, and included people of different ages, ethnicities,
and both males and females (slightly more females)
The average length of the illness delay was 3.1 days, and if it was a new
symptom (sensory factor), this reduced the delay, as compared to an old or
recurring symptom. Furthermore, if the patient imagined a negative
consequence such as having to receive surgery (imagery factor), this increased
the delay
The average length of utilisation delay was 2.5 days, and those who were
worried about the cost had a longer delay (situational factor), but those who
felt that their symptom could be cured, or experienced pain (sensory factor)
had a shorter delay
Alternative explanations for delay:
Health Belief Model:
The health belief model helps to explain health-related behaviours, including delay in
seeking treatment and includes the following factors:
Perceived severity: the likelihood of a person changing their health behaviours depends
on how serious they think the consequences are
Perceived susceptibility: a person is more likely to change their health behaviour if they
feel they are particularly at risk
Perceived benefits: people are less likely to change a behaviour if they don’t think the
change will benefit them
Perceived barriers: barriers such as expense, effort, discomfort and inconvenience can
lead to delays
Cues to action: an external event such as an ad advising people to get symptoms
checked, can act as a trigger to make someone take action
Self-efficacy: Someone who believes they are capable doing something is more likely to
do it, such as believing one can go through the diagnosis and treatment process is more
likely to not delay seeking treatment
MUNCHAUSEN SYNDROME vs MALINGERING
Munchausen syndrome is a mental disorder where someone pretends
to be unwell deliberately, and this can be done so by hurting
themselves
Ajeem and Ajarim report a case of a 22 year old female who attended a
hospital in Saudi Arabia with a painful swelling over her right breast
She had a history of similar recurrent swellings and needed surgical
drainage
From the age of 17 she had been seen by many hospitals regularly for
issues relating to her menstrual cycle during which she developed
symptoms that suggested deep vein thrombosis (blood clot) which did
not respond to the medication
The patient came from a supportive family and did not have any
financial problems
Her mother had breast cancer
Soon after the swelling, she presented complaints of swellings in the groin
area and weakness in lower limbs
She had physical examinations such as a CT scan, and the doctors
suspected haemotoma (swelling of clotted blood within the tissue)
She was an intelligent individual but lacked much medical knowledge
She was treated with antibiotics, but the surgical drainage had to be
completed to remove an abscess she had developed
After four days in the hospital, she developed a similar abscess on the
opposite breast
It was found that the abscess contained several different bacteria
At this point the doctors started suspecting a case of factitious disorder
and a psychiatric consultation was made after counselling her without
giving her a hint about suspicion of a factitious disorder
One day when she was not in the bed in the hospital, nurses found a
syringe full of faecal material along with needles
When she returned, another patient had told her about what the nurses
had found
She became very angry and hostile and left the hospital and did not
return
At this point she was officially diagnosed with Munchausen Syndrome