Distributional Semantics in Linguistic and Cognitive Research Article in Italian Journal of Linguistics

Download as docx, pdf, or txt
Download as docx, pdf, or txt
You are on page 1of 3

Distributional semantics in linguistic and cognitive research

Article in Italian Journal of Linguistics · January 2008

Outside generative grammar, cognitive linguistics has stressed a conceptualist view of


semantics, according to which the meaning of a lexical expression is a particular
conceptualization of an entity or situation it is able to evoke. The definition of
conceptualization given by Langacker is illuminating to understand the particular view of
semantics favored in cognitive linguistics:

“The term conceptualization is interpreted broadly as embracing any kind of mental


experience. It subsumes (a) both established and novel conceptions; (b) not only abstract or
intellectual “concepts” but also sensory, motor and emotive experience; and (c) full
apprehension of the physical, social, cultural, and linguistic context (Langacker 1998:3)”

Although the linguistic context appears as one of the ingredients of human


conceptualization, the emphasis of cognitive semantics is on an intrinsically embodied
conceptual representation of aspects of the world, grounded in action and perception
systems. The distributional constraints to which linguistic constructions obey are intended to
receive a functional explanation in terms of the principles governing our processes of
conceptualizing the world. Therefore, it is embodied conceptualization to be the source of
meaning and to explain linguistic distributions, rather than the other way round.

Logico-philosophical and formal models of language have always emphasized a denotational


approach to semantics, within the tradi tion of model-theoretic and referential semantics of
Gottlob Frege, Alfred Tarski, Rudolf Carnap, Donald Davidson, Richard Montague, among
many others. The basic tenet of this view is best described by David Lewis’s
statement that “Semantics with no treatment of truth-conditions is not
semantics” (Lewis 1972:169).

Even if we admitted that distributional analysis tells us something interesting at all about
language, this could not be said to be something about meaning

cogitive approaches and model-theoretic ones agree on refusing distributional semantics


because meaning can not be explained in terms of language-internal word distributions, but
needs to be anchored onto extra-linguistic entities, being them either conceptual
representations in the speakers’ mind or objects in the world.

J.R. Firth “You shall know a word by the company it keeps” (Firth 1957:11)

Semantica distribucional  methodological principle for semantic analysis.

The distributional method is indeed common in lexicography, which keeps an unbreakable


tie with corpus linguistics. Corpora and statistical methods to analyze the word behavior in
contexts (e.g. concordances, association measures, etc.) are parts and parcels of the
lexicographer’s toolbox.
In psychology, the DH finds one of its strongest and explicit assertions (under the name of
Contextual Hypothesis) in the work by George Miller and Walter Charles, who argue for a
“usage-based” characterization of semantic representations:
“What people know when they know a word is not how to recite its dictionary definition –
they know how to use it (when to produce it and how to understand it) in everyday discourse
[...]. Knowing how to use words is a basic component of knowing a language, and how that
component is acquired is a central question for linguists and cognitive psychologists alike.
The search for an answer can begin with the cogent assumption that people learn how to use
words by observing how words are used. And because words are used together in phrases
and sentences, this starting assumption directs attention immediately to the importance of
context (Miller & Charles 1991:)”

CONTEXTUAL REPRESENTATION

Miller and Charles try to turn this general claim into a more operative “context-based”
characterization of word meaning. In fact, they argue that repeated encounters of a word in
the various linguistic contexts eventually determine the formation of a contextual
representation, defined as follows: the cognitive representation of a word is some
abstraction or generalization derived from the contexts that have been encountered. That
is to say, a word’s contextual representation is not itself a linguistic context, but is an
abstract cognitive structure that accumulates from encounters with the word in various
(linguistic) contexts. The information that it contains characterizes a class of contexts
(Ibidem:5; the emphasis is mine).

Miller George A. & Walter G. Charles 1991. Contextual correlates of semantic similarity.
Language and Cognitive Processes VI. 1-28.

 Distributional semantics offers both a model to represent meaning with vectors and computational
methods to learn such representations from language data (but not only ...)

 cf. Multimodal Distributional Semantics (Feng & Lapata 2012, Bruni et al. 2014) Distributional representations
are continuous and gradable

 Distributional semantics is based on a contextual and usage-based view of meaning

 The output of DSMs is a measure of semantic similarity/relatedness Distributional semantics is


primarily a model of the lexicon

“words are not mental objects that reside in a mental lexicon. They are operators on
mental states. From this perspective, words do not have meaning; they are rather cues to
meaning” (Elman 2014: 129)
Erk K. 2012. Vector Space Models of Word Meaning and Phrase Meaning: A Survey.
Linguistics and Language Compass 6:635–653
Erk K. 2016. What do you know about an alligator when you know the company it keeps?
Semantics & Pragmatics 9:1–63
Erk K, McCarthy D, Gaylord N. 2013. Measuring Word Meaning in Context. Computational
Linguistics 39:511–554

In standard distributional semantics, each word is assigned a single vector, which is an


abstraction over all its contexts of use, thus encompassing all the word senses that are

3.1. Single representation, polysemy via composition

The predominant, single representation approach is similar in spirit to structured ap- proaches to the lexicon like the
Generative Lexicon (Pustejovsky 1995), Frame Semantics (Fillmore et al. 2006),

These approaches aim at encoding all the relevant information in the lexical entry, and then define mechanisms to deploy the
right meaning in context, usually by composition. As an example, Pustejovsky (1995, 122-123) formalizes two readings of
bake, a change of state (John baked the potato) and a creation sense (John baked the cake), by letting the lexical entries of the
verb and the noun interact: If bake combines with a mass-denoting noun, the change of state sense emerges; if it combines
with an artifact, the creation sense emerges.

You might also like