Self-Regulation Through Goal Setting
Self-Regulation Through Goal Setting
Self-Regulation Through Goal Setting
BEHAVIOR
AND
HUMAN
Self-Regulation
DECISION
PROCESSES
50,
212-247 (1991)
GARY P. LATHAM
University of Toronto
AND
EDWIN A. LOCKE
University of Maryland
The extant literature on goal setting through 1990 has been reviewed and
integrated by Locke and Latham (199Oa).The result was the development of
a theory of goal setting with special emphasis on its practical implications for
the motivation of employees in organizational settings. The purpose of the
present paper is twofold. First, the theory is summarized and updated with
respect to research completed since publication of the 1990book. Second, the
self-regulatory effects of goal setting are described. Emphasis is given to ways
that people can use goals as a self-management technique.
0 1991 Academic
Press. Inc.
SELF-REGULATION
THROUGH
GOAL
SETTING
213
214
LATHAM
AND
LOCKE
goal difficulty, see Wright, 1990.) Goals can be easy (try to get 5 problems completed in the next 30 minutes), moderate (try to get 10 . . .),
difficult (try to get 15 . . .), or impossible (try to get 50 . . .).
Difficulty is a concept of relationship; it pertains to the relationship between a person and a task or goal. Thus the same task or goal can be easy
for one person and hard for another depending on the persons ability and
experience. On the average, however, the higher the absolute level of the
goal the more difficult it is for a person to achieve it.
Approximately 400 studies have examined the relationship of goal attributes to task performance. It has been found consistently that performance is a linear function of goal difficulty. Given adequate ability and
commitment to the goal, the harder the goal the higher the performance.
We attribute this finding mainly to the fact that people normally adjust
their level of effort to the difficulty of the task undertaken and thus try
harder for difficult than for easy goals. A scatter-plot based on some of
the earliest studies of goal difficulty (derived from Locke, 1968) is shown
in Fig. 1. This linear function is different in shape than the function in
Atkinsons (1958) theory which relates task difftculty to performance.
Atkinsons research showed a performance drop at the highest level of
task difficulty, thus yielding an inverse U function. Knowing task difliculty, however, does not reveal the persons goals and thus makes it
difftcult to predict how well a person will perform the task (Locke &
.
6-
.
.
.
4-
-4
.
:
:
-6
1I
I
100
90
80
70
60
50
40
30
20
10
FIG.
1. The relation
of goal difficulty
to performance
(based on Locke,
1968).
SELF-REGULATION
THROUGH
GOAL
SETTING
215
216
LATHAM
AND
LOCKE
SELF-REGULATION
217
Goal Level
FIG. 2. Main and interaction effects of goals and commitment. Reproduced, by permission of the publisher, from Locke and Latham (l!WOa).
218
LATHAM
AND
LOCKE
and performance; people are more likely to do what they say they will do.
However, when commitment is low, people do not perform in line with
their goals.
In the typical laboratory experiment as well as in many natural settings
in which people are rewarded for compliance, gaining goal commitment is
rarely a problem. In fact, Bassett (1979) viewed goal commitment as so
routine that he argued that a theory of goal rejection rather than of goal
commitment should be developed. Thus, it is not surprising that assigning
people goals, accompanied by a rationale, leads to as high a level of goal
commitment as having people participate in the setting of their goals.
Nevertheless, there has been considerable controversy in the literature
concerning the effectiveness of assigned versus participatively set goals in
achieving goal commitment and increasing performance on the part of
subordinates. A series of 11 studies by Latham and colleagues generally
showed little or no difference in the effectiveness of the two goal setting
methods. In contrast, several studies by Erez and her colleagues showed
that participatively set goals produced greater commitment than did assigned goals. To resolve this disagreement, Latham and Erez, with Locke
as mediator, jointly designed a series of 4 studies in which the effect of
methodological differences between the Latham and Erez studies were
systematically assessed (Latham, Erez, & Locke, 1988). The results revealed that the main reason for the differences in their results was that
Erez assigned goals with curt, brief tell instructions, whereas Latham
assigned goals in a more supportive manner and provided a rationale for
them. This tell and sell style used by Latham was found to be just as
effective in increasing performance as was participation; and both styles
were significantly more effective than the tell style that had been used
by Erez.
More recently, Latham, Winters, and Locke (1991) have suggested that
the key benefits of participation are not due to motivation (e.g., goal
commitment) but rather to cognition (e.g., task strategy development).
Their study found that although participation enhanced a self-report measure of goal commitment, it was not sufficient to make a difference in
actual performance. In contrast, participation in developing effective task
strategies had substantial effects on performance through the mediating
effects of self-efficacy and the quality of the strategies which the subjects
developed and used.
Factors which have been found to enhance commitment fall into two
broad categories, namely, those which convince people that achieving the
goal is possible and those which convince them that achieving the goal is
important or appropriate (Klein, in press). The first class of factors raise
the individuals expectancy of success or what Bandura (1982, 1986) has
termed self-efficacy. These include ability, experience, training, informa-
SELF-REGULATION
THROUGH
GOAL
SETTING
219
tion about appropriate task strategies, past success, and internal attributions (e.g., Earley, 1986a; Hall 8c Foster, 1977; Locke, Frederick, Lee, &
Bobko, 1984; Silver & Greenhaus, 1983).
Managers can play an important role in facilitating goal commitment in
subordinates by persuading them that the goals are both attainable and
important. This can be done by managers asserting their legitimate authority, conveying normative information, showing that the goals provide
opportunities for self-improvement, challenging people to show what they
can do, being physically present at the work site, being supportive and
trustworthy, providing a convincing rationale for the goal, exerting reasonable pressure for performance, being knowledgeable about the task
and job, and serving as a role model for the behavior they desire in the
subordinate (e.g., Earley, 1986b; Likert, 1%7; Mento et al., 1990; Ronan,
Latham, & Kinne, 1973; Podsadoff & Fahr, 1989). For a complete literature review see Locke, Latham, and Erez (1988) and Locke and Latham
(199Oa).
Goals that are assigned by legitimate authority figures typically influence peoples personal goals. Instructions to try for a certain goal even
carry over to later trials in which people are free to choose whatever goals
they want to attain (Locke, Frederick, Buckner, & Bobko, 1984; Locke er
al., 1984). These findings are in alignment with Dember (1975), who, after
examining the literature on the cognitive aspects of motivation, concluded
that in certain settings being asked to do something is tantamount to being
motivated to do it. A similar argument has been made by Salancik (1977).
He stated that assigned goals lead to goal commitment because listening
to the assignment without objection is in itself a form of consent. Moreover, assigning the goal implies that the recipient is capable of attaining it
which in turn increases the persons self-efficacy regarding the task.
It should not be concluded from the above that the persuasive requests
of authority figures compel commitment. Commitment is still a choice
process; it is often easy for the manager to obtain precisely because the
goal assignment is appraised as legitimate by the subordinate.
Peers can influence goal commitment by conveying normative information, by persuasion, and by serving as role models (Earley & Kanfer,
1985). In addition, they can generate competition.
Agreeing publicly to strive for a goal can also enhance commitment as
compared with agreeing to it only in private (Hollenbeck, Williams, &
Klein, 1989). Finally, rewards can affect goal commitment, but the manner in which these operate is not fully understood. It appears that large
rewards are generally more effective than small ones in this regard; but
rewards also interact with goal difficulty. Rewards offered for moderate
or easy goals appear to raise commitment to those goals but to lower
commitment to impossible goals, perhaps because people resent being
220
LATHAM
AND
LOCKE
SELF-REGULATION
THROUGH
GOAL
SETTING
221
perform well at a task (e.g., adaptability, creativity, resourcefulness, perceived capacity to orchestrate complex action sequences). Self-efficacy is
measured by asking subjects whether they believe thay can attain each of
a graded series of performance levels (self-efficacy magnitude) and by
asking them to rate their degree of confidence in attaining each level
(self-efficacy strength). It should be noted that some studies of expectancy theory have measured performance expectancy (E,) in a similar way
(Ilgen, Nebeker, & Pritchard, 1981).
It has been shown consistently that self-efficiency has powerful, direct
effects on performance (Bandura, 1986). This finding holds when goals
are manipulated as well. Thus both goals and self-efficacy have direct,
independent effects on performance. In addition to affecting performance
directly, self-efficacy can affect it indirectly by affecting personal goal
choice and commitment to assigned goals. Finally, assigning goals influences self-efficacy in that people who are assigned challenging goals are
more likely to have high self-efficacy than those who are assigned low
goals since assigning high goals is in itself an expression of confidence
(Salancik, 1977).
The above relationships are summarized in Fig. 3. We have added
ability to this figure, because it has been found that it has independent
effects on both self-efficacy and performance (Locke et al., 1984). A
recent series of studies by Earley and Lituchy (in press) showed considerable support for the model in Fig. 3. In two of the three studies, however, self-efficacy, while showing a significant first order r, did not add a
significant increment to the performance relationship beyond that provided by goals. In one of these studies (grade performance), the selfefficacy correlation was approximately the same magnitude as that found
by Wood and Locke (1987) who had used the same task. However, Wood
and Lockes self-efficacy scale was more elaborate and they used a larger
number of subjects. In their studies, the self-efficacy increment was significant. There is a possible explanation for the null result in the third
study. The task was complex and the self-efficacy measure was taken
after only two practice trials. A measure of self-efficacy may not be
meaningful this early in the learning process on such a task.
Ability
--+
Self-Efficacy
Performance
AsI.
Goal
FIG.
x
-
1
Personal Goal
222
LATHAM
AND
LOCKE
Keman and Lord (1990) claimed that expectancies do not affect performance on single goal tasks; however, they measured expectancy of
goal achievement rather than expectancy of attaining each of a number of
performance levels. Locke, Motowidlo, and Bobko (1986) have shown
that the former is a poor method of measuring expectancy, because the
referent for answering the item is different for each goal. Using the latter
(performance-anchored) method, the results are significant and quite consistent (Locke 8z Latham, 199Oa).
In contrast to self-efficacy and personal goal level, we have not found
subjective goal difficulty to be useful in predicting performance (e.g.,
Yukl & Latham, 1978). The reason, we believe, is that measures of subjective goal difficulty are confounded. On the one hand, they are positively associated with goal level (which would imply a positive association
with performance) and, on the other hand, they are negatively associated
with self-efficacy (which would imply a negative association with performance). To the degree that the two associations cancel one another out,
the net ability to predict performance is small (Locke & Latham, 199Oa).
Goals, Valences, and Instrumentalities
Garland (1985) reported a negative relationship between goal level and
valence measured as expected or anticipated satisfaction with attaining
each of a number of performance levels. This finding was replicated by
Klein (in press) and in a series of eight studies by Mento et al. (N90). The
explanation for this finding is that goals are at the same time targets to
shoot for and standards for evaluating ones performance (Bandura,
1986). This is shown graphically in Fig. 4. If one views ones goals as
minimally acceptable levels of performance (Locke & Bryan, 1%8), we
can see that a person with low goals will be satisfied with reaching a low
level of performance and thus even more satisfied with attaining more
than this minimal level. A person with high goals, on the other hand, will
be minimally satisfied only with reaching the high goal and thus will be
quite dissatisfied with reaching the low goal. The person with moderate
goals will be between the other two. Thus goals affect the calibration
of the satisfaction scale, raising it when the goals are high and lowering it
when the goals are low. Self-satisfaction, therefore, is harder to attain
when goals are hard than when they are easy.
It might be assumed from this that people, therefore, should set only
low goals in life because that would produce more satisfaction with less
effort. However, there is another set of factors involved in choosing a
goal, As noted above, in the real world, additional rewards typically come
to the person who sets and achieves high rather than low goals. Thus high
goals are more instrumental in gaining practical as well as psychological
benefits than are low goals. Mento et al. (1990), for example, asked MBA
SELF-REGULATION
Low
Medium
223
High
FIG. 4. Idealized
224
LATHAM
low
AND LOCKE
medium
Goal or Perlormence
high
Level
what is possible and those pertaining to what, among the total array of
possibilities, one wants.
Goals and Feedback
Few concepts in psychology have been written about more uncritically
and incorrectly than that of feedback. In organizational settings the aphorism what gets measured gets done describes cogently the positive
halo surrounding feedback. Actually, feedback is only information, that
is, data, and as such has no necessary consequences at all. Like any fact,
its effect on action depends on how it is appraised and what decisions are
subsequently made with respect to it. Studies of the effects of feedback
typically show positive effects (Kopelman, 1986), but this is because people often set improvement goals when given information about their past
performance. The only way to isolate the effects of feedback as such is to
give it in such a form that it cannot be used to set goals (e.g., vary the
length of each work period so that the subjects cannot directly compare
their performance from one trial to the next). When this is done, feedback
has no motivational effect on performance (Locke & Latham, 199Oa).
Even more intriguingly, a field experiment showed that even when engineers and scientists were urged to do their best, their subsequent performance was not significantly different from that of a control group. This
SELF-REGULATION
THROUGH
GOAL
225
SETTING
occurred despite the fact that they received the same amount of feedback
as those people who were in the specific goal conditions (Latham, Mitchell, & Dossett, 1978). Feedback that does not lead to the setting of and
commitment to specific difficult goals does not increase motivation to
increase ones performance. Figure 6 from Locke and Bryan (1969a) illustrates this point. Goal subjects in this study did have feedback about
their progress in relation to goals, but feedback subjects did not have
goals. Feedback alone did not affect performance. Thus with respect to
feedback as a motivator, goal setting is a mediator (cause) of its effects on
performance.
This relationship is easiest to envision when considering the case where
the individual receives multiple types of information. In such cases, the
individual cannot act, at a given time, on all of it and thus must select
which feedback elements to attend to and act upon. Goals single out for
attention one or more elements by providing a standard indicating
whether the feedback is good or bad; the elements with value
significance will be those accompanied by goals which serve as standards of evaluation (e.g., Nemeroff & Cosentino, 1979). In real-life, of
course, people are bombarded with information of every sort, but they act
only in response to a small segment of it, namely that segment which they
decide is relevant to their own life interests and goals.
On the other side of the same coin, goal setting is not very effective
without feedback (Erez, 1977). Thus feedback moderates the effect of
goals on performance.
Integrating the above results leads to the conclusion that goals and
124
2 1
Hard
GO.9
No KR
KR
Pw
Trial No.
2
3
Trial No.
226
LATHAM
AND
LOCKE
G&F>
G or F only
G&Fc
G or F only
17
20
37
1
2
3
o Based on Locke & Latham (MOa, Fig. 8-4). In this table contingent results are classified as failures rather than as half successes.
SELF-REGULATION
Low Self-efficacy
Low Goals
Satisfied
Mixed
Combinations
227
High Sell-efficacy
High Goals
Dissatisfied
There are at least three attributes of motivated action, namely direction, intensity, and duration. These are precisely the mediators or causal
mechanisms by which goals regulate performance. First, a goal directs
activity toward actions which are relevant to it at the expense of actions
which are not goal-relevant. In prose learning, for example, giving readers
learning objectives leads them to pay more attention to content which is
relevant to those objectives and less attention to the remainder (Rothkopf
& Billington, 1979). The same selective function is revealed in the multiple feedback situations discussed above in which goals single out from
an array of information those fed back scores to be acted upon (e.g.,
Locke & Bryan, 1%9b). Further, a specific goal can affect the manner in
which information is processed (Cohen & Ebbeson, 1979). Another aspect of the direction of action is the automatic arousal of previously
acquired skills which are perceived as relevant to goal accomplishment.
228
LATHAM
AND
LOCKE
SELF-REGULATION
THROUGH
GOAL
229
SETTING
Hollenbeck, 1990; Smith, Locke, & Barry, 1990) than is the case with do
best goals. Group goal setting may require planning for the explicit purpose of coordinating member activities so that the goal can be achieved.
When such planning is needed, goal setting is effective if coordinated
planning actually occurs (Larson & Schaumann, 1990).
(2) When a difficult, quantity goal is assigned, people may lower work
quality as an implicit strategy to attain it (Bavelas & Lee, 1978). Deemphasizing quality for quantity under quantity goals is most likely when
people are not highly confident of their task ability (Erez, 1990). To ensure performance quality, of course, goals must be set for quality.
(3) On complex tasks:
(a) Goals are more strongly related to performance when subjects
utilize suitable task strategies than when they do not. This is illustrated in
Table 2, based on Chesney and Locke (1991). Challenging goals increase
the likelihood that known strategies will be used (Earley, Lee, & Lituchy,
1989).
Time
period
1
2
3
.47**
.69**
.69**
.49**
.27*
-.22
* p < .05.
**p-c
.Ol.
230
LATHAM
AND
LOCKE
(b) When the task is heuristic and no strategy training is given (Huber, 1985; Earley, Connolly, & Ekegren, 1989);
(c) When there is pressure to perform well immediately and no
strategy training is given.
Wood and Locke (1990) have discussed the issue of goal and strategies
on complex tasks in some detail from a theoretical perspective (see also
Locke 8z Latham, 199Oa,Chap. 13).
Other Moderators
In addition to commitment, feedback, and task complexity, there are at
least two additional moderators of the goal-performance relationship.
The first is ability. Battle (1966) found that the goal-performance relationship is somewhat stronger among high than among low ability subjects, especially insofar as the goals are moderate to challenging (Battle,
1966). However, Kanfer and Ackerman (1989) found that goal effects can
be stronger for low than high ability subjects on complex tasks when
implemented after some initial learning has taken place. Motivation in the
absence of ability is unlikely to affect performance positively unless people are working below capacity. And obviously, ability limits the goalperformance relationship at very high (i.e., impossible) goal levels because such goals exceed the reach of virtually all people (Locke, 1982).
An intriguing study by Wood and Bandura (1989) showed that beliefs
regarding ones intellectual ability affect performance. In a management
simulation exercise, the people who viewed their ability as an acquirable
skill that could be enhanced through practice set challenging goals engaged in effective problem solving strategies, and subsequently attained
high performance. Those people who viewed intellectual ability as a more
or less fixed capacity viewed their errors as indicative of the fact that they
were indeed not intelligent. Consequently, they set low goals, their problem solving strategies deteriorated, and they subsequently performed
poorly.
The second additional moderator involves situational constraints. Peters, Chassie, Lindholm, OConnor, and Kline (1982) found that goal level
was significantly associated with performance when situational constraints were low rather than high. If the situation can be managed, of
course, high goals could motivate a person to overcome obstacles, especially if the person has high commitment and self-efficacy. Nevertheless
there are limits to a persons ability to change situations.
There is little evidence that factors such as race, age, education, gender, or tenure moderate the goal-performance relationship. Nor is the
evidence clear with respect to such factors as personality and culture.
However, only a limited number of studies have been conducted on these
latter variables (Locke & Latham, 1990a).
SELF-REGULATION
THROUGH
GOAL
231
SETTING
- .57*
* p < .05.
** p i .Ol.
Satisfaction
- .61*
.72**
232
LATHAM
AND
LOCKE
ued consequences (e.g., stress, failure, punishment, job insecurity, pressure, conflict). The typical field experiment result is no change in satisfaction (e.g., Latham et al., 1978) possibly because any positive consequences (e.g., role clarity, satisfaction with success) were offset by
negative consequences.
Correlational field studies typically show positive associations between
various positive attributes or concomitants of goal setting programs (e.g.,
clarity, participation, supervisory supportiveness, feedback, rewards for
goal attainment, communication) and satisfaction with the job or some
aspects of it. In contrast, various negative attributes of goals or goal
setting programs (e.g., stress, failure, overload, punishment, conflict) are
negatively associated with satisfaction (Lee, Bobko, Earley, & Locke, in
press). The reason for the clear-cut results of the correlational in contrast
to the experimental field studies is that these former studies distinguished
valued from disvalued attributes of goal programs. The results make it
clear that such programs can have very different affective consequences
depending on how they are implemented.
Since a major factor causing satisfaction with goal setting is goal success, a certain dilemma is posed for applied goal setting programs. Since
goal success is increasingly more frequent as goals become easier, it
means that the greatest degree of satisfaction is experienced when goals
are easy. On the other hand, it was noted earlier that the highest degree
of performance was attained when goals were difficult, that is, hard to
achieve. The dilemma, then, is how to balance the two outcomes. Since
satisfaction is based on both internal and external rewards which are also
typically based on success, an associated dilemma is how to reward performance under a goal setting program. If we maximize productivity, we
minimize satisfaction and rewards and vice versa. There are several possible solutions to this dilemma:
(1) Satisfice by setting moderate goals and rewarding success, so that
the net total of satisfaction and productivity is maximized.
(2) Give credit for partial goal attainment; that is, give credit and rewards for performance rather than for success as such.
(3) Follow the Japanese principle of Kuizen or constant improvement
(Imai, 1986). Make goals reachable at any given time, but strive for continual increments above this initial level by constantly raising the goals by
small amounts. (This does not necessarily imply working harder; it can
also be done by working smarter.)
(4) Use multilevel goal and reward structures, so that some reward is
provided for reaching a minimum goal, more is provided for reaching a
more challenging goal, and maximum reward is given for achieving
stretch goals.
Each of the above procedures has its pros and cons. There is no
SELF-REGULATION
233
right way to decide among them (and among additional, not-yetconceived structures) without further experimental study.
The High Performance
Cycle
The integrated goal setting model has been described elsewhere as the
high performance cycle (Locke & Latham, 199Oa, 199Ob, 1990~). The
model starts with high challenge in the form of specific, difftcult goals. If
there is commitment to these goals, adequate feedback, high self-efficacy
(and ability), and suitable task strategies, high performance will result. If
high performance leads to desired rewards (including self-rewards in the
form of self-satisfaction) high satisfaction will result. Job satisfaction is, in
turn, highly associated with commitment to the job (r = .64, based on 11
studies summarized in Locke & Latham, 199Oa), although the causal
relationship between these two variables is not definitively established.
High commitment in turn is associated with an increased propensity to
stay on the job (Mobley, 1982; Mowday, Porter, & Steers, 1982). People
who are satisfied and stay on the job are then ready and willing to accept
new challenges. Thus the cycle repeats itself. Deviations from the requirements of the cycle (e.g., low challenge, dissatisfaction) lead to a low
performance cycle.
SELF-REGULATION
reduction.
234
LATHAM
AND
LOCKE
SELF-REGULATION
THROUGH
GOAL
SETTING
235
236
LATHAM
AND
LOCKE
from the trainees regarding these skills, and clarified expectations for the
self-management of the training programs effectiveness.
Observe that the training took explicit account of goal setting moderators and facilitators. For example, commitment to goals was the focus of
Sessions 5 and 6 where rewards and punishers were selected, and a behavioral contract was written. Feedback through self-monitoring was emphasized in Session 4. The complexity of the task and the situational
constraints were the focus of Session 2 where the people explained why
they could not come to work, Session 6 where they specified in writing
the behavior that they believed would enable them to get to work, and
Session 7 where they outlined possibilities for a relapse and what could be
done to overcome such issues.
Participatory group discussions occurred throughout the 8 weeks of
training. The main benefit of participation, as noted earlier, is cognitive;
thus the training focused the attention of each person in the group on
problem solving effective strategies for overcoming obstacles to attaining
the goal. In this way self-efficacy was increased. Self-efficacy correlated
significantly in the study with subsequent job attendance.
With the goal setting programs in place, Frayne and Latham (1987)
found that 3 months later employee attendance was significantly higher in
the training than in the control group. Latham and Frayne (1989) conducted a 6-month and a 9-month follow-up study to determine the long
term effects of this training. Employees who had been trained in selfmanagement continued to have higher job attendance than those in the
control group. Moreover, when the people in the control group were
subsequently given the same training in self-management, but by a different trainer, they too showed the same positive improvement in their
self-efficacy with regard to coping with obstacles perceived by them as
preventing them from coming to work. Moreover, their job attendance
increased to the same level as that which the original training group had
achieved 3 months after it had been trained (Latham & Frayne, 1989).
The importance of skills in self-management is by no means restricted
to blue-collar employees. Frayne and Geringer (1990) investigated the
characteristics of general managers who are effective in international joint
ventures. These people differ from their counterparts in established corporate positions in that the latter typically receive training to prepare
them for their respective jobs. This training usually includes orientation
on appropriate lines of communication, existing company policies, the
political-legal environment, and the like. Such training seldom exists for
those people in the start-up phase of a joint venture. By definition the
venture is usually operating in an uncertain or little-known environment
due in part to the two or more parent firms having disparate objectives,
resources, and policies.
Frayne and Geringer found that leader skill in self-management, spe-
SELF-REGULATION
THROUGH
GOAL
SETTING
237
238
LATHAM
AND
LOCKE
First, Stock and Cervone found that the assignment of a proximal goal
increased the strength of the persons self-efficacy for completing the
task. Those people who were assigned a proximal goal increased the
initial strength of their self-efficacy for completing the task. People who
had been assigned a proximal goal in addition to the distal goal of task
completion had significantly higher initial ratings of self-efficacy than did
those people who only had a distal goal. Mentally breaking down the
task appeared to make it appear to be manageable which in turn enhanced
the persons perception that she was capable of performing it effectively.
Second, reaching the proximal goal enhanced self-efficacy. As people
attained the subgoal they became more confident of their capability to
complete the task. Those people who reached the same level of performance without knowing that they had achieved a proximal goal showed
no increase in their self-efficacy.
Third, the attainment of the proximal goal affected self-evaluative reactions positively. Those who achieved the proximal goal were more
satisfied with their progress than were those people who either did not
attain the subgoal or who had not been assigned one to attain.
Finally, those people with proximal goals persisted on the task significantly longer than did those people who had not been assigned them.
Stock and Cervone (1990) concluded that when individuals are uncertain
of their ability to perform a complex, challenging endeavor, setting proximal goals can influence positively self-referent thought, motivation, and
performance.
There are circumstances, however, where proximal goals may fail to
enhance performance. In long term programs of behavior change, where
the person has a high degree of interest in the activity, moderately distal
goals can allow greater flexibility in the use of tactics than proximal goals
(Kanfer & Grimm, 1978). The demanding standards represented by specific, proximal goals on such tasks can impair thinking and problem solving activities by diverting attention to non-task-related activities (Bandura
& Wood, 1989).
Using a complex computer simulation game over 10 weeks of business
activity, Cervone, Jiwani, and Wood (in press) investigated whether different goal structures affected the strength of relations between selfregulatory processes and performance. To optimize performance, the
subjects were required to learn a large number of nonlinear and compound rules which were difficult to master.
Consistent with goal setting theory, the assigned goals, which in this
study were distal, affected the subjects use of analytic strategies. Specifically, the people who were assigned a specific distal goal were more
systematic in their testing of analytic strategies for managing the simulated organization than were the people who were not assigned goals.
SELF-REGULATION
THROUGH
GOAL
SETTING
239
Again, consistent with goal setting theory, the higher goals led to higher
levels of performance than did the moderately difficult goals. In both goal
conditions, higher levels of self-efftcacy, self-satisfaction with past performance, and personal goals predicted higher levels of performance. In
contrast, Cervone ef al. found no evidence of a positive relation between
performance and either self-efftcacy or self-evaluative reactions within
the no-goal condition. When subjects were trying to do their best,
variations in performance feedback were unrelated to either selfevaluative reactions or self-efficacy judgments. The authors concluded
that explicit goals imposed a standard for performance that strongly affected self-reactions and their role in the self-regulation of task performance .
Seidman, Sevelius, and Ewald (1984) found that self-set (within a predetermined range) weight goals significantly affected the weight loss of
employees and their dependents in a program-contest conducted at six
sites of the Lockheed Co. Employees could enter either as individuals or
as members of a team. Team members, however, lost more weight than
non-team members suggesting that commitment to goals may have been
higher as a result of encouragement from others.
That assigned goals can sometimes be more effective than those that
are self-set by workers untrained in self-management techniques is evident from a study conducted in an electronics plant in Germany (Schmidt
& Kleinbeck, 1990). The German government mandates a relaxation allowance to permit employees to reduce fatigue or other kinds of physiological and psychological strain. In the case of self-paced work, this time
can be taken at the discretion of the individual worker in the form of
voluntary rest pauses. Schmidt and Kleinbeck found, however, that the
employees carried out their daily amount of work without any breaks so
that they could finish 1 h before the end of the official work day. The
result was a decrease in performance quality. Consequently, the employees were assigned, by computer, goals for each 30-min period of the work
day. Through the assignment of these proximal goals, the daily amount of
work was partitioned into clearly defined subgoals. These proximal goals
served as checkpoints by which the employee could assess how much
work had been accomplished in relation to the distal goal. The result was
a significant increase both in the use of rest breaks and in the quality of
performance. Performance quantity remained unchanged.
Leadership
240
LATHAM
AND
LOCKE
interaction with customers, work standard variance, and the like (Mills,
1983).
In our view, effective leaders first develop a vision for the organization
that galvanizes employees by providing them with a distal goal which
gives them a sense of purpose. The vision (Bennis & Nanus, 1985) inspires people by making clear to them that what they are doing is worthwhile. The danger in vision statements is that they can become rhetoric.
Thus effective leaders also set specific challenging proximal goals that
reflect and implement the vision. Proximal goals make the vision concrete
by providing benchmarks for coordinating and guiding action. A third
characteristic of effective leadership is modeling behavior for others on
problem-solving and decision-making and taking action steps for attaining
the proximal goals that are necessary for achieving the distal goal. Fourth,
effective leaders are accessible to employees to listen to their ideas and
concerns regarding both distal and proximal goals. Through their accessibility they stimulate people to formulate strategies to achieve these
goals. Fifth, effective leaders know that what gets measured gets done if
goals are set in conjunction with the feedback. The act of measurement
signals to employees the goals that are truly valued by the organization.
It thus strengthens (regulates) goal commitment among those people who
value membership in the organization.
Manz and Sims (1989) argued that leadership from above should
evolve, within the constraints of the organizational vision, into what they
call superleadership. This involves upper management teaching employees to lead themselves through such mechanisms as self-set goals,
self-monitoring, and self-administering rewards and punishment.
Our conclusions about goal setting and self-direction are these: although people are natural self-regulators in that goal-directedness is inherent in the life process, they are not innately effective self-regulators.
Skill in self-regulation must be acquired through experience, training, and
effort. We can add to this, based on Binswanger (1991), that the benefits
of experience and training will depend on the degree to which people
engage in volitionally initiated thought processes.
REFERENCES
Andrews, F. D., & Farris, G. F. (1972). Time pressure and performance of scientists and
engineers: A five-year panel study. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance,
8, 185-200.
Atkinson, J. W. (1958). Towards experimental analysis of human motivation in terms of
motives, expectancies, and incentives. In J. W. Atkinson (Ed.), Motives in fantasy,
action and society. Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.
Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. American Psychologist, 37,
122-147.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social-cognitive theory.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
SELF-REGULATION
241
4, 634642.
Bavelas, J., & Lee, E. S. (1978). Effect of goal level on performance: A trade-off of quantity
and quality. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 32, 219-240.
Bennis, W., & Nanus, 9. (1985). Leaders. New York: Harper & Row.
Binswanger, H. (1990). The biological basis of teleological concepts. Los Angeles: Ayn
Rand Institute.
Binswanger, H. (1991). Volition as cognitive self-regulation. Organizational Behavior and
Human Decision Processes, SO, 154-178.
Brickner, M. A., & Bukatko, P. A. (1987). Locked into perfomance: Goal setting as a
moderator of the social loafing effect. Unpublished manuscript, University of Akron.
Brief, A. P., 8t Hollenbeck, J. R. (1985). An exploratory study of self-regulating activities
and their effects on job performance. Journal of Occupational Behavior, 6, 197-208.
Bryan, J. F., & Locke, E. A. (1%7). Parkinsons law as a goal-setting phenomenon. Organizational
Behavior
2, 258-275.
Chesney, A. A., & Locke, E. A. (1991). Relationships among goal difficulty, business strategies, and performance on a complex simulation task. Academy of Managemenr Journal, 34, 400424.
Cohen, C. E., & Ebbesen, E. 9. (1979). Observational goals and schema activation: A
theoretical framework for behavior perception. Journal of Experimenral Social Psychology, 15, 305-329.
Dember, W. N. (1975). Motivation and the cognitive revolution. American Psychologist, 30,
161-168.
Earley, W. N. (1986a). An examination of the mechanisms underlying the relation of feedback to performance. Academy of Management Proceedings, 214-218.
Earley, P. C. (1986b). Supervisors and shop stewards as sources of contextual information
in goal setting: A comparison of U.S. with England. Journal of Applied Psychology, 71,
111-117.
242
LATHAM
AND LOCKE
Earley, P. C., Connolly, T., & Ekegren, G. (1989). Goals, strategy development and task
performance: Some limits on the efficacy of goal setting. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 24-33.
Earley, P. C., & Erez, M. (1990). Time dependency effects of goals and norms: The role of
cognitive processing on motivational models. Unpublished manuscript, Department of
Strategic Management & Organization, University of Minnesota.
Earley, P. C., & Kanfer, R. (1985). The influence of component participation and role
models on goal acceptance, goal satisfaction and performance. Organizational Behavior
and Human Decision Processes, 36, 378-398.
Earley, P. C., Lee, C., & Lituchy, T. R. (1989). Task strategies and judgements in goal
setting: The effects of a learning emphasis and training sequence on performance.
Unpublished manuscript, Management and Policy, University of Arizona.
Earley, P. C., & Lituchy, T. R. (in press). Delineating goal and efficacy effects: A test of
three models. Journal of Applied Psychology.
Earley, C. P., Shalley, C. E., & Northcraft, G. B. (in press). I think I can, I think I can. . . .
Processing time and strategy effects of goal acceptance/rejection decisions. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes.
Earley, P. C., Wojnaroski, P., & Prest, W. (1987). Task planning and energy expended:
Exploration of how goals influence performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 72,
107-l 14.
Erez, M. (1977). Feedback: A necessary condition for the goal setting-performance relationship. Journal of Applied Psychology, 62, 624-627.
Erez, M. (1990). Performance quality and work motivation. In U. Kleinbeck, H. Quast, H.
Thierry, & H. Hacker (Eds.), Work motivation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Erez, M., & Zidon, I. (1984). Effect of goal acceptance on the relationship of goal difficulty
to performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 69, 69-78.
Frayne, C.A., & Geringer, M. (1990). Self-management practices and performance of international joint venture general managers. Paper presented at the annual meeting of
the Academy of Management, San Francisco.
Frayne, C. A., & Latham, G. P. (1987). Application of social learning theory to employee
self-management of attendance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 72, 387-392.
Garland, H. (1985). A cognitive mediation theory of task goals and human performance.
Motivation and Emotion, 9, 345-367.
Gist, M. E., Bavetta, A. G., & Stevens, C. K. (1990). Transfer training method: Its influence on skill generalization, skill repetition, and performance level. Personnel Psychology, 43, 501-523.
Gollwitzer, P. M., Heckhausen, H., & Ratajczak, K. (1990). From weighing to willing:
Approaching a change decision through prior or postdecisional mentation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 45, 4145.
Hall, D. T., & Foster, L. W. (1977). A psychological success cycle and goal setting: Goals,
performance, and attitudes. Academy of Management Journal, 20, 282-290.
Hollenbeck, F. R., Williams, C. R., & Klein, H. J. (1989). An empirical examination of the
antecedents of commitment to ditEcult goals. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 18-23.
Huber, V. L. (1985). Effects of task difficulty, goal setting, and strategy on performance of
a heuristic task. Journal of Applied Psychology, 70, 492-504.
Huber, V. L., & Neale, M. A. (1987). Effects of self- and competitor goals on performance
in an interdependent bargaining task. Journal of Applied Psychology, 72, 197-203.
Iigen, D., Nebeker, D., & Pritchard, R. (1981). Expectancy theory measures: An empirical
comparison in an experimental simulation. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 28, 189-223.
SELF-REGULATION
THROUGH
GOAL
SETTING
243
74, 657-690.
Keman, M. G., & Lord, R. G. (1989). The effects of explicit goals and specific feedback on
escalation processes. Journal of Aplied Social Psychology, 19, 1125-l 143.
Keman, M. C., & Lord, R. C. (1990). Effects of valence, expectancies, and goal performance discrepancies in single and multiple goal environments. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75, 194-203.
Klein, H. .I. (in press). Further evidence on the relationship between goal setting and expectancy theories. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes.
Kopelman, R. E. (1986). Objective feedback. In E. A. Locke (Ed.), Generalizing from
laboratory to field settings. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.
LaPorte, R. E., & Nath, R. (1976). Role of performance goals in prose learning. Journal of
Educational
Psychology,
68, 260-264.
Larson, J. R., & Schaumann, L. J. (1990). Group goals, group coordination, and group
member motivation. Unpublished manuscript, Department of Psychology, University
of Illinois at Chicago.
Latham, G. P., & Baldes, J. J. (1975). The practical significance of Lockes theory of
goal setting. Journal of Applied Psychology, 60, 122-124.
Latham, G. P., Erez, M., & Locke, E. A. (1988). Resolving scientific disputes by the joint
design of crucial experiments by the antagonists: Application to the Erez-Latham dispute regarding participation in goal setting. [Monograph]. Journal of Applied Psychology, 73, 753-772.
Latham, G. P., & Frayne, C. A. (1989). Self-management training for increasing job attendance: A follow-up and a replication. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 41 l-416.
Latham, G. P., & Kinne, S. B. (1974). Improving job performance through training in goal
setting. Journal of Applied Psychology, 59, 187-191.
Latham, G. P., & Lee, T. W. (1986). Goal setting. In E. A. Locke (Ed.), Generalizing from
laboratory toj7eld settings. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.
Latham, G. P., & Locke, E. A. (1975). Increasing productivity with decreasing time limits:
A field replication of Parkinsons law. Journal of Applied Psychology, 60, 524-526.
Latham, G. P., Mitchell, T. R., & Dossett, L. L. (1978). Importance of participative goal
setting and anticipated rewards on goal difficulty and job performance. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 63, 163-171.
244
LATHAM
AND
LOCKE
Latham, Cl. P., & Saari, L. M. (1982). The importance of union acceptance for productivity
improvement through goal setting. Personnel Psychology, 35, 781-787.
Latham, G. P., Winters, D. C., & Locke, E. A. (1991). Cognitive and motivational mediators ofthe effects of participation on performance. Unpublished manuscript, University of Toronto.
Latham, G P., & Yukl, G. A. (1975). A review of research on the application of goal setting
in organizations. Academy of Management Journal, 18, 824-845.
Lee, C., Bobko, P., Earley, P. C., & Locke, E. A. (in press). An empirical analysis of a goal
setting questionnaire. Journal of Organizational Behavior.
Likert, R. (1967). The human organization. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Locke, E. A. (1968). Toward a theory of task motivation and incentives. Organizational
Behavior and Human Performance, 3, 157-189.
Locke, E. A. (1969). Purpose without consciousness: A contradiction. Psychological Reports, 25, 991-1009.
Locke, E. A. (1976). The nature and causes of job satisfaction. In M. Dunnette (Ed.),
Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology. Chicago: Rand McNally.
Locke, E. A. (1982). Relation of goal level to performance with a shortwork period and
multiple goal levels. Journal of Applied Psychology, 67, 512-514.
Locke, E. A. (in press). Goal theory vs. control theory: Contrasting approaches to understanding work motivation. Motivation and Emotion.
Locke, E. A., & Bryan, J. F. (1%7). Performance goals as determinants of level of performance and boredom. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51, 120-130.
Locke, E. A., & Bryan, J. F. (1%8). Grade goals as determinants of academic achievement.
Journal of General Psychology, 79, 217-228.
Locke, E. A., & Bryan, J. F. (1969a). Knowledge of score and goal level as determinants of
work rate. Journal of Applied Psychology, 53, 59-65.
Locke, E. A., & Bryan, J. F. (1969b). The directing function of goals in task performance.
Organizational Behavior and Human Peflormance, 4, 35-42.
Locke, E. A., Cartledge, N., L Knerr, C. (1970). Studies of the relationship between satisfaction, goal setting and performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Per$ormance, 5, 135-158.
Locke, E. A., Chah, D. O., Harrison, D. S., L Lustgarten, N. (1989). Separating the effects
of goal specificity from goal level. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 43, 270-287.
Locke, E. A., Frederick, E., Buckner, E., & Bobko, P. (1984). Effect of previously assigned goals on self-set goals and performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 69,
694-699.
Locke, E. A., Frederick, E., Lee, C., & Bobko, P. (1984). Effect of self-efficacy, goals, and
task strategies on task performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 69, 241-251.
Locke, E. A., L Henne, D. (1986). Work motivation theories. In C. Cooper & I. Robertson
(Eds.), International review of industrial and organizational psychology. Chichester,
England: Wiley.
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1984). Goal setting: A motivational technique that works!
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (199Oa).A theory of goal setting and task performance.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: PrenticeHall.
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (199Ob).Work motivation and satisfaction: Light at the end
of the tunnel. Psychological Science, 4, 240-246.
Locke, E. A., & Latharn, G. P. (199Oc).Work motivation: The high performance cycle. In
U. Kleinbeck, H. Quast, H. Thierry, & H. Hacker (Eds.), Work motivation. Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
SELF-REGULATION
245
Locke, E. A., Latham, G. P., & Erez, M. (1988). The determinants of goal commitment.
Academy of Management Review, 13, 23-39.
Locke, E. A., Motowidlo, S. J., & Bobko, P. (1986). Using self-efficacy theory to resolve
the conflict between goal-setting theory and expectancy theory in organizational behavior and industrial/organizational psychology. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology,
4, 328-338.
Locke, E. A., Shaw, K. M., Saari, L. M., & Latham, G. P. (1981). Goal setting and task
performance: 1969-1980. Psychological BulLetin, 90, 125-152.
Mahoney, M. J., Moura, N. G., & Wade, T. C. (1973). The relative efficacy of self-reward,
self-punishment, and self-monitoring techniques for weight loss. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical
Psychology,
40, 404-407.
Manz, C., & Sims, H. (1989). Super-leadership. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Matsui, T., Okada, A., & Inoshita, 0. (1983). Mechanism of feedback affecting task performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 13, 114-122.
Mento, A. J., Locke, E. A., & Klein, H. J. (1990). The relation ofgoal level to valence and
instrumentality.
Unpublished manuscript, College of Business, Loyola College, Baltimore.
Mento, A. J., Steel, R. P., & Karren, R. J. (1987). A meta-analytic study of the effects of
goal setting on task perfromance: 19661984. Organizational
Behavior and Human
Decision
Processes,
39, 52-83.
Meyer, J. P., & Gellatly, I. R. (1988). Perceived performance norm as a mediator in the
effect of assigned goal on personal goal and task performance. Journal of Applied
Psychology,
13, 410-420.
66, 598-603.
of anchoring
and strategy-development
Unpub-
Peters, L. H., Chassie, M. B., Lindholm, H. R., OConnor, E. J., & Kline, C. R. (1982).
The joint influence of situational constraints and goal setting on performance and affective outcomes. Journal of Management, 8, 7-20.
Podsakoff, P. M., & Farh, J, (1989). Effects of feedback sign and credibility on godi setting
246
LATHAM
AND LOCKE
SELF-REGULATION
247