Assessing Children's Gross Motor Development (Implication For School and Wellbeing)
Assessing Children's Gross Motor Development (Implication For School and Wellbeing)
Assessing Children's Gross Motor Development (Implication For School and Wellbeing)
2021
online ISSN: 2247 - 806X; p-ISSN: 2247 – 8051; ISSN - L = 2247 - 8051 © JPES
Original Article
Abstract
Psychological assessments, particularly with young children, rely on developmental estimations reported by different
caregivers (i.e. parents and teachers). The degree of accordance between different informants is very important to
detect early impairments or delays in different developmental domains.To date, recentresearch studies have drawn
attention to the association between gross motor proficiency in infancyand later academic successorchildren's personal
wellbeingthrough the long-term effects of gross motor domain on social acceptance, general participation in play, and
willingness to take part in social activities in general.This investigationexamined parents' and teachers'agreement in
rating107 pupils' gross motor development. Caregivers' estimationswere compared with children's actual motor
skillsperformedat standardized clinical assessment testing for their accuracy. The convenient sample was composed
from 47 kindergarten children, 60 first graders (age range 4-6 yearolds). Parents and teachers were interviewed with
the Vineland Adaptive Behaviour Scales(VABS-II) and children completed the gross motor proficiency tasks of the
Movement Assessment Battery for Children-2 (MABC-2). Findings revealed significant differences in caregivers
ratings of children's gross motor development with respect to the Walking and Running, Play Activity and Total Score
of the VABS-II. Teachers' ratings were higher when assessing the ability to run and throwing balls but were lower
when reporting children's performances in handling bikes and balance skills. Estimations differed in the Aiming and
Catching and Static-Dynamic Balance subscales of the MABC-2. Overall caregiver estimations correlated with
children's actual motor proficiency. Parent judgements have been found to be more accurate than those of teachers.
Results are discussed in light of practical considerations for the school setting, teacher disciplinary preparation in
estimating children's motor skills, and situation specificity influence on caregivers' assessment.
Key Words: gross motor development ratings, parent teacher agreement, school, wellbeing
Introduction
In recent years research on children's motor skills has been linked to cognitive development and early/long-
term school achievements(Cameron et al., 2016).Some authors have argued thatcognitive and motor
development are associated so that neural processes underlying motor learning are the same that regulate
cognitive learning (Marsh et al., 2008).However, how much and how each skill or process can contribute to
school outcomesremain largelyundefined, but an increasing number of longitudinal studies attest this association
shedding new light on this interesting topic.
In this regard, fine motor skills have been found to be one of the strongestearly predictors of later school
outcomes positively related with name writing, written expression, literacy and math skills being involved in
reproducing visual representation or transcription of ideas(Grissmer et al., 2010); (Taverna et al., 2020). On the
other hand, gross motor skills also play a key role in children'sdevelopment of social competencies and physical
wellbeing (Sattelmair&Ratey, 2009), engaging pupils in learning and participating to social activities, sport and
games within and outside educational context(Skinner & Piek, 2001). Particularly important are movement
activities in the first years of schooling when children are more active(Eaton et al., 2001). At this time children
play in groups, and movement skills contribute to their social status(Chase & Dummer, 1992). A lack in motor
competenceperformance could influence a decrease of participation in social physical activities as shown
bychildren with motor difficulties whoare less likely to play games(Smyth & Anderson, 2001). Moreover, gross
motor skills significantly contribute to general health and physical wellbeing, reducing body mass index(Okely
et al., 2004), improving cardiorespiratory fitness(Okely et al., 2001)and physical activity(Jones et al., 2020).
560 ---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
Corresponding Author: LIVIA TAVERNA, E-mail: [email protected]
LIVIA TAVERNA, MARTA TREMOLADA, SABRINA BONICHINI, FRANCESCA SANGIULIANO INTRA,
ANTONELLA BRIGHI
---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
Given the impact that motor competence has on further developmental functions, an early detection of
delays, clumsiness or impairments in fundamental movement skills would result in an increase of likeliness of
prompt and effective interventions.Psychological assessments, particularly with young children, rely on
developmental estimations reported by different caregivers (i.e. parents, kindergarten educators and
schoolteachers).Indeed, parent and teachers proxy-reports provide valuable insights of children's actual motor
competence in their own environment(Kramer et al., 2009). Liong and colleagues(Liong et al., 2015), for
example, report that parents ratings were accurate and predicted children's object control ability and locomotion
skills, although sex differences in parents perception were detected and affected the appreciation of their
children's movement proficiency. Physical education teachers are considered reliable informants of children's
motor competence, reporting accurately pupils' motor control and being able to detect school children with
coordination and motor control difficulties in all age groups between 4 and 12 years (Ruiz-Pérez, 2001).Sports
teachers showed an accurate understanding of their students’ physical ability potential and activity behavior and
are more reliable when asked to report on general perceptions of the children's physical ability than on specific
motor domains (Faught et al., 2008).However, Lalor and colleagues reported that classroom teachers predicted
children's body coordination but not manual coordination, agility or strength(Lalor et al., 2016).
Moreover, numerous studies have shown that estimates of children's skills depend not only from caregivers
knowledge and competences, but also from seeing subjects within the same environment and under the same
conditions(Achenbach et al., 1987). Judgementsof informants playing similar social roles with respect to
children (e.g. mother vs father)correlate to a higher degree than estimation of ratersbelonging to different
contexts and having different roles(e.g. father vs teacher).Cross-situational assessment is further complicated by
the developmental area assessed to the extent that multi-informant agreementvary with respect to skill domain
(Deng et al., 2004). Parent and teacher reportson children's behavioral problems showed poor inter-rater
reliability(Mitsis et al., 2000); (Antrop et al., 2002), whereas observers agreedwhen estimating children'
cognitive skills(Schrader, 2001).
To date, limited attention has been paid to studying the extent of agreement or possible discrepancies
between parents and teachers on children's gross motor skills leaving this domain largely unexplored. Given the
fact that schoolteachers are continually exposed to their students behaviors involving physical activity, the
current study was aimed at investigatingparents' and teachers' agreement on children's gross motor domain. A
further objective of this study was to verify informants' estimations accuracy by comparing their developmental
ratings with children's actual motor performance measured by standardized tests.
---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------- 561
JPES ® www.efsupit.ro
LIVIA TAVERNA, MARTA TREMOLADA, SABRINA BONICHINI, FRANCESCA SANGIULIANO INTRA,
ANTONELLA BRIGHI
---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
conversation the respondent describes key developmental milestones or adaptive behaviors that represent the
child’s usual functioning. The examiner rates each item of the VABS-II with scores ranging from 0 to 2
depending whether the activity is never or habitually performed. Standard scores are provided for each VABS-II
domain and subdomain.
Data collection and analysis
Ethics approval for the current study was obtained from the Research and Ethic Committee of the Free
University of Bozen-Bolzano (BW2046).Consent form and demographic information were distributed prior to
study participation. Children completed the MABC-2 tasks during class hours and were tested individually in a
quiet room. VABS-II interviews were arranged at time convenient for informants and took place at school.
Statistical analysis
Tests of normality and homogeneity were conducted before test selection. There were no outliers in the
data, as assessed by inspection of a boxplot for values greater than 1.5 box-lengths from the edge of the
box.Paired-samples t-test were used to investigate differences between teacher and parent responsesat the
VABS-II interviews on gross motor development at subdomain, cluster and item level. Pearson's correlation
coefficient was calculated for inter-rater reliability, when comparing agreement between parent and
teachers'ratings on the VABS-II, and to determine children's actual performances at the MABC-2.
Results
According to the inclusion criteria for the study, Table 1 presents the background characteristics of the
participants. Significant differences were found onlyfor the mothers' ages. Mothers of first-grade
childrenreported to be slightly older (t(97)=-2.349; p=.021)when compared to kindergarten mothers.
Table1 Demographic data of families included in the study (n = 107). Information are reported in absolute
values, percentages, means and standard deviations.
Kindergarten (n=57) First Graders (n=50)
n (%) n (%)
Gender
Boys 32 (56.1) 29 (58.0)
Girls 25 (43.9) 21 (42.0)
Age
4 2 (3.5)
5 45 (78.9)
6 10 (17.5) 50 (100.0)
Mother's Educational Level
junior secondary school (0-8yr.) 13 (22.7) 5 (10.0)
high school (9-13yr.) 32 (56.3) 25 (50.0)
university (14-18yr.) 5 (8.8) 13 (26.0)
post-university (beyond 19yr.) 3 (5.2) 2 (4.4)
not reported 4 (7.0) 5 (19.6)
Father's Educational Level
junior secondary school (0-8yr.) 9 (15.8) 8 (16.0)
high school (9-13yr.) 34 (56.2) 28 (56.0)
university (14-18yr.) 9 (15.8) 8 (16.0)
post-university (beyond 19yr.) 1 (1.8) 1 (1.8)
not reported 6 (10.4) 5 (10.2)
Employment Mother
housewife 11 (19.3) 7 (14.0)
process worker 10 (17.5) 9 (18.0)
office worker 30 (52.6) 26 (52.0)
professional 2 (3.5) 3 (6.0)
not reported 4 (7.0) 5 (10.0)
Employment Father
unemployed 1 (1.8)
process worker 23 (40.4) 18 (36.0)
office worker 21 (36.8) 15 (30.0)
professional 5 (8.8) 13 (26.0)
not reported 7 (12.3) 4 (8.0)
Number of siblings
1 sibling 7 (12.3) 12 (24.0)
2 siblings 34 (59.6) 24 (48.0)
3 siblings 10 (17.6) 10 (20.0)
not reported 6 (10.5) 4 (8.0)
Mother's age (Mean; SD)* 36.68 (6.12) 39.48 (5.66)
Father's age (Mean; SD) 40.35 (7.29) 41.91 (9.55)
562 ----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
JPES ® www.efsupit.ro
LIVIA TAVERNA, MARTA TREMOLADA, SABRINA BONICHINI, FRANCESCA SANGIULIANO INTRA,
ANTONELLA BRIGHI
---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
Comparing parent and teacher ratings of children'smotor proficiency assessed with VABS-II,significant
differences were detected by means of paired-samples t-tests in the gross motor total score(t(99)=-3.786; p=.0001)
and two clusters scores, Walking and running(t(99)=-3.159; p=.002) and Play activity (t(99)=-2.425; p=.017),
respectively. The motor development estimates expressed by teachers are overall higher than those reported by
parentsas seen in Figure 1, while no significant differences emerged exploring the following clusters: Sitting,
Standing, Creeping and Crawling.
Figure 1Significant mean differences betweenparent and teacher ratings in VABS-II gross motor domain.
A series of paired-samplest-tests were run to analyze which children's motor skills highlighteda lack of
agreement between parents and teachers. Findings show that teacher's ratings are significantly higher when
assessing the ability to run (item 20: t(99)=-3.129; p=.002; item 23: t(98)=-2.242; p=.027) and throwingballs(item
26: t(99)=-2.283; p=.025), but report significantlylower performancesin children'shandling bikes (item 35:
t(89)=4.818; p=.0001) and balance skills(item 40: t(90)=3.885; p=.0001) as reported in Table 2.
Table2Mean differences and SD ofparent and teacher ratingsin gross motor subdomain of the VABS-II.
Since the areas in which parent and teacher ratings highlighted poor agreement relate to skills assessed in
the Aiming and Catching and Static-Dynamic Balance subscales of the MABC-2, a series of correlations were
run to analyze the reliability of informants' estimateson children's actual locomotor competence and their
reciprocal agreement. As reported in Table 3, parent and teacher reports on pupils' overall gross motor skills
reveal a positive correlation (r(100)=.255; p=.011), with a moderate association between informants on the play
activity cluster of the VABS-II (r(100)=.282; p=.004). Interestingly,the only association between parental
---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------- 563
JPES ® www.efsupit.ro
LIVIA TAVERNA, MARTA TREMOLADA, SABRINA BONICHINI, FRANCESCA SANGIULIANO INTRA,
ANTONELLA BRIGHI
---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
assessments and children's performance can be observed for the dynamic balance test of walking with heels
raised (MABC-2 B2: r(98)=-.222; p=.004). The association is negative and of moderate-intensity, while as regards
the teachers' judgments, no associations whatsoever can be detected.
Table 3 Pearson's product moment correlations between parent and teacher VABS-II scores in gross motor
subdomain and children's performances at the MABC-2 in the Ball Skills and Balance tasks.
VABS-II MABC-2
GMTS-P GMTS-T W&R-P W&R-T PA-P PA-T BS 1 BS 2 B1 B2 B3
VABS-II
GMTS-P 1 .255* .988*** .171 .992*** .251* .004 .124 .096 -.222* -.121
GMTS-T 1 .221* .667*** .287** .984*** .117 .138 -.011 .109 .033
W&R-P 1 .147 .966*** .218* -.004 .078 .096 -.248* -.123
W&R-T 1 .194 .523*** .061 .004 .038 .078 .001
PA-P 1 .282** .019 .163 .115 -.213* -.095
PA-T 1 .118 .154 -.021 .105 .037
MABC-2
BS 1 1 .291** .171 -.065 .173
BS 2 1 .274** -.033 .120
B1 1 -.085 .098
B2 1 -.110
B3 1
Note: VABS-II GMTS-P: Gross Motor Total Score Parent; GMTS-T:Gross Motor Total Score Teacher; W&R-
P: Walking and Running Parent; VABS-II W&R-T: Walking and Running Teacher;PA-P: Play Activity Parent;
PA-T: Play Activity Teacher; MABC-2 BS1= Ball Skills 1; BS2= Ball Skills 2; B1= Balance 1; B2= Balance 2;
B3= Balance 3.
* p <.05; ** p <.01; *** p <.001
Discussion
Although gross motor skills are observable both from teachers and from parents our results show that
caregivers' assessments differ with respect to some specific areas of observationof the VABS-II. Item content
analysis reveals that the disagreement between informants is about coordination in running, throwing balls in any
direction and riding a bicycle.Except for the cycling skill, teachersattribute to children a higher motor
competence than their parents. Moreover, teachers' reports are in no way associated with gross motor skills tested
in children, while parents' judgementsagree with their pupils' dynamic balance competence. Taken together,
thesefindings raise some questions about interobserver reliability, the use of different informants' assessments of
child's development, and the quality of opportunities / tools provided for structured observations.
First, considering poor agreement between informants as equal with unrealiabilitymeans to ignore that
the situational specificity issue could also concern children's motor skills. A rich literature has illustrated how
children's behavioral problems emerge in a different way according to the context of observation, giving
substance to the international debate on the situational specificity topic.As already observed from Achenbach
(Achenbach et al., 1987) this neglects the possibility that different informants validly contribute information
which is at odds. Teachers spend a lot of time with children and are a valuable source of observation about the
skills they already master or can develop in the school setting.Interobserver poor agreement or discrepancies
could reflect differences in opportunities to detect the target variables depending from the specific situation
rather than from the informants' reliability. Indeed, significant variations at item levelwith respect to cycling, and
in an opposite direction compared to other VABS-II cluster,may raise the doubt that teachers were lacking in
opportunity or tools to estimate children's cycling skills. Finally, this study has involved general classroom
teachers and not physical education teachers, while other studies have shown that disciplinary preparation helps
in identifying motor skills(Estevan et al., 2018). Since the use of trained and disciplinary-prepared observers to
distinguish between informant and situational variance is not always possible, a valid alternative could be
investing in tools and school materials with the intent towidenobservationin structured and unstructured play
activities. Moreover,as already shown from Tortella and colleagues (Tortella et al., 2019); (Tortella et al.,
2016)specifically designed playground that are available serve as an opportunity to promote gross motor skills in
preschool children as well as school-based intervention programs including task-specific activities could improve
children's motor proficiency (Mathisen, 2016).
Conclusions
This study has investigated parent and teacher agreements in rating 107 children's gross skills.
Caregivers' estimations were correlated withpupil's actual motor skills performed at MABC-2 testing for their
accuracy. The main findings were: (1)teachers express an overall motor development higher than that reported
by parents; (2) main discrepancies emerged on running and throwing balls with teachers attributing higher motor
competence thanparents, and conversely on cycling and balance skillswith parents reporting higher skills on
564 ----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
JPES ® www.efsupit.ro
LIVIA TAVERNA, MARTA TREMOLADA, SABRINA BONICHINI, FRANCESCA SANGIULIANO INTRA,
ANTONELLA BRIGHI
---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
these domains; (3) associations between caregivers' assessments and children's performances were found only for
parents on balance skills.
Several useful implications follow from our analyses and from the discussion of our findings. First, any
estimation of children's gross motor competence must take account of variance in the situations on which
assessment takes place, requiring further research studies on the situational specificity topic related to the
assessment oflocomotor skills domain. In this regard, discrepanciesbetween parents and teachersestimates would
suggest differences in the opportunities for observing children skills and their actual motor performance. Second,
poor or lacking correlations between informants' ratings - regardless of their type (parents or teachers) - and
children's motor proficiency raise the question of the importance ofdisciplinary preparation in estimation of
children's motor skills, suggesting the involvement of physical education teachers in motor assessment. Third,
trained observers enhance informationreliability, but this does not overcomethe problem of limited observation
opportunities which can only be resolved by equipping schools with specifically designed materials and
playgrounds. Fourth, disciplinary-prepared teachers can significantly contribute to the increase of children's
motor skills and wellbeing.
References:
Achenbach, T. M., McConaughy, S. H., & Howell, C. T. (1987). Child/adolescent behavioral and emotional
problems: Implications of cross-informant correlations for situational specificity. Psychological Bulletin,
101(2), 213–232.
Antrop, I., Roeyers, H., Oosterlaan, J., & Van Oost, P. (2002). Agreement Between Parent and Teacher Ratings
of Disruptive Behavior Disorders in Children with Clinically Diagnosed ADHD. Journal of
Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 24(1), 67–73.
Balboni, G., Belacchi, C., Bonichini, S., & Coscarelli, S. (2016). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Second
Edition (Vineland-II) – Survey Form. Adattamentoitaliano.Giunti OS.
Cameron, C. E., Cottone, E. A., Murrah, W. M., &Grissmer, D. W. (2016). How Are Motor Skills Linked to
Children’s School Performance and Academic Achievement? Child Development Perspectives, 10(2), 93–
98.
Chase, M. A., &Dummer, G. M. (1992). The role of sports as a social status determinant for children. Research
Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 63(4), 418–424.
Deng, S., Liu, X., &Roosa, M. W. (2004). Agreement Between Parent and Teacher Reports on Behavioral
Problems Among Chinese Children. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 25(6), 407–414.
Eaton, W. O., McKeen, N. A., & Campbell, D. W. (2001). The Waxing and Waning of Movement: Implications
for Psychological Development. Developmental Review, 21(2), 205–223.
Estevan, I., Molina-García, J., Bowe, S. J., Álvarez, O., Castillo, I., & Barnett, L. M. (2018). Who can best report
on children’s motor competence: Parents, teachers, or the children themselves? Psychology of Sport and
Exercise, 34, 1–9.
Faught, B. E., Cairney, J., Hay, J., Veldhuizen, S., Missiuna, C., &Spironello, C. A. (2008). Screening for motor
coordination challenges in children using teacher ratings of physical ability and activity. Human
Movement Science, 27(2), 177–189.
Grissmer, D., Grimm, K. J., Aiyer, S. M., Murrah, W. M., & Steele, J. S. (2010). Fine motor skills and early
comprehension of the world: Two new school readiness indicators. Developmental Psychology, 46(5),
1008–1017.
Jones, D., Innerd, A., Giles, E. L., & Azevedo, L. B. (2020). Association between fundamental motor skills and
physical activity in the early years: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Sport and Health
Science, 9(6), 542–552.
Kramer, J., Bowyer, P., O’Brien, J., Kielhofner, G., &Maziero-Barbosa, V. (2009). How interdisciplinary
pediatric practitioners choose assessments. Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy. Revue
Canadienne D’ergotherapie, 76(1), 56–64.
Lalor, A., Brown, T., &Murdolo, Y. (2016). Relationship between children’s performance-based motor skills
and child, parent, and teacher perceptions of children’s motor abilities using self/informant-report
questionnaires. Australian Occupational Therapy Journal, 63(2), 105–116.
Liong, G. H. E., Ridgers, N. D., & Barnett, L. M. (2015). Associations between skill perceptions and young
children’s actual fundamental movement skills. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 120(2), 591–603.
Mathisen, G. (2016). Effects of school-based intervention program on motor performance skills. Journal of
Physical Education and Sport, 16(3), 737–742.
Mitsis, E. M., McKay, K. E., Schulz, K. P., Newcorn, J. H., & Halperin, J. M. (2000). Parent–teacher
concordance for DSM-IV attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a clinic-referred sample. Journal of
the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 39(3), 308–313.
Okely, A. D., Booth, M. L., & Chey, T. (2004). Relationships between body composition and fundamental
---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------- 565
JPES ® www.efsupit.ro
LIVIA TAVERNA, MARTA TREMOLADA, SABRINA BONICHINI, FRANCESCA SANGIULIANO INTRA,
ANTONELLA BRIGHI
---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
movement skills among children and adolescents. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 75(3), 238–
247.
Okely, A. D., Booth, M. L., & Patterson, J. W. (2001). Relationship of Cardiorespiratory Endurance to
Fundamental Movement Skill Proficiency among Adolescents. Pediatric Exercise Science, 13(4), 380–
391.
Ruiz-Pérez, L. M. (2001). Observing and detecting pupils with low motor competence in school physical
education. International Journal of Physical Education, XXXVIII(2), 73–77.
Sattelmair, J., &Ratey, J. J. (2009). Physically Active Play and Cognition: An Academic Matter? American
Journal of Play, 1(3), 365–374.
Schrader, F. W. (2001). Diagnostic competency of teachers: Components and effects (91-96). InD.H. Rost,
Dictionary of educational psychology (pp. 91–96). Weinheim.
Skinner, R. A., &Piek, J. P. (2001). Psychosocial implications of poor motor coordination in children and
adolescents. Human Movement Science, 20(1–2), 73–94.
Smyth, M. M., & Anderson, H. I. (2001). Football participation in the primary school playground: The role of
coordination impairments. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 19(3), 369–379.
Taverna, L., Tremolada, M., Tosetto, B., Dozza, L., &ZaninScaratti, R. (2020). Impact of psycho-educational
activities on visual-motor integration, fine motor skills and name writing among first graders: A kinematic
pilot study. Children, 7, 1–16.
Tortella, P., Haga, M., Ingebrigtsen, J. E., Fumagalli, G. F., &Sigmundsson, H. (2019). Comparing free play and
partly structured play in 4-5-years-old children in an outdoor playground. Frontiers in Public Health,
7(JUL).
Tortella, P., Haga, M., Loras, H., Sigmundsson, H., &Fumagalli, G. (2016). Motor skill development in Italian
pre-school children induced by structured activities in a specific playground. PLoS ONE, 11(7).
Zoia, S., Biancotto, M., Guicciardi, M., Lecis, R., Lucidi, F., Pelamatti, G. M., Carrozzi, M., Skabar, A., Sugden,
D. A., Barnett, A. L., & Henderson, S. E. (2019). An evaluation of the Movement ABC-2 Test for use in
Italy: A comparison of data from Italy and the UK. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 84, 43–56.
566 ----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
JPES ® www.efsupit.ro