#Hooked - Revealing The Hidden Tricks of Me - Patrick Fagan PDF
#Hooked - Revealing The Hidden Tricks of Me - Patrick Fagan PDF
#Hooked - Revealing The Hidden Tricks of Me - Patrick Fagan PDF
‘This book is essential reading for anyone wanting to bridge the gap between
the science and practice of influence. Packed with original, yet evidence-
based, suggestions, it digests the core findings of behavioural economics and
psychology to help readers make smarter decisions and increase their impact
on others. Highly recommended!’
Tomas Chamorro-Premuzic, Professor of Business Psychology at UCL
and Columbia University, and CEO of Hogan Assessments
‘Warning! Learnings from science will make your communications more effective.
Patrick makes the science accessible and shows how to apply it pragmatically.
A must-read for marketers and agencies.’
Phil Barden, author of Decoded: The Science Behind Why We Buy
‘Want more people to listen to what you have to say? This book provides a
handy toolkit to make anyone a better communicator.’
Jonah Berger, Wharton Professor and bestselling author of Contagious:
Why Things Catch On
PATRICK FAGAN
The right of Patrick Fagan to be identified as author of this work has been asserted by him in
accordance with the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988.
The print publication is protected by copyright. Prior to any prohibited reproduction, storage in a
retrieval system, distribution or transmission in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical,
recording or otherwise, permission should be obtained from the publisher or, where applicable, a
licence permitting restricted copying in the United Kingdom should be obtained from the Copyright
Licensing Agency Ltd, Barnard’s Inn, 86 Fetter Lane, London EC4 1EN.
The ePublication is protected by copyright and must not be copied, reproduced, transferred,
distributed, leased, licensed or publicly performed or used in any way except as specifically permitted
in writing by the publishers, as allowed under the terms and conditions under which it was purchased,
or as strictly permitted by applicable copyright law. Any unauthorised distribution or use of this text
may be a direct infringement of the author’s and the publisher’s rights and those responsible may be
liable in law accordingly.
All trademarks used herein are the property of their respective owners. The use of any trademark in
this text does not vest in the author or publisher any trademark ownership rights in such trademarks,
nor does the use of such trademarks imply any affiliation with or endorsement of this book by such
owners. Pearson Education is not responsible for the content of third-party internet sites.
10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
20 19 18 17 16
NOTE THAT ANY PAGE CROSS REFERENCES REFER TO THE PRINT EDITION
PART 1 AN INTRODUCTION
1 The science of communication............................................................................ 6
2 The brain...............................................................................................................20
Conclusion..........................................................................................................181
Index.....................................................................................................................183
viii
This book is so scientific that the references list – at almost 20,000 words – is
too large to include! To view the citations, please go to:
http://www.brainchimp.co.uk/hooked/notes
ix
PUBLISHER’S ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We are grateful to the following for permission to reproduce copyright material:
Photos
Photo on pxi © Liliya Kulianionak/Shutterstock.com; photos on p12 ©
R. Gino Santa Maria/Shutterstock.com (woman with rose) and © John Foxx
Collection/Imagestate (man with sunglasses); photos on p28 © 123rf.com (spider,
rock and rose) and © Andrey Starostin (mushroom); photo on p38 © Richard
Levine/Alamy Stock Photo; photo on p40 © Eric Isselee/Shutterstock.com;
photo on p42 © Simon_g; photo on p48 © Jack Hinds/Alamy Stock Photo;
photo on p53 © David Pearson/Alamy Stock Photo; photo on p61 © Guy Bell/
Alamy Stock Photo; photo on p63 © USBFCO/Shutterstock.com; photo on
p68 ‘Monkey Sparrow’ by Sarah DeRemer; photo on p70 © Ana Blazic Lav-
lovic; photo on p71 © Justin Kase Zninez/Alamy Stock Photo; photos on p76
from Sajjacholapunt, P. and Ball, L. J., ‘The influence of banner adverstise-
ments on attention and memory: human faces with averted gaze can enhance
advertising effectiveness’, Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 166, 2014; photo on p112
© razorpix/Alamy Stock Photo; photo on p123 © Chris Howes/Wild Places
xi
Figures
Figure on p14 adapted from Lavidge, R. J and Steiner, G. A. (1961). ‘A model
for predictive measures of advertising effectiveness’, Journal of Marketing,
25, 59–62; figure on p22 © 123rf.com; figure on p82 adapted from Huron, D.,
Sweet A nticipation: Music and the Psychology of Expectation, MIT Press, 206;
figure on p90 adapted from Johnson, E. J. and Goldstein, D. G. (2003). ‘Do
defaults save lives?’, Science, 302, 1338–1339.
xiii
“
When I tried explaining to people what we were thinking about, no one got
it. ‘Well, there’s this duck’, I’d say. ‘And he quacks Aflac’. The response was
always the same: a silent stare. So I stopped telling people. I didn’t even tell
our board; I just said we were trying something very bold and creative for our
advertising campaign.
Just like the Aflac executives, you probably wouldn’t think that a bizarre,
semi-sentient duck would work, that it could actually influence people and help
the business; but it did. Amos reported the campaign helped to drive up rev-
enues by 44% over the following seven years.
So, how do messages work – and how don’t they work – and what does
psychology tell us about it?
Well, read on, dear reader, and find out! In the meantime though, an interest-
ing experiment will serve as an introductory appetiser.
The study in question investigated urban legends.3 Have you heard the one
about the man who bought a McDonald’s chicken burger, only to bite into a fat,
juicy tumour? What about the one that says if chocolate makes you sneeze, it’s
because of all the crushed up insects that fall into it during production? (That
one appears to be true4 – sorry!)
These examples give you a clue as to what kinds of urban legends are the
stickiest. The researchers asked participants to read some popular urban leg-
ends and rate them on a range of factors, such as the elicitation of emotions
and how likely they would be to pass the story on. The chart below shows the
predictive power of various factors for the pass-along variable.
1 Primal Our ‘chimp brains’ still have a huge effect on what we pay
attention to; for example, it’s not very sexy to say so, but sex does sell.
2 Affective We all have first-hand experience with the attention-grab-
bing power of emotion – most of us have wasted hours online looking
at videos of kittens and puppies.
3 Self-relevant Imagine you’re walking down the street, and a stranger
walks past wearing a T-shirt with a picture of your face on it; you would
notice it immediately.
4 Surprising When a waiter drops a tray of plates onto the floor, most
everyone will look (some will even applaud). It’s in our nature to pay
attention to anything surprising.
5 Mystery A whole industry of crosswords, codewords and sudokus
speaks to our innate desire to ‘close the gaps’ in our understanding
and solve mysteries.
6 Ease People generally have little time and motivation to attend to
messages – but mostly, people are just lazy. Which do you take more
often, the escalator or the stairs?
7 Narrative As a species, we have been structuring and sharing infor-
mation using narrative for eons – stories are part of our collective psy-
che; stories are relatable.
8 Memory There is typically a long break between the time a message
is read and the time it’s meant to have an effect on behaviour – so it’s
vital that it’s remembered.
9 Autopilot We have very limited brainpower, so we use non-conscious
shortcuts or rules-of-thumb – called heuristics – to guide our behav-
iour. These can be ‘hotwired’ via a message.
10 Priming Don’t think of a white bear. You will have invariably thought of
a white bear. Priming is influencing behaviour by bringing certain ideas
to the front of people’s minds.
With these insights, your messages will be seen, and your voice will be
heard!
PART
AN
INTRODUCTION
“
HeadOn: Apply directly to the forehead! HeadOn: Apply directly to the
forehead! HeadOn: Apply directly to the forehead!
So goes perhaps one of the worst adverts of all time. HeadOn is a chapstick-
like product that, the brand claims, can be rubbed onto the forehead to relieve
headaches; unsurprisingly, there is no actual proof that it works (see http://
abcnews.go.com/GMA/Health/story?id=2695490).1 Accordingly, the brand was
perhaps a little limited in what it could say in its advertising, and ended up with
the very simple and literal copy you see quoted above.
It was basic and irritating, but guess what? It worked. Sales increased by
234% year on year after the ad’s launch.2
Let’s take another example. Most British readers will be all too familiar with
the price comparison site GoCompare.com – or more specifically, its infuriating
tenor mascot Gio Compario. For those lucky enough to be unaware, in 2009
the brand released an advert featuring a portly opera singer wearing a tuxedo
and sporting a ridiculous curtains haircut and an even more ridiculous twirly
moustache, and singing an extremely catchy song.3
The advert was weird, it was unique, and it was hated. It was so despised and
detested, in fact, that it was named the most irritating ad of the year in 20094 –
and 2010!5 It was hated so much that the brand followed up with a campaign in
which the tenor was killed by various means, including a bazooka; eventually, in
2014, the brand dropped him altogether.6 The brand’s Marketing Officer, Kevin
Hughes, said, ‘It was risky, but a brand has to listen to its customers’.7
But does it?
It seems intuitive that the mascot should be dropped – customers did hate
it, after all. But intuitions and assumptions can often be wrong; what really mat-
ters is the evidence.
Within three months of originally introducing the advert, GoCompare saw an
increase in customer count by 20%, quote volumes by 44%, and brand aware-
ness by a massive 450%.8 In other words, it worked. Dropping the mascot may
have been a mistake, and one based on false assumptions about how commu-
nication works (although it’s too early to tell from the data).
Examples like these highlight the need to take a fresh look at how communi-
cation works, and to challenge old ways of thinking.
2 PART 1 AN INTRODUCTION
“
. . . we are not made to view things as independent from each other. When
viewing two events A and B, it is hard not to assume that A causes B, B causes
A, or both cause each other. Our bias is immediately to establish a causal link.
PART 1 AN INTRODUCTION 3
ACTION POINT
Imagine you work as Communications Manager at a grocery super-
market in the aftermath of the ‘horsemeat scandal’, when many of the
store’s meat products were found to contain horsemeat (when they
were not supposed to).
Craft a Tweet (maximum 140 characters) to send from the brand’s
Twitter account in response to the scandal.
Now spend five minutes thinking about why you wrote that Tweet.
What was your intended aim and how did you go about achieving it?
What principles were behind your thinking? Where did those principles
come from? Are you sure they are correct? What is the scientific evi-
dence for them?
In The 7 Habits of Highly Effective People, Steven Covey outlines the
‘See, Do, Get’ model shown below.18 While many people spend time on
‘Do’ (i.e. putting their beliefs into action),
See
and ‘Get’ (i.e. reviewing and interpreting
the results of their actions), very few spend
time on ‘See’ – in other words, Covey
states that few people spend time think-
ing about and challenging their underlying
Do
beliefs, and that doing so is an important Get
factor for success.
4 PART 1 AN INTRODUCTION
“
Medical practitioners, who were trained in the practice of bloodletting, never
questioned its efficacy. They just assumed it worked because they were taught
it worked and they credulously attributed the fact that some people got
well to the treatment . . . We are faced with a similar problem today . . . For
almost 20 years, advertising programs, marketing courses, and professional
development classes have taught these people principles that can now be
labelled either seriously flawed or outright baloney . . . never before in my
experience has a whole generation of marketing and advertising people been
taught an entire set of principles that is so lacking in a factual basis, and so
influenced by anecdotes and fantasies.
If you answered that any of the statements are true, then you may not be
communicating messages as effectively as you could be.
So what does the science say?
PART 1 AN INTRODUCTION 5
SOCIAL COGNITION
The massive influence of social information on human behaviour is illustrated
by a wealth of case studies from the field of social cognition.
Perhaps the most famous is Kitty Genovese.7 The story goes like this . . . Cath-
erine ‘Kitty’ Genovese was an attractive 28-year-old woman who was stabbed to
death near her home in Queens, New York, one evening in 1964. The important
detail is that Kitty allegedly screamed for help, and although dozens of people
living nearby heard her cries, none called the emergency services. The princi-
ple believed to be at play was ‘the diffusion of responsibility’ – people felt less
responsibility to help since they believed others would do so instead.
Recent investigations suggest that the Kitty Genovese example may be
rather apocryphal;8 however, psychological experiments since then have sup-
ported the principle. One fantastic piece of research involved participants left
sitting in a room to await the start of the experiment;9 as they were waiting,
smoke was released into the room through a vent. The subject was either sit-
ting in the room on their own, or sitting with two other people who were in
fact actors instructed to ignore the smoke and do nothing. On their own, three-
quarters of participants left the room and sought help; in the presence of pas-
sive others, only one in ten did so.
When it comes to messages that work, there are two important implications
from the points made previously. Firstly – and as we’ll cover in more detail later
in the book – messages can be made more effective through understanding
social biases.
8 PART 1 AN INTRODUCTION
However, there are ostensibly three issues with this model, and the many
others like it in the communications literature.
The first potential problem is that this model focuses on attitude change as
paramount. The assumption behind this model, and more explicitly champi-
oned in Azjen’s theory of planned behaviour,21 is that changing attitudes will
lead to a change in behaviour.
But is this certainly the case?
SAY VERSUS DO
Going back to HeadOn and GoCompare, it seems irrelevant to an advert’s suc-
cess whether consumers have a positive or negative attitude towards it. In fact,
a large-scale review of advertising research found that the more an advert was
repeated, the worse consumers’ attitudes became towards it – but that sales
were maintained or even increased in line with repetition!22
In 1934, Richard LaPiere conducted a seminal study which questioned the
assumed attitude–behaviour relationship.23 LaPiere travelled around 1930s Amer-
ica with his student and his student’s wife, both of whom were Chinese; the three
patronised several hotels, motels and restaurants, only one of which (out of 251)
refused them service. Later, LaPiere sent a survey to all of the establishments
asking, ‘Will you accept members of the Chinese race in your establishment?’
10 PART 1 AN INTRODUCTION
Sources: © R. Gino Santa Maria/Shutterstock.com (woman with rose); © John Foxx Collection/Imagestate (man with sunglasses)
their teeth, like a rose, and the others in their mouth, like a cigarette. They
then watched some cartoons and rated them in terms of funniness. Those
made to smile by the pen rated the cartoons as funnier: in this experi-
ment, the behaviour (smiling) drove the attitude (a positive perception of the
cartoons).30
Another great study to illustrate this point asked participants to fill out a sur-
vey on a clipboard to indicate their personal opinions on Israel and Palestine.
When participants flipped the sheet to answer the rest of the survey, a sneaky
adhesive patch on the back of the clipboard peeled away the participant’s orig-
inal answers, leaving responses which completely opposed their actual beliefs.
Then, the participants were asked to explain their answers. Over half (53%) did
not notice any change and consequently argued in favour of opinions they did
not actually give.31
The takeaway is that attitudes are often poor predictors of behaviour; if the
goal of a message is to change behaviour, it is far more effective to do that
directly through psychological principles.
12 PART 1 AN INTRODUCTION
SO WHAT WORKS?
In summary, then, in contrast to standard models of communication, in order
to be effective, messages need to take more of a non-conscious approach
to their audience – using principles of psychology to get noticed and remem-
bered, and ‘nudging’ behaviour rather than trying to change attitudes.
Preference
Affective stage
(i.e. Feel)
Liking
Knowledge
Cognitive stage
(i.e. Learn) Low level of consumer response/
Awareness advertising effect
Source: Adapted from Lavidge, R. J. and Steiner, G. A. (1961). ‘A model for predictive measures of
advertising effectiveness’, Journal of Marketing, 25, 59–62.
While there is a lot of support for the first two steps and the final step (i.e.
awareness, knowledge and purchase), which will be explored throughout this
book, the other three steps are perhaps less robust. These three phases – liking,
preference and conviction – form an attitude component and may actually play
little to no part in the process of an effective message – since, as discussed
before, the link between attitudes and behaviours is often weak or inconsistent,
and since the majority of behaviour is driven by non-conscious factors. What
matters isn’t whether people consciously profess to like or agree with a mes-
sage, but rather if that message is at the front of their minds when it counts.
So, this books suggests a three-step framework:
14 PART 1 AN INTRODUCTION
B Ignite thinking
Getting people to notice a message is the first step, but if they don’t process
the message, it will neither stick in their minds nor influence their thoughts and
C Incite action
There is often a significant gap between the time a message is seen by an
audience, and the time it’s supposed to have its effect. What’s more, psycho-
logical ‘nudges’ can be used to influence behaviour directly.
16 PART 1 AN INTRODUCTION
EXERCISE
SUMMARY
18 PART 1 AN INTRODUCTION
Don’t
EXERCISE
20
Since – as we will see – Spock plays a far lesser role than Kirk, simply look-
ing at a message’s audience in terms of the rational brain is insufficient. To give
an example, eye-tracking research has shown that 75% of prices are not even
looked at once in-store;4 similarly, one study found that consumers form last-
ing impressions of websites within just 50ms of exposure.5 It seems, therefore,
that conscious processes do not account for the entirety of attention and deci-
sion-making – much (if not most) occurs automatically and without conscious
thought.
THREE BRAINS
So how influential is the conscious brain compared to the non-conscious brain
in consumer behaviour?
The first thing to note is that the non-conscious mind typically takes prec-
edence over the conscious mind. As Carter and Frith stated, ‘Where thought
conflicts with emotion, the latter is designed by the neural circuitry in our brains
to win.’6
The reason for this is evolutionary. In the Triune Brain Theory,7 it was sug-
gested that the human brain is the result of the evolution of regions and their
functions from one evolutionary ancestor to the next. Indeed, three distinct
regions (i.e. reptilian, mammalian and human) are visible in the brain, as seen
in the diagram below. Although this model is somewhat simplistic and out-
dated, the general principle has subsequently been confirmed by brain imaging
research.8
2 THE BRAIN 21
The emotional
(‘mammalian’) brain
Cerebellum
Brain stem
Source: © 123rf.com
The oldest and innermost part of the brain, known as the reptilian region, is com-
prised of the brain stem and cerebellum. This region is responsible for reflexes and
maintaining homeostasis; it reacts to basic stimuli in our environment (such as a
wasp flying at our face), and urges us towards base desires such as food and sex.
Next, around this part of the brain developed the mammalian region, com-
prising areas including the amygdala and the limbic system. This area is
responsible for emotional, learning, reward and social processes.
Finally, the human brain – that is, the cerebral cortex – developed around the
outside of the brain, and brought with it functions like planning, reasoning and
language. The cerebral cortex is seen in the diagram above as the ‘wrinkled’
part of the brain. In fact, it is these wrinkles which give human beings their
intelligence and the advanced cognitive functions which set us apart from most
other animals;9 the cerebral cortex being so wrinkled and folded means that it
has a large ratio of surface area to volume – essentially, it means more brain
can be squeezed into the same space.
The upshot of this model is that the emotional, automatic parts of the
brain are evolutionarily older than the conscious cerebral cortex. Since the
non-conscious brain has ensured our evolutionary fitness for longer, and since
it is more deeply embedded in behavioural systems, it is more influential in
decision-making. It is for this reason that, just as one cannot consciously stop
oneself from sneezing or from flinching from sudden movement, the older brain
regions and their goals will take precedence over the rational brain. Therefore,
we cannot help but look at, say, pictures of a sexy topless model, despite,
sometimes, our conscious preference not to.
22 PART 1 AN INTRODUCTION
2 THE BRAIN 23
24 PART 1 AN INTRODUCTION
2 THE BRAIN 25
SUMMARY
The brain
Do
Don’t
26 PART 1 AN INTRODUCTION
PART
SECTION 1
INVITE ATTENTION
Which picture did you look at first? Chances are it was the spider.
There are certain stimuli which will automatically catch our attention. There
are evolutionary reasons for this – if we didn’t look at spiders straight away, we
probably wouldn’t have survived as a species.
Recall that our brain’s ‘doorman’1 processes an estimated 11,000,000 ‘bits’
of sensory information every second, but our conscious brain only has the
capacity to attend to 40 of these,2 so the ‘doorman’ directs this limited atten-
tion to that which is most important – like deadly spiders.
Now recall that there is a tonne of information in the world. Taking Face-
book as just one example, over half of all users have more than 200 Face-
book friends, and the average number of friends is 338.3 That’s a lot of status
updates, photos and chats to process every day. Meanwhile, Ofcom reports
that people spend an average of eight hours and 41 minutes a day on all media
devices combined, and eight hours and 21 minutes a day asleep.4
These two points highlight why it’s so important that any message gets
noticed. Any message you send into the world is competing with two forces:
firstly, there are many messages competing for attention; and secondly, that
attention is extremely limited. Only a very select few messages make it.
EXERCISE
Uncontrollable urges
Can you think of a time in your own personal life when you were
s eemingly unable to resist your baser urges – and perhaps acted
differently to how you might ‘normally’ act? How would you have acted
differently in hindsight? And what was it that caused you to act this
way originally?
I’m going to tell you a personal story – please try not to judge me. In my early
twenties, I was in a ‘hair metal’ band (think Mötley Crüe). I was sure I was
going to be famous – why wouldn’t people go crazy for 80s glam rock in 2008?
As such, and being a young psychology undergraduate, I spent my time trying
to understand the psychology behind successful bands.
The first lesson I learned was that successful bands and musicians gen-
erally had to get people’s attention. For example, one study of music vid-
eos found that physiological arousal was associated with positive emotions
and preference towards that band.1 Of course, being attention-grabbing is a
clear goal of popular music artists – whether it’s Elvis Presley wiggling his
hips salaciously or covering himself in sequins, Miley Cyrus licking a ham-
mer and rolling around naked on a wrecking ball, or Janet Jackson ‘acciden-
tally’ whipping out her pierced nipple at the Superbowl (which went on to be
named by Guinness World Records as the ‘Most Searched-For News Item’
and ostensibly helped her album, released the following month, go triple
platinum2).
The second, related lesson was that certain themes in songs and music vid-
eos help to catch that attention – specifically, sex and violence. Or as some like
30
3 PRIMAL 31
3 PRIMAL 33
Similarly, there is, of course, a strong evolutionary reason for why we pay
attention to sexual stimuli. Remember that Dawkins suggests we exist solely to
serve goals – to survive, and reproduce.23 If we did not pay attention to sex as a
species, we probably would not have done very well on the second front.
Pandas are a nice example. Pandas are currently categorised as an ‘endan-
gered species’, with potentially fewer than 2,500 left in the wild, and a few hun-
dred living in captivity in zoos.24 The fate of the species ostensibly lies on these
captive pandas’ shoulders – yet, they simply don’t want to breed, despite their
handlers best efforts with Viagra, pornography and ‘sexercise’.25 As British
conservationist Chris Packham suggested, perhaps it’s time we just let them
die out.26
Ultimately, sex is very good at getting attention, and thus helps to get
messages hooked in people’s minds. Incorporating sex into a message results
in: more attention paid to it;27 more thoughts about it;28 more interest in it;29
and better memory for it30 – a smorgasbord of effects!
Let’s take an example. During the 2013 Super Bowl, internet hosting pro-
vider GoDaddy.com aired an advert featuring an ungainly geek (apologies to
the actor) explicitly making out with supermodel Bar Rafaeli. The advert was
one of the best-remembered of the Super Bowl,31 and had significant business
effects for the company, increasing domain sales by 40% year on year the day
after the ad aired.32
FOOD
Pinterest is an image-based site where users can manage and share their own
projects, or discover other people’s. Can you guess what topic is interacted
with most on the site?
It’s food – with one survey discovering that 57% of users interact with food
content.33
If you use social media, you have probably not escaped the trend among
users to take artistic shots of their food and share them. The trend is popular
3 PRIMAL 35
FACES
In 1994, Floridian Dian Duyser settled into her kitchen to eat a nice piece of
cheese on toast she had just removed from the grill.43 The 42-year-old took
one bite from the dish before looking down and realising she could eat no
more. She explained, ‘When I took a bite out of it, I saw a face looking up at
me – it was Virgin Mary staring back at me. I was in total shock.’
Duyser kept the sacred sandwich for ten years, during which it brought
her, she claimed, a lot of luck – including $70,000 of winnings from a nearby
casino. For reasons known only to herself, she decided, ten years later, to put
the magical mouthful up for auction on eBay. There, the auction received over
100,000 hits, and the holy toast eventually sold for $28,000.44
Of course – whether Dian Duyser truly believed it or not – the Virgin Mary
did not materialise in her toasted sandwich; rather, it was just a random pattern
of melted cheese and burnt toast, and an example of the phenomenon known
as ‘pareidolia’, where we see a face in what is actually a random pattern.45
CEREAL MASCOTS
Have you ever noticed that children’s cereal brands very often seem to
have their mascot’s faces on the front of the box? And that they’re typi-
cally looking down from the box?
▲
3 PRIMAL 37
SUMMARY
Primal
Do
✓ Use sex, innuendo and attractive people to get attention paid to your
messages.
Don’t
3 PRIMAL 39
Let me tell you another personal story. Again, please try not to judge me for it.
Throughout my life, up until a few months ago, I had never given money to
charity. I had never donated a pound to PETA, a euro to the aged, nor a shekel
to tsunami relief. The point is, I never gave my cash away to any cause.
One cause in particular to which I never donated was The Big Issue. As a
Londoner, I encounter Big Issue sellers on almost a daily basis. Every single
time, however, I essentially ignored the seller; I said, ‘No thank you,’ but every
Big Issue seller failed to pass into that crucial 40 ‘bits’ of consciously pro-
cessed information, and I never bought a copy.
However, one day, I did pay attention. I noticed the seller and his magazine,
and I bought a copy. In fact, I even gave the seller £10 and told him to keep the
change, on a £2.50 magazine. (Watch out, Pride of Britain Awards.)
So, what happened?
Bob the Cat happened.1
Bob the Cat wouldn’t let me use his likeness, so here’s another cute cat instead.
Thirty-six-year-old James Bowen had been a homeless heroin addict for over
a decade; by 2007, he was on a methadone programme and living in assisted
40
4 AFFECTIVE 41
4 AFFECTIVE 43
WHICH EMOTIONS?
Moving on, including fear, which we’ve discussed, there are believed to be
six basic, universal human emotions,27 all of which capture attention (whether
through faces, pictures, words, or whatever), and all of which have an evolu-
tionary explanation.
Emotion Detail
Fear Research34 with the ‘dot probe’ task showed an attentional bias towards fearful
pictures like sharks and guns over neutral ones like furniture. Fear is adaptive as it
helps us avoid harm.35
▲
4 AFFECTIVE 45
As well as these six basic emotions, there are other higher-order emo-
tions, like love, jealousy and awe, which can be used for message stickiness
as well. For example, researchers in France conducted an experiment in which
a moneybox, soliciting money for African children in need, was placed next
to a cash register in 14 bakeries. The moneybox contained one of three slo-
gans across different stores and different days: in some cases there was no
slogan; in others the slogan was ‘DONATING = HELPING’; and in the others
it was ‘DONATING = LOVING’. On average, those who donated gave €0.54 in
the control condition and €0.62 in the ‘helping’ condition. However, when the
slogan contained the word ‘loving’, the average donation per giver was €1.04.28
The takeaway is that there are several emotions across a range of media
which can be used to capture your audience’s attention – for example, feature
a picture of a shark in your PowerPoint when talking about your business’ com-
petitors, replace the word ‘like’ with ‘love’ in your Tweets, use emotional faces
in your adverts instead of neutral ones; and so on.
However, there is ostensibly one important remaining question: which
emotion should you use?
A study on The New York Times found that the only negative predictor of an
article’s popularity was sadness – a standard deviation increase reduced the
likelihood of the article being shared by 16%.29 Additionally, research suggests
that sad faces and images only capture attention over other faces among peo-
ple who are, for example, depressed.41 Sadness, it seems, should be avoided –
so are positive emotions more effective than negative ones?
While there is some evidence which shows, for example, that positive televi-
sion messages are more likely to be remembered than negative ones,30 there is
Alarmed Astonished
Afraid Excited
High Arousal
Angry Happy
Tense Aroused
Frustrated Delighted
Annoyed Glad
Distressed Pleased
Miserable Contented
Sad Satisfied
Low Arousal
Depressed Serene
Gloomy At ease
Bored Calm
Droopy Relaxed
Sleepy / Tired
Source: Data from Russell, J. A. (1980) A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 39(6), 1161.
4 AFFECTIVE 47
EMOTIONS AS MOTIVATORS
It is important to realise the powerful effect emotion can have, not just on
drawing attention, but on motivating behaviour.
Fear, for example, can be incredibly effective at driving decisions. There is
a fantastic sketch by the British comedy duo Mitchell and Webb in which a
boardroom of toothbrush company executives are debating how to increase
their profits – one suggests, to the others’ early disbelief, that they might
be able to convince people that they need to brush their tongues via raising
awareness of ‘microscopic antitongueanoids’ and ‘a gritty tongue surface’.
Similarly, antiperspirant is believed to have become popular in the early twen-
tieth century after advertising played on consumers’ fears that they smelled
bad. Messages can cleverly use fear – be it fear of death, loneliness, rejection,
failure, or anything else – to drive their audience’s behaviour in a certain direc-
tion. A meta-analytic review of almost 100 studies on fear appeals found that
such messages – on topics like, yes, persuading people to brush their teeth –
found that strong fear appeals are more effective at changing behaviour than
low or moderate levels of fear.38
The Corsodyl campaign below, for example, used the power of fear to moti-
vate people to use mouthwash if they suffered from bleeding gums – lest their
SUMMARY
Affective
Do
✓ Keep an eye on internet trends (just like cat videos) to see what kind
of things catch attention and therefore are useful in messages.
▲
4 AFFECTIVE 49
Don’t
51
5 SELF-RELEVANT 53
5 SELF-RELEVANT 55
SEASONAL PERSONALISATION
Here are two great examples of brands using the principle of seasonal
personalisation to increase the chance of hooking your audience:
5 SELF-RELEVANT 57
15%
10%
5%
0%
Dear Student Dear John Smith Dear John
So, ultimately, in addition to the primal and the emotional, there is a third
category of stimuli that will draw attention to messages and help them hook
people – and this time it’s personal.
EXERCISE
Future billboard
Imagine there are no limitations on technology and money. Your job is
to create an interactive billboard to sit in the high street and communi-
cate the message that people should pay their road tax on time. Think
creatively and 5–10 years ahead, when there may well be an entirely
free flow of data from a person’s mobile device to devices around them.
What functionality would you include in the billboard to make sure it
grabbed people’s attention? What would the billboard do and say in
order to be as effective as possible?
SUMMARY
Self-relevant
Do
5 SELF-RELEVANT 59
Don’t
Damien Hirst’s art will almost certainly have grabbed your attention just now
because it’s surprising.
There are two types of surprise to look at: one is basic and reptilian, like
being surprised by a slap in the face; whereas the other is more cognitive, like
being surprised by a bisected cow in formaldehyde in an art gallery. Let’s take
the slap in the face first.
CONTRAST
I always wondered why Michael Bay’s Transformers films were so popular.
Transformers: Age of Extinction was the first film of 2014 to make $1 billion 1
and its $90 million opening-weekend takings in China were an industry record.2
While global audiences love the films, critics loathe them: Age of Extinc-
tion has earned a measly 18% on Rotten Tomatoes. The films generally lack
any plot, character development, narrative tension or intellectual interest; the
franchise appears to be nothing more than just a bunch of big, noisy robots
(sometimes grotesque racial stereotypes and sometimes featuring dangling
61
200 $800
100 $400
0 $0
3
nd
ys
II
bo
er
oc
do
s
ys
Bo
la
er
er
ar
R
ed
Is
Bo
m
or
lH
d
e
ag
e
or
or
Ba
sf
Th
Th
ar
an
m
sf
sf
Ba
Pe
an
an
Ar
Tr
Tr
Tr
For example, you will probably have looked at this illusion because it
appears to be moving.
Interestingly, on the point of Michael Bay’s explosion fetish, there is evi-
dence to suggest that the internet and television may be so addictive simply
because they involve a lot of movement and visual contrast.9 Several studies
have shown that introducing lots of contrast into a television message (such as
cuts and edits) results in increased attention towards that message.10
An experiment in the US selected a group of twenty one-minute-long tele
vision messages from adverts and TV shows; these messages were defined
as being either slow, medium, fast or very fast in terms of edits (0–7, 8–15,
16–23 and 24+ edits, respectively), which were defined as a change in one
camera shot to another within a visual scene. Arousal was measured using skin
conductance and self-report, while a reaction-time memory test of visual rec-
ognition for the clips was taken after viewing. As can be seen, the more visual
contrast or movement in a message, the more attention is paid towards it and
the better it is encoded in memory.11
6 SURPRISING 63
3%
2%
1%
0%
Music at shelf Brand arranged in TV POS display
(noise contrast) block (colour contrast) (movement contrast)
Increase in % stopping to look Increase in % buying
6 SURPRISING 65
SURPRISE
Moving on, the second type of surprise is similar, but more cognitive and less
banana in nature: this kind refers to the automatic attending to stimuli that are
unexpected, distinctive or new. Whereas ‘contrast’ is about stimuli being differ-
ent from the surround environment in some way, ‘surprise’ is more about being
different from mental representations of the world.
For example, your brain probably went into overdrive when you read the
word ‘banana’ just now, because the word didn’t fit your predictions of how
the sentence might have continued.
Researchers from the University of California published the results of a
study which looked at this very thing.22 Participants, wearing EEG caps to
measure their brain activity, read a series of sentences of which some con-
tained semantically incongruous words – like, for example, ‘Other well-known
reptiles are snakes, lizards, eyeballs and alligators,’ or, ‘Turtles are not as smart
as mammals like socks or dogs.’
The data showed a large spike in brain activity when participants read the
incongruous word in the sentence, suggesting these words resulted in a signifi-
cant degree of involuntary attention and processing; as the researchers said,
these words ‘do not go unnoticed’. Their previous experiments also showed
such words were well recognised and remembered, too.23
Essentially, the unexpected – or surprising – words resulted in attention. This
was echoed in the results of the paper An Experimental Analysis of Surprise,24
which presented participants with a pair of words on screen, one above the
other. After the words, a dot appeared on screen for 100ms, either below the
bottom word or above the top one, and participants pressed one of two but-
tons to indicate the dot’s location. Participants did this 29 times; on the 30th
trial, one of the words was written in white text on a black background, whereas
all other words had been written in black on white. This broke with people’s
expectations; it did not match the pattern to which they had habituated.
6 SURPRISING 67
“
I was trying to decide what made a channel consistently enjoyable. A
common factor in my favorite hangouts seemed to be a focus on original
and unpredictable content on each line. It didn’t necessarily need to be
useful, just interesting . . . And then I had an idea – what if you were
only allowed to say sentences that had never been said before, ever?
6 SURPRISING 69
6 SURPRISING 71
EXERCISE
SUMMARY
Surprising
Do
✓ Be creative.
Don’t
✗ Overdo it; again, you don’t want the movement or noise to distract
from or overcrowd the message itself.
6 SURPRISING 73
PART
SECTION 2
IGNITE THINKING
Source: Adapted from Sajjacholapunt, P. and Ball, L. J. (2014). ‘The influence of banner advertisements on attention and memory:
human faces with averted gaze can enhance advertising effectiveness’, Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 166.
80%
60%
40%
20%
0%
No face Mutual gaze Averted gaze
As for the why, the ‘Spreading Activation Theory’ of memory5 illustrates why
depth of processing is so important; a crude illustration is given below.
Smoking
Cancer
is bad
Low elaboration message: smoking is bad for you – it may cause cancer.
Illness
Lung
Smoking
Family Cancer
is bad
Mouth
Misery
High elaboration message: think about how bad smoking is for you. Imagine
the coughing and wheezing caused by lung cancer, mouth cancer – and what
else? What do you think passive smoke is doing to your family – and how will
they feel watching you die?
The elaborated-on message will be better remembered because the mes-
sage has been linked to a greater number of memory ‘nodes’, and the con-
nections are likelier to be stronger. Therefore the chance of the message being
triggered in memory is increased: for example, thinking about family might trig-
ger the memory of the elaborated message, but not the superficial one.
In addition, the elaborated-on message will be more persuasive because the
audience has processed the message and what it means to a greater extent,
therefore having a better understanding of it. For example, for the elaborated
message, the audience will have a much better understanding of the potentially
harmful effects of smoking, which will ostensibly better motivate their future
behaviour.
So how do you facilitate the processing of a message?
If I have three, I have three; if I have two, I have two; but if I have one, I have
none. What are they?
Turn to page 237 for the answer.
Did you turn to page 237? How did it make you feel? Would you like to
know the answer? Would you like to know, also, what the mysterious YouTube
videos were for?
I bet you would!
We are all, as will be discussed, hardwired to solve mysteries and to close
the gaps in our understanding. Whereas new, unusual or unpredictable things
get our attention through surprise, curiosity subsequently causes us think
about this surprising event in order to understand how it fits into our under-
standing of the world – and it can be a very strong motivator to process a
message.
What, for example, would you do with a finished crossword at the back of a
newspaper? Would you treasure it and keep it forever, going back over its fac-
toids again and again?
No. You would throw it away.
A new crossword would sustain your interest since it contains p uzzles
to be solved – and gaps to be closed. In fact, there is a huge amount of
79
of the crashes received significantly more attention than the other; as can be
seen in the Google Trends chart of searches above.7
Why? MH370 ostensibly received so much more attention because it was,
and is, a mystery. There is no puzzle about MH17: we know it was shot down
over Ukraine, and we’ve seen the wreckage in the news. But MH370 disap-
peared without a trace; what happened to the plane is a complete enigma.
Researchers at the University of Michigan published the results of a fan-
tastic experiment in which participants were given thirty problems to solve,
comprising maths, logic and spatial reasoning; afterwards, they were asked
to recall as many of the problems as they could.8 On average, participants
recalled 33% of the problems they solved, but 45% of the unsolved problems.
It seems that mysteries stick in our heads.
A great example comes from the critically acclaimed television show The
Sopranos. The final episode was aired in 2007, and yet there continues to be
speculation in the media about the series.9 At the end of the series finale, right
before the expected denouement, the screen simply cut to black.
Many fans were outraged – they reportedly crashed HBO’s website in their
rush to complain10 – and, even eight years later, speculation is rife about what
the ending really meant, and what really happened to Tony Soprano.
Research suggests that shows like The Sopranos, Breaking Bad, American
Horror Story and the rest, are, at least in part, so successful because of their
use of curiosity through techniques like narrative tension and cliff-hangers.
For example, in one study, participants read one of two versions of a
crime story, in which both suspects were equally likely to have committed
the crime and so uncertainty was high, or where uncertainty was low since it
was clear which of the two was the suspect; rated enjoyment of the story was
7 MYSTERY 81
Tension
Prediction
Time
Source: Adapted from Huron, D.B. (2006). Sweet Anticipation, MIT Press.
Recall from the chapter on surprise that people show a significant increase
in brain activity to incongruous words in sentences, like, ‘Turtles are not as
smart as mammals like socks or dogs.’
White
Fur?
socks?
Cat
GAP
Mammal Sock
Dog
In a practical sense, there are ostensibly two uses for curiosity when it
comes to crafting effective messages.
7 MYSTERY 83
7 MYSTERY 85
PUZZLES IN MESSAGES
On the one hand, of course, it’s possible to use very literal puzzles in a mes-
sage. For instance, using questions instead of statements has been shown to
increase message elaboration:24 recall of information in a presentation was sig-
nificantly enhanced by asking rhetorical questions rather than just presenting
information. Similarly, simply asking people to imagine a message rather than
read it has been shown to be effective.25
However, on a more subtle note, it is possible to engage curiosity and co-
creation through the use of metaphors.
Metaphors work by presenting a small riddle to the audience. The definition
of a metaphor is the application or juxtaposition of a word or phrase (the vehi-
cle) to an object which is not actually applicable or related (the target). Since
the two things are not literally related, the audience is presented with a puz-
zle to solve – that is, what is the connection between these two things? The
result, as researchers have suggested, is the semantic and evaluative mapping
of attributes from the vehicle to the target.26
Let’s take a famous metaphor from Romeo and Juliet: ‘Juliet is the sun’.
Presumably, Romeo wasn’t actually suggesting that Juliet is a massive sphere
of hot plasma; thus, with this juxtaposition of unrelated elements, an audi-
ence’s curiosity is piqued. How can Juliet be a sun? Loewenstein’s gap theory
tells us that the audience will seek to find the semantic connection between
these two things and close the gap, and in so doing, the attributes of the sun
are transferred to Juliet.
The sun
Juliet
7 MYSTERY 87
EXERCISE
Mystery
Do
Don’t
✗ Be stale or predictable.
✗ Feel that the ‘puzzles’ need to be overt: metaphors can also be very
effective.
7 MYSTERY 89
“
Human nature is above all things lazy’
HARRIET BEECHER STOWE
100%
80%
60%
40%
20%
0%
k
en
nd
ce
ry
ria
ga
ar
an
nd
U
iu
ga
an
ed
la
st
rtu
m
rla
lg
Po
Au
un
Fr
en
Sw
Be
er
Po
he
H
D
et
N
Source: Adapted from Johnson, E. J. and Goldstein, D. G. (2003). Science, 302, 8–1339.
This is data for consent rates for organ donation: the countries on the left
are opt-in, and those on the right are opt-out.1 Even with something as affect-
ing as organ donation, people are generally happy to do whatever’s easiest – in
this case, sticking with things as they are rather than having to make an effort-
ful decision for oneself.
This is a cognitive bias known as ‘status quo bias’: essentially, people tend
to leave things as they are since change requires effort.
Recall that we are cognitive misers – we have very limited brainpower, not to
mention physical energy and time, to spend on making choices and following
up with them behaviourally. The upshot of this is that we tend to avoid deci-
sions that are too difficult, and we generally take the path of least resistance.
People are so lazy, for example, that they will eat less food if it is simply
placed out of their reach. Food psychologist Brian Wansink conducted an
experiment with secretaries in an office, who were given an open bowl of
Hershey’s Chocolate Kisses to munch on.2 The researchers counted how many
90
SUGGESTION
The first implication is that people are highly susceptible to the power of sug-
gestion. In other words, people will often simply do what they are told – to do
otherwise might require effortful thought.
Going back to the study on people’s choice of the escalator over stairs,5
it was found that the proportion of people could be increased from 8% to
16% simply by introducing a sign saying, ‘Stay Healthy, Save Time, Use the
Stairs’.
One paper explained how a $5 intervention helped a small business owner
increase her turnover to a phenomenal degree, using just the power of sug-
gestion.7 The $5 was spent on a cardboard prompting card for use by the
business owner and her employees: the card served to remind the staff to
8 EASE 91
SIMPLICITY
The second upshot of our tendency to be lazy is that messages should be sim-
ple if they are to succeed; the truth is that audiences typically lack both the
motivation and the resources to digest a long, complex message.
Remember – if something is too difficult, often people simply will not bother.
For example, leading behavioural scientist Sheena Iyengar and colleagues
looked at pension plans in the United States:10 specifically, does adding more
fund options – from, for example, a range of banks, insurers and investors –
increase enrolment? In actual fact, no; Iyengar found that participation rates
decreased as the number of funds offered increased, ostensibly since this
made the pension plan more complicated. With all other factors being held
equal, adding ten fund options resulted in a drop in participation rates between
1.5% and 2.0%; plans offering only two funds enjoyed a 75% participation
8 EASE 93
CONCRETENESS
In a similar vein to simplicity, the third and final implication of the inherent lazi-
ness of our species is that messages should always be concrete over abstract.
Try, for example, to think of the word this definition is describing:
The first definition was for philosophy, and the second was for bed. The
second word was probably much easier to name; this is ostensibly because
bed is a concrete concept while philosophy is abstract. Concrete words have
clearly defined, familiar meanings which are easily accessible, while abstract
words are less inherently meaningful to us.
Concreteness enhances memorability and persuasion because it makes
cognitive processing much easier: concrete concepts are retrieved and pro-
cessed much more efficiently, while abstract concepts are more logical and
less emotional, and are retrieved and processed slowly. Evidence suggests
that abstract words involve linguistic processes in the brain, while concrete
words more heavily involve nonverbal, semantic processes, although they acti-
vate linguistic processes as well.16 To put it simply, concrete stimuli are more
effective since they engage the ‘monkey brain’.
Putting it all together, as illustrated in the crude diagram below, concrete
messages will form quick and strong connections in memory, while abstract
messages will form slow and weak connections.
New brain
String
theory
Old brain
New brain
String
theory
Old brain
8 EASE 95
MAKE A FISCAL
TRANSACTION WITH
THE UPMOST BUY NOW!
URGENCY!
EXERCISE
8 EASE 97
Ease
Do
✓ Use concreteness (e.g. short words over long words, or images over
words at all) to engage the emotional brain.
Don’t
99
17.8
17.6
17.4
17.2
17.0
Statistics and Statistics Narrative Neither
narrative
9 NARRATIVE 101
EMPATHY
When you hear a story, an amazing thing happens inside your brain.
To understand this, we first need to go back to the 1980s, when Italian
neurophysiologist Giacomo Rizzolatti and his colleagues conducted a series
of experiments which would go on to be extremely influential – although they
didn’t know it at the time. Rizzolatti et al. placed electrodes in the brains of
macaque monkeys in order to record neuronal activity when the monkeys
picked up pieces of food – but they discovered something amazing. The same
neurons that fired when the monkeys picked up the food also fired when they
simply saw a human researcher do the same thing.21
This was ultimately the first study of a great many in what was to be an exciting
and seminal field – mirror neurons. Essentially, when we see someone else doing
something, to some degree we experience it ourselves; when you see someone
get slapped in the face, in your mind you get slapped in the face as well.22
FAMILIARITY
Take a group of very young goslings – if you move a cut-out in the shape of a
hawk over their heads as if it were flying above, they will instinctively cheep,
9 NARRATIVE 103
MEANING
In a landmark study published in 1944, a group of college students were
shown a short film in which two triangles and a circle moved across the screen
in an abstract fashion – and the students were asked to describe what they
thought was happening.33
All but one of the students described the scene in terms of narrative –
that is, social actors setting out to achieve a goal. One example included the
explanation that the two triangles were men who were fighting over the woman
circle, who, herself, was trying to escape.
The implication is that stories are how we make sense of the world; we
interpret events in terms of cause and effect. This illustrates the third and final
9 NARRATIVE 105
Similarly, research has shown that a set of stimuli is more easily remem-
bered if they are causally related. In one experiment, participants viewed 32
pairs of sentences and then, subsequently, were given one of the sentences
from each pair and asked to recall its match. The sentence pairs were of vary-
ing degrees of causal relatedness, as confirmed by a pre-study: for example,
40% 5.3
30% 5.1
20% 4.9
10% 4.7
0% 4.5
Causal Non-causal
Cued recall Perceived truthfulness
To sum up, the final reason why stories are so effective is that they make
messages meaningful: by providing a link between the different parts of a mes-
sage, those parts become easier to process, comprehend and remember.
So, overall, narrative is effective because it makes an audience personally
experience information, and because it makes processing easier by hijacking
existing memory structures and providing a meaningful link between different
elements.
And as for the power of narrative in a business context, this was fantas-
tically demonstrated in a recent analysis of Super Bowl adverts.38 The five-
act model of narrative supposes that effective stories comprise five parts: the
9 NARRATIVE 107
EXERCISE
Storytelling
You want to tell a story to engage and persuade audiences around
cycling more and driving less. Create a story by coming up with each of
the prerequisite parts, and then putting them together in a final narrative:
1 Setting
2 Character
3 Character’s goal
4 Character sets out to achieve the goal
5 Conflict and tension
6 Climax of story and resolution of tension
7 Epilogue
Narrative
Do
Don’t
✗ Forget every element that is needed for narrative – that is, with a
beginning, a character, a goal, an effort to achieve the goal, dramatic
tension, and a resolution.
9 NARRATIVE 109
PART
SECTION 3
INCITE ACTION
80%
60%
40%
20%
10 months
2 months
12 hours
2 weeks
3 weeks
1 month
4 hours
8 hours
2 years
10 min
20 min
2 days
3 days
6 days
1 hour
2 hour
1 day
0%
1 2 3 4 5 6
Per cent choosing Dasani out of a choice of four bottled water brands
50%
40%
30%
20%
10%
0%
0 4 12
How many of the twenty pictures had a Dasani bottled water
As for why some stimuli are better remembered than others, there are four
basic principles.
115
REPETITION
‘Education, education, education.’
This was Tony Blair’s slogan on, yes, education in the run up to the 1997
election. It was so successful that it has been named as Blair’s ‘winning gen-
eral election mantra’ by one commentator,3 with another speculating that it
should be inscribed in the Oxford Book of Quotations.4 The book The Elements
of Eloquence: How to Turn the Perfect English Phrase is particularly laudatory,
noting that it received the biggest round of applause and the most headlines of
the Labour party conference.5
Part of the massive success of this mantra is ostensibly repetition: repetition
is key for memory.
There is a huge amount of research to suggest this, that repetition is key
for memory. To give just one illustration, American psychologist Benton Under-
wood presented his participants with a long list of two-syllable nouns, in which
specific words had been repeated one, two, three or four times throughout the
list; afterwards, participants wrote down as many words as they could recall.6
There was a strong positive correlation between repetition and recall: only 27%
of the words shown once were recalled, compared to 71% of the words shown
four times.
Every time a message is repeated, that message’s links in memory are
refreshed and strengthened – as shown in the crude illustration below – making
subsequent spontaneous remembering, or cue-based triggering, of a memory
much more likely.
From a message perspective, research has shown that increasing the fre-
quency of message exposure enhances the likelihood of it being remembered
– as one might expect.
An experiment conducted in the 1980s asked its participants to watch two
half-hour TV shows with advertisements sandwiched in the ‘breaks’; a week later,
they did the same thing again and then completed various measures for each
advert that was shown to them.7 The adverts embedded in the total two-hour pro-
gramming varied according to how many times they were repeated. As can be seen
below, ad repetition had a positive impact on both recall and purchase intentions.
80% 0.4
60% 0.3
40% 0.2
20% 0.1
0% 0
1 2 4
Ad frequency
Ad recall Purchase intentions
10 MEMORY 117
80%
Recall probability
60%
40%
20%
0%
1 2 3 4 5 6
Location in word list
The primacy effect is believed to occur because people have more time
to absorb and rehearse the stimuli at the beginning than those which come
after; and they are better able to remember these before the following stimuli
becomes overwhelming and depletes their cognitive resources which would be
used to process and remember the rest of the stimuli. Meanwhile, the recency
effect is believed to occur because the last stimuli are still available in short-
term memory after the list. This does however mean that the recency effect
may not persist over time – that is to say, it’s often short lived.
In a great example of this point, and of applying the primacy principle to
message stickiness, social psychology pioneer Solomon Asch wrote two
descriptions of a person, which were identical in the traits they described,
except that the traits were in the reverse order.15 One group read about a chap
whose traits were described in order as intelligence, industry, impulsiveness,
criticism, stubbornness, and envy; the other group read a description with the
traits in the reverse order.
Among those who read the traits from intelligence to envy, 32% rated the
person as happy, 74% as good-looking and 64% as restrained, among other
things. Among those who read the traits in the opposite direction, 5% rated the
person as happy, 35% as good-looking and 9% as restrained.
10 MEMORY 119
PEAK–END RULE
The final memory principle to consider here is ‘the peak–end rule’, which posits
that both the end of an experience and its emotional peak will define how it is
remembered.17
Celebrated behavioural scientist Daniel Kahneman co-authored some
papers looking at the sexy world of colonoscopies. One of the papers found
that patients’ ratings of procedure discomfort were positively predicted
by two factors: the maximum (peak) amount of pain the patients felt dur-
ing the procedure; and the pain they felt in the final (end) moments of the
procedure.18
Similar to the recency effect, the peak–end rule partly argues that people
remember the end of an experience. Another of Kahneman’s bum-camera
studies randomly split patients into two groups: those who underwent the
usual colonoscopy procedure; and those who underwent the normal proce-
dure, but with the tube left in place for an additional three minutes in a way
80%
60%
40%
20%
0%
Immediate brand recognition Brand recognition after 24h
Neutral programme Violent programme Sexual programme
10 MEMORY 121
SUMMARY
Memory
Do
✓ Make sure the message (or, within the message, its key point) is the
emotional peak.
Don’t
✗ Forget (!) that people are exposed to countless messages each day,
and that only a select few stick in memory.
✗ Put messages in the middle of a context (or put the key points within
the middle of the message itself); instead, use the beginning and the
end.
“
You are in a different country for one evening on a business trip. For dinner, you
can order something simple at the hotel, but you are thinking of going out to a
restaurant. You are visiting a fairly small town in a developing country with a
poor reputation for food hygiene. Having walked around for 20 minutes, you are
thinking of giving up – but you then find two places where the food looks good.
A. One of the restaurants is empty. You decide to look the restaurant up on an
app on your smartphone, but you can’t find any reviews.
B. The other restaurant is extremely busy and there is a queue of people waiting
at the door. You decide to look the restaurant up on an app on your smartphone,
and you see that hundreds of people have visited the place and left reviews.
123
Almost certainly you will have plumped for the second one. Our experiment
found the same. Respondents were randomly assigned to one of two restaurants,
and then rated on a scale of one to five how likely they would be to eat there.
People were a third more likely to eat at the restaurant if it was popular.
This is one of the most popular examples of a heuristic (i.e. a rule-of-thumb
run on autopilot) – whether you would choose a busy, noisy restaurant to eat
your dinner, or an empty, quiet one. Nearly everybody chooses the busy res-
taurant straight away: the thinking is ostensibly that if the restaurant is busy,
the food must be good.
That is precisely what a heuristic is – a shortcut, or rule-of-thumb, that is
used without thinking to make quick decisions that work. The vast majority of
the time, the busy restaurant will be better and safer than the quiet restaurant;
using the number of people as an instant shorthand is much quicker and easier
than, for example, consciously and deliberatively reading the menu, looking for
the restaurant’s Food Standards Agency certificate, and looking at how clean
the place is inside.
Remember, we have very limited brainpower to spend on decisions: for
example, the best estimate (although it is still a crude estimate) is that we only
process 0.0004% of sensory information consciously.2 At the same time, there
is a huge amount of complexity in the world: we are faced with over 200 deci-
sions a day for food alone.3
SCARCITY
I’m going to tell you a secret (gents, don’t tell your missus): diamonds are
intrinsically worthless.
If you don’t believe me, listen to what former chairman of De Beers dia-
mond company, Nicky Oppenheimer, had to say:11 ‘Diamonds are intrinsically
worthless . . .’ (see http://www.independent.co.uk/life-style/the-gem-trail-
diamonds-from-angolan-mine-to-third-finger-left-hand-1070530.html)
The truth is that De Beers, after essentially forming a diamond monopoly,
restricted global supply of the jewels,12 with the effect that the value of dia-
monds, and thus prices, rose significantly (see McConnell, C. R., and Brue,
S. L. (2005). Economics: Principles, Problems, and Policies (pp. 456)). This hap-
pened because we are hardwired to want something more if it is scarce.
11 AUTOPILOT 125
0.5 $0.50
0.4 $0.40
0.3 $0.30
0.2 $0.20
Liking Attraction Cost (per lb)
2 Cookies 10 Cookies
EXERCISE
SOCIAL PROOF
If all your friends jumped off a bridge, would you do it too? Actually, you very
well might do.
Imagine you’re walking down the street, and you notice a group of people
standing there staring up at the sky; there doesn’t seem to be anything there
but a lot of people are looking. Would you stop to look?
Influential social psychologist Stanley Milgram published a paper in 1969
looking at this very thing.21 Milgram corralled a group of researchers onto a
New York street and had them nonchalantly mingle with the passing pedes-
trians; at a given, discreet signal, the group stood together and stared for a
minute at a building’s sixth-floor window – where nothing was actually happen-
ing. During this time the street was filmed, so that a pair of judges could count
11 AUTOPILOT 127
80%
60%
40%
20%
0%
0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15
Stopping Looking
Source: Adapted from Milgram, S., Bickman, L. and Berkowitz, L. (1969). ‘An note on the drawing
power of crowds of different size’, Journal of Personality and Psychology, 13 (2), 78–82.
This is the social proof heuristic – the rule-of-thumb which says, ‘If everyone
else is doing it, it must be good.’
This is why, to give an example, you rarely see discarded newspapers
on their own around London Underground: instead, they seem to gather
together in certain places, like the bottom of the escalator. In fact, a study
was c o-authored by Cialdini in which unknowing participants were discretely
observed as they returned to their car, where researchers had placed a leaflet
on their windshield for half of the participants, a person walked past at that time
and picked up a piece of litter, while for the other half, the person walked past
but didn’t pick anything up. In the latter instance, 39% of people dropped the
leaflet on the floor; however, when the social norm of environmental responsibil-
ity was activated by a person picking up a piece of rubbish, only 18% littered.22
Social proof is adaptive. Not only does it aid social interactions and hierar-
chies, but it provides an instant barometer of what is good and safe to do: if
everybody eats the red berries, I can eat the red berries; if everyone avoids the
dark cave, I would do well to avoid it; and if everyone is running in the opposite
direction, I should probably run too.
For communications, social proof helps to make messages sticky by implic-
itly encouraging audiences that the target behaviour is the correct course of
action to take.
Imagine, for example, that you own a public radio station that depends on
fundraising to keep afloat. During an on-air fund drive, the DJ will play clips
Or imagine that you work at Her Majesty’s Revenue and Customs and you
want to make sure everyone pays their taxes on time. If you want to increase
the proportion filing their tax return on time, what single sentence might you
add to the letters reminding people to do so?24
80%
60%
40%
20%
0%
No sentence Recipient's Recipient's Recipient's
country town postcode
EXERCISE
AUTHORITY
Stanley Milgram conducted another seminal experiment – you may be famil-
iar with it.25 Forty men participated in what they thought was a test of learn-
ing and memory: each was told to take the role of a ‘teacher’ while another
11 AUTOPILOT 129
40%
20%
0%
Dentist Dental assistant Secretary
If, however, as a sender you are personally lacking in authority, it’s always
possible to borrow some from someone else. One experiment discovered
that accompanying a picture of an object with a picture of a relevant celebrity
expert – such as a sports shoe with Andre Agassi – increased favourable atti-
tudes towards the object by 12% and recognition memory by 10%.32
Furthermore, persuasiveness can be increased through authoritative com-
municating. For example, a salesperson trying to sell a cleaning device for
an eight-track tape player (it was the 1970s . . .) was successful 22% of the
time when their spiel included, ‘Here is a thing we have on special that they
tell me will keep your tape player clean,’ and 67% of the time when it included
‘Here is a device we have on special that will clean the dirt and tape oxide from
the guides, the head, and especially the drive wheels of your tape player.’33
Though making messages authoritative can be more subtle than this. A
study used real court transcripts to simulate a witness being questioned, in
which the witness’ language made them out to be either powerful or power-
less.34 Powerful speech contained more intensifiers (e.g. ‘definitely’), hedges
(e.g. ‘kind of’), questions, gestures, uses of ‘sir’, and dropped consonants (e.g.
‘jus’ now’), and fewer one-word answers. Participant ratings showed that wit-
nesses using powerful language were rated as more credible; and a different
paper found the same thing for ads.35
In the end, adding authority to your message can increase compliance by
making the request appear to be the sensible thing to do – even if that request
is to murder with electricity!
11 AUTOPILOT 131
RECIPROCITY
Think back to last Christmas. Did you receive any presents which ended up
in the cupboard, down the charity shop, in the bin, or rewrapped for someone
else? Perhaps a pair of goofy Simpsons socks, a head-massager, or cheap
perfume?
To put it kindly, we tend to buy a lot of pointless, worthless tat at Christ-
mas. A popular gift of recent years has been ‘The Useless Box’ – a nondescript
black box with a single switch, which, when pushed on, activates a robotic arm
which pops out of the box and pushes the switch back off.
These gifts might seem fun and harmless, but the situation goes from ‘ho,
ho, ho’ to ‘bah humbug’ when we actually look at the figures: one report esti-
mated that Brits waste over £2 billion on unwanted Christmas gifts every year.36
So why do we act so irrationally at Christmas? Why do we buy presents that
we know people don’t want?
To answer this, we need to first turn to a Christmas-themed experiment
published in 1976.37 Sociologists Phillip Kunz and Michael Woolcott bought
almost 600 Christmas cards and sent them to inhabitants of Midwestern Amer-
ica selected at random from Polk Directories. To emphasise, these recipients
were chosen completely at random and had no relation to the researcher who
signed and sent the card.
One in five people sent a Christmas card back.
The principle at play here is reciprocity – a deeply hardwired heuristic which
says, ‘If somebody has helped me, I will help them in return.’
Reciprocity is a powerful motivator because it’s so key to who we are as a
species. Humans are very social animals who have evolved to live in groups
and survive by cooperating with one another; if all (or most) of a group’s mem-
bers were freeloaders, taking from the group without giving back, the social
group could not function.38
11 AUTOPILOT 133
EXERCISE
LIKING
One sunny June evening in 1994, a nondescript white Ford Bronco was cruis-
ing through the streets of Los Angeles at a leisurely 35 miles per hour. Several
police cars trailed behind with their sirens blaring, as news helicopters thun-
dered above. This was of course OJ Simpson’s nationally televised slow-speed
chase before his trial for the murder of his ex-wife Nicole Brown and her friend
Ronald Goldman.
To state some facts of the case (see http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/
O.J.Simpson_murder_case):52 Simpson owned a pair of gloves holding his
DNA, with one of the pair being found at the crime scene and the other testing
positive for Goldman’s blood and being found in a bush on Simpson’s estate,
near where a house guest reported hearing movements around the time of the
murder; before his car chase, Simpson wrote what was most likely a suicide
letter, signing off, ‘Don’t feel sorry for me. I’ve had a great life, great friends.
Please think of the real O.J. and not this lost person’.
Whatever did happen that day, one would infer that the evidence, rightly or
wrongly, would be against Simpson and that he would be found guilty; yet, he
was acquitted. Why?
In 1920, noted psychologist Edward Thorndike published a paper in which
he first coined the term ‘the halo effect’ to refer to his finding that percep-
tions of individuals’ different traits were strongly connected.53 Thorndike asked
11 AUTOPILOT 135
80%
60%
40%
20%
0%
No note Post-it Signed Post-it
EXERCISE
11 AUTOPILOT 137
40%
30%
20%
10%
0%
Eight stamps Ten stamps with two
already completed
EXERCISE
SUMMARY
Autopilot
Do
✓ Realise that most decisions are made on the basis of quick shortcuts.
11 AUTOPILOT 139
✓ Imply that everyone else is doing the behaviour you wish your
recipients to do.
Don’t
✗ Think that people can ‘outsmart’ heuristics: they are just like optical
illusions which cannot be ‘unseen’.
141
The results, shown in the above chart, tell us two things. Firstly, choosing
Lipton Ice could only be significantly subliminally influenced when participants
were thirsty; the authors concluded that goals cannot be subliminally manipu-
lated, but the solutions to those goals can. This is an important point which will
be addressed later – namely, subliminal persuasion may only work in certain
conditions. The second implication is that there is, again, support for Vicary’s
idea that brand purchases can be subliminally influenced.
Looking at these two studies alone, it is clear that subliminal advertising
may actually work. Specifically, what these studies show is that making some-
thing mentally salient – that is, essentially reminding people of something, or
putting it into their minds – can subsequently influence behaviour. This has
been demonstrated earlier in the book where, for example, seeing orange Hal-
loween decorations makes people more likely to think of orange brands like
Sunkist,9 or seeing chocolates on your work desk makes you more likely to eat
them.10
This principle is known as ‘priming’, which refers to the process wherein
exposure to a stimulus activates that stimulus’ memory node in the brain, and
consequently those memory nodes attached to it. To illustrate, the word ‘prim-
ing’ was first used in its contemporary meaning to explain why people who
were given a list of words to memorise, and then asked to think up a spontane-
ous list of words, tended to include those words they had memorised, indicat-
ing those words were still active in memory from earlier.11
In the diagram below, as a very crude illustration, seeing a chair con-
sequently activates, or ‘primes’, the construct of a chair in memory. To a
lesser extent, this also activates memory nodes connected to the chair
node, such as a table, or the behaviour of sitting down. It might therefore
be hypothesised from this crude example that flashing images of a chair
below conscious awareness could make people more likely to sit down than
stand up.
12 PRIMING 143
12 PRIMING 145
Priming people
You are presenting a PowerPoint deck to a group of potential clients.
Think of a way you can prime them – either supraliminally through, say,
a conscious exercise, or subliminally through, say, a subtly placed pic-
ture – in order to achieve each of the results below.
SUMMARY
Priming
Do
Don’t
✗ Overestimate the power of priming, as its effects are often weak and
unreliable.
PART
PUTTING IT TO USE
“
Gents, is there anything worse than going into a lavatory and coming out with
wet shoes? Whether you accidentally marked yourself as your own territory,
or whether you had to slosh through the mess made by others, it’s not a nice
experience. Fortunately, there is a clever – and now extremely popular –
‘nudge’ which reduces splashback at urinals: a fly sticker. There is a certain
point in a urinal where the potential for splashback is at its lowest; if you put
a small sticker or etching of a fly there, mess is significantly reduced simply
because men have an innate urge to aim for it. It’s an amazing application of
the principle of ‘monkey see, monkey do’.
However, back at the workshop, one of the attendees raised his hand.
‘That’s great . . . But how does it help me sell yoghurts?’
The point is – to paraphrase a popular saying – knowledge without a practi-
cal application is useless.
So, in this final section of the book, we’ll take the scientific principles out-
lined previously and explain how to use them in specific contexts – namely
promotion, social media, direct mail and the workplace. We will give concrete,
practical advice on how to hook people with these messages.
But first, the insights are recapped in a handy-reference toolkit for creating a
sticky message.
Every time you are crafting a message, just remember that, when you’re
lost, rather than asking them to ask for directions you should pass men a map.
Here is your communication toolkit for crafting the perfect communication;
simply make sure you follow this process every time you want to create a
sticky message.
1 INVITING ATTENTION
People are usually busy and distracted, and they have relatively limited
attention spans; yet, there is an awful lot of information out there in the
world. Will people even notice your message?
Make sure your message includes at least one of the items for the
following checklist so that it can cut through the noise:
2 IGNITING THINKING
Getting noticed is just the first step: if people do not ‘absorb’ your mes-
sage then they won’t remember it and it won’t influence their thoughts
and behaviours.
In order to be cognitively processed, and thus have an effect in the
minds of recipients, your message must use at least one of the follow-
ing checklist items:
In addition, make sure your message uses at least one of the follow-
ing techniques to enhance its persuasiveness:
The story is very similar for other promotions beyond advertising, as well.
Approximately 50% of all fast-moving consumer goods in the UK are bought
on price promotion, with a 26% average discount.5 This is an awful lot of
money to brands, and the problem is they just don’t work. While discounts may
produce a short-term sales increase, in the long-run they are ineffectual for a
152
On the whole, consumers are cognitive misers who spend very little time
thinking or caring about brands; the most important thing for a brand is to get
inside people’s heads and to be easy to buy. The principles outlined in this
book for getting people hooked can be applied to promotions as follows.
Invite attention
– Be creative or emotional to succeed. Rational arguments are an added
bonus: they can be nice, but they are by no means necessary. Likewise,
sex sells; it’s a cliché but it’s absolutely true. Plus, cute animals and babies
are a sure-fire winner, as are threatening things like spiders and snakes.
13 PROMOTIONS 153
DISTINCTIVE ASSETS
There is a branding principle which can help to bring all of these ele-
ments together and enhance the efficacy of advertising. ‘Distinctive
assets’ are perceptual elements unique to a brand like a logo, colour,
font, jingle, celebrity, mascot, and so on; they are vital to brand growth.11
One example in particular comes from the Felix brand of cat food in
the UK.12 In 1998, Felix accounted for just 5% of the market in single-
serve wet cat food pouches; the market leader, Whiskas, had actually pio-
neered the product. However, over the next five years, Felix actually came
to overtake Whiskas as the market leader, all while consistently spending
less than half the amount that Whiskas was spending on advertising.
▲
40%
30%
20%
10%
0%
Apr-01
Jul-01
Apr-02
Jul-02
Apr-03
Jul-03
Apr-04
Jul-04
Apr-05
Jul-05
Jan-01
Oct-01
Jan-02
Oct-02
Jan-03
Oct-03
Jan-04
Oct-04
Jan-05
Oct-05
Whiskas Felix
13 PROMOTIONS 155
Ignite thinking
– Build narrative into advertisements: those which use stories are more
effective than those without.
– Adverts should be simple – as simple as possible in fact, as research
has shown script complexity is negatively related to effectiveness. Like-
wise, the shorter the sweeter when it comes to slogans and ad copy.
– Prioritise images over words, for both advertisements and promotions.
– Use curiosity and mystery in adverts to pique your audience’s interest –
give your audience puzzles or riddles to solve. Metaphors are particularly
useful.
– Fluency is paramount in promotions – make the product as easy to find
and buy as possible (i.e. utilise physical availability to the maximum).
Incite action
– Use a reach strategy for advertising, if it is to work: reach as many peo-
ple as possible as many times as possible.
– Be the emotional peak of the context in which the advert sits – for
example, do not advertise during highly arousing sex or violence content.
– Do not crowd out your message in your advert by over-the-top emotion
which may be remembered instead.
Principle Explanation
Yellow background13 A price/discount will be perceived as better value if written on a yellow
background, since yellow is associated with discounts.
Reference price14 A reference price (e.g. ‘Was £2, now £1’) makes the sale price appear to
be better value by comparison.
Font size15 A sale price in a smaller font than the reference price causes the buyer to
perceive the sale price as, literally, smaller.
Phonemes16 Prices with phonemes associated with smallness (e.g. ‘teeny tiny’) are seen
as smaller – which may be why 99 works well.
Product descriptors17 Accompanying the price with, say, ‘Low Maintenance’ (versus ‘High
Reliability’) results in the price being seen as lower.
Charm prices18 Certain price endings, especially 99, are associated with value; raising a
price to a 99 end can actually increase sales.
Precise prices19 Large numbers are usually rounded up, so prices with 00s are perceived as
high, and precise prices (e.g. £1.26) as low.
Bundling20 Bundling products/prices together increases product evaluations and sales,
as discounts are inferred.
Drip pricing21 ‘Dripping’ the full price through stages is effective because consumers
commit to the product at the first, cheaper price.
Product options22 Consumers pay more options when starting with ‘the full Monty’ and then
removing features, due to being loss averse.
Price length23 The more digits a number has, the larger it is seen to be; so dropping the
pennies, or having them small (e.g. £1.26) is best.
Price obfuscation24 Prices are associated with a painful loss, so hiding the fact a price is a
price (e.g. by losing the £) can be beneficial.
Time limit25 Making a promotion time-limited or saying ‘While Supplies Last’ increases
sales efficacy.
13 PROMOTIONS 157
Practical value
Awe
Interest
Anxiety
Time at top of home page
General emotionality
Surprise
Positivity
Sadness
–20% –10% 0% 10% 20% 30% 40%
Chance of making ‘Most Shared’ list
158
In the end, online content ostensibly follows the same rules of effectiveness
as other messages, except that memory is perhaps of less importance since
online content is typically more urgent. Other than that, online content needs to
get attention and incite an action; the formula for an affective online message
is presented below.
On the whole, people are cognitive misers online too – they react to web-
sites in just a twentieth of a second, for example. If anything, attention spans
may be even more fractured on the internet. Practical applications for ‘the
psych of like’ are as follows.
INVITE ATTENTION
– Emotion is the most important factor in social updates and online con-
tent like articles and videos according to research: specifically, it should be
highly arousing, and negative emotions tend to do better than bad ones.
– Know what people want online and use it to entice them. As a great
illustration, a strong predictor of success of Kickstarter campaigns was
found by one study to be the use of the word ‘cats’ in the pitch!11
IGNITE THINKING
– Use questions to engage potential readers through the principle of curi-
osity. One piece of research discovered that Tweets containing question
marks are more likely to be retweeted.14
– Make content surprising – bizarre, unintuitive or new information will
make readers curious and encourage them to turn that link from blue to
purple. There is a lot of ‘noise’ online and people are very thirsty for any-
thing new or engaging.
– Keep things short and simple, and don’t overload the message
with content. What is the one key thing you want it to communicate?
While a large study showed that more useful information, such as
hashtags or links) increases the virality of a Tweet, it also showed that
too much information is damaging; the optimum length is 100–120
characters.15
– Post content at the optimum time to maximise its digital ‘physical avail-
ability’: submit it at the highest-traffic days and times for your audience
(typically weekends, and lunch and evenings in the week). Facebook and
Twitter engagement rates are 32% and 17% higher, respectively, at the
weekend; and during the week, Twitter engagement is highest around 12
pm and 6 pm.15
– Attach images to your posts, as they are always effective at both catch-
ing attention and communicating a message.
INCITE ACTION
– Post each message multiple times: reminder posts have been shown to
increase the effectiveness of online campaigns. In fact, one of the strong-
est predictors of a Kickstarter campaign’s success is how many reminder
emails are sent out.16
– Be the emotional peak in your audience’s newsfeed. Your post is likely
to get ignored and forgotten if it’s posted at the same time a celebrity
scandal breaks.
– Heuristics will make social content more behaviourally persuasive. To
illustrate, Kickstarter campaigns can be made more likely to be success-
ful by using heuristics like scarcity (e.g. writing phrases like, ‘You are
being given the chance to . . .’).11
– Give readers something for free in order to encourage reciprocity – a
low-cost giveaway like a free download, or even a joke, weather forecast
or interesting fact, will do the trick.
– Don’t exploit your audience by spamming them with messages
or constantly asking for help. On the Reddit forum ‘Random Acts of
Pizza’, where people ask strangers to buy them a pizza delivery for
free, a strong predictor of success is reciprocity – paying back the
favour.17
– Be trustworthy and credible (i.e. use the authority heuristic) when com-
municating online as trust is low and scepticism is high: use the correct
signals to do this. On ‘Random Acts of Pizza’, the number of ‘karma’
points a user has when asking for pizza is a strong predictor of success
as well.17
EXERCISE
An effective Tweet
Rewrite the following three Tweets using the formula outlined above:
be simple;
excite;
inspire action.
165
On the whole, despite there being a lot of it, and consumer perceptions gen-
erally being unfavourable, direct mail is relatively effective; it can be made more
so using psychological principles.
INVITING ATTENTION
– Personalise the message at the point of contact (e.g. on the front of
the envelope or in the subject line) to catch your recipient’s attention.
Researchers at the Bowman Gray School of Medicine conducted an exper-
iment in which 1,000 households were posted information about cervical
cancer and given the chance to enter a prize draw by returning a postage-
paid postcard:10 envelopes which included the recipient’s name saw a
response rate of 21%, compared to 13% for those which read ‘resident’.
– Use memory/priming to make the message more self-relevant,
so that it gets noticed. In other words, prime the recipient to recognise
the message by making it front-of-mind. A review of almost 300 studies
INCITE ACTION
– Signal authority at the point of contact in order to encourage recipients
to trust, and therefore open, the message. A review found that surveys
EXERCISE
170
INVITE ATTENTION
– Use images and animations in your slides to get attention – sounds are
useful as well, but don’t overdo it as they can be distracting. Research
has shown they enhance comprehension.7 Always incorporate videos into
presentations where feasible – at any rate, avoid reams of dull, monoto-
nous text or speech.
– Be creative in using contrast to get attention – such as punctuating
important points with a loud clap.
– Incorporate emotional stimuli – and include images and words – that
will get your audience’s attention; examples might be pictures of babies,
pictures of faces, swear words, and so on.
– Have a sense of humour when meeting people or giving presentations
– humour is very good at activating the amygdala (i.e. engaging people
emotionally).8
– Don’t be afraid of the unusual Including emotional images, telling jokes,
clapping, and so on, will all captivate your audience, but you may be
concerned about how people will perceive you. Well, remember that atti-
tudes are far less important than behaviours. Do you want the client to
buy what you’re selling, or to like you?
– Be personally distinctive – you don’t have to wear a polka-dotted bow
tie everywhere, but unique, noticeable earrings or cufflinks will earn you
attention personally in your career without being crass.
IGNITE THINKING
– Present interesting and new content Make sure your audience has not
heard everything before – if that is unavoidable, find a way to present it in
a new light.
– Don’t be dry In other words, don’t present statistics and bullet points
one after another – remember that we are dominated by our ‘monkey
brain’. Keep things light and fun.
INCITE ACTION
– Repeat key points throughout the presentation to make them memor
able; also make sure to repeat them at the beginning and the end of the
presentation.
– Be likeable in order to be persuasive. One study videotaped a clip of a
manager – who was actually an actor – interacting with employees, and
participants rated how competent he was;13 when the manager was seen
displaying liking signals (such as asking, ‘How are you doing?’) he was
rated as significantly more competent.
– Use similarity as a route to liking. As an example, Americans were asked
to listen to an audio clip of a candidate applying for a job and rate how
likely they would be to give the candidate the role; however, the appli-
cant’s accent varied.14 These American participants were much more
favourable towards the American-accented candidate.
– Use body language to be liked – for example, mirror people’s stances,
expressions and gestures (known as ‘mimicking’),15 physically touching
them,16 and smiling at them.17
– Be as attractive as you can! Attractive job candidates are more likely to
be hired,18 while obese candidates fare poorly in job interviews.19
A power booster
Before you enter a meeting or go on stage to present, improve your per-
ceived authority by simply spending a couple of minutes in a ‘power
stance’ – for example, lean back in a chair with your feet on the desk
and your hands folded behind your head. A ‘power stance’ involves tak-
ing up lots of space and having open arms and legs.
▲
176
Firstly, you need to think about what your desired outcome is for
the message. Most of the time it will be some sort of behaviour,
like an increase in sales or donations, an increase in website traffic,
or a reduction in the number of people littering. However, some-
times the goal may be more abstract, like inducing more positive
perceptions of salad. What change do you want your message to
achieve?
▲
Next, you must consider how this goal is to be achieved using the
principles outlined in this book. For example, if you want to make
an email more effective, you may want to test the effect of putting
the word ‘damn’ in the subject line; alternatively, you might want
to see if putting a kitten in your dating profile picture gets it more
views. What change do you want to make to affect the outcome
chosen previously? This is your independent variable.
Finally, now that you have designed your test, you need to make
sure it is scientific. For example, depending on the experimen-
tal design, you may need a control group which is identical to the
experimental group except for the thing being tested. Also, you
must subject the data to statistical analysis and check for statisti-
cal significance; additionally, your sample must be sufficiently big
in every group. Overall, your test must be robust if the results are to
mean anything.
Measles cases per year in the USMeasles cases per year in the US
700
600
500
400
300
200
100
0
2001 2002 2003 2004 2005 2006 2007 2008 2009 2010 2011 2012 2013 2014
Many parents in America refuse to have their child vaccinated against mea-
sles out of fear that the measles-mumps-rubella vaccine (MMR) causes autism.
It doesn’t. There has been tonnes and tonnes of reliable research showing
this;2 besides which, the original paper suggesting a link has been discredited
and withdrawn, and the authoring doctor has even lost his medical licence.3
But here’s the thing: these are very rational arguments.
The anti-MMR lobby, on the other hand, has the power of stickiness on
its side: fear is both attention-grabbing and a very strong motivator of behav-
iour; the ‘needles equal bad’ message is a very simple one to comprehend;
the lobby parades apparent case studies of autistic children who had the jab,
using the power of narrative; and parents are ostensibly following the lead of
their friends as well as liked and trusted celebrities like Jenny McCarthy. Dry
double-blind studies have nothing on these techniques.
‘We’re half a chromosome away from chimpanzees and it shows.’ The late
majestic misanthrope Christopher Hitchens once said this,4 and it is vital to
keep in mind. The truth is, we are barely evolved monkeys: despite what we
might think, our behaviour is influenced by basic and hardwired principles,
which are often far more simplistic than we like to admit.
Did you know, for example, that in the month of a suicide highly publicised
in the news, the national suicide rate sees a 3% increase?5
181
EXERCISE
Supporting MMR vaccines
The anti-MMR lobby has a very sticky message, and has had a signifi-
cant influence on behaviour as a result.
So far, the responses from governments have tended to target
rational thinking. For example, Australian prime minister Tony Abbot
announced that parents who don’t immunise their children could be lia-
ble to stop receiving childcare benefits from the state6 – though a lot
of research suggests financial incentives can be poor motivators for
behaviour.7
Using everything you’ve learned in this book, what would you advise
the Australian government to do instead?
Firstly, design a poster advocating the MMR vaccine: what should it
look like, what should be on it, and what should the copy (i.e. headline)
say? Remember, you need to capture attention, get the message ‘taken
on board’, and influencing recipients’ behaviours.
Next, imagine the government is sending out letters to Australian
households encouraging parents to get their children vaccinated. What
one sentence could you add to the letter to make it more effective?
182 CONCLUSION
183
184 INDEX
INDEX 185
186 INDEX
INDEX 187
188 INDEX
INDEX 189
190 INDEX
INDEX 191