Asp 1295
Asp 1295
Asp 1295
la revue du GERAS
39-40 | 2003
Varia
Electronic version
URL: http://journals.openedition.org/asp/1295
DOI: 10.4000/asp.1295
ISBN: 978-2-8218-0392-3
ISSN: 2108-6354
Publisher
Groupe d'étude et de recherche en anglais de spécialité
Printed version
Date of publication: 1 May 2003
Number of pages: 59-72
ISSN: 1246-8185
Electronic reference
Shirley Carter-Thomas and Elizabeth Rowley-Jolivet, « Analysing the scientific conference
presentation (CP), A methodological overview of a multimodal genre », ASp [Online], 39-40 | 2003,
Online since 10 May 2010, connection on 19 April 2019. URL : http://journals.openedition.org/
asp/1295 ; DOI : 10.4000/asp.1295
Introduction
1 In ESP, knowledge of written academic genres is now well advanced. Numerous ESP
analysts have devoted extensive study to the functioning and organisation of written
scientific texts, of which the research article (RA), is the most obvious example (Swales
1990; Dudley-Evans 1994; Candlin & Hyland 1999; Hyland 2000, Trosborg 2000). Our
knowledge of spoken academic genres is however much less complete and much remains
to be done in terms of generic characterisation, both in distinguishing different types of
spoken academic communication (seminars, conference presentations, keynote
addresses, didactic lectures) and in characterising these across disciplines.
2 Among the various spoken genres that the academic researcher needs to master, a key
one is the conference presentation genre (CP). Conference presentations play an essential
role in the life of research communities (Rowley-Jolivet 1999), but due to this relative lack
of interest in spoken academic discourse, specific work on this important genre is only
just beginning to appear (Ventola et al. 2002; Simpson & Swales 2001; Swales, in press). If
ESP researchers have until recently concentrated on written academic genres at the
expense of spoken ones is it however because of the relative difficulties of acquiring
spoken corpora or is it because these genres are less easily apprehended? Can the
conference presentation be analysed and deconstructed in the same way as a written
scientific text?
3 In this article we will consider three different approaches that can contribute towards
characterising the CP in science. We will consider the advantages and limitations of the
three approaches examined, and highlight some of the methodological issues involved.
The first approach considered is that of a more microscopic bottom-up analysis, focusing
primarily on the recurrent syntactic features of the CP. Such an approach provides us
with useful information on the relation between generic features, communicative context
and language choices at a local level; it does not however enable us to gain an insight into
the higher level structural organisation of the conference presentation. In a second
approach we therefore adopt a more top-down macroscopic view, examining to what
extent move analysis can be used to explore the rhetorical structure of the CP. We
present a move analysis model which we feel can be usefully applied to analysing CP
introductions. This approach though, in the same way as the bottom-up syntactic
analysis, places the emphasis on the verbal realisation of the textual product and does not
take into account the multimodal nature of the scientific conference presentation. To
capture the generic features of the conference presentation it would seem necessary to
consider all the semiotic resources used, and not only language. In a third stage we
therefore investigate the methodological issues involved in analysing the visual channel
of communication, an essential but until now little studied aspect of this multimodal
genre.
4 The spoken data used for the study comprise 44 15-20 minute presentations given at
international scientific conferences in three fields (geology, medicine, and physics) by
native speakers. The presentations were recorded on video and then transcribed. A
smaller comparative corpus composed of 9 of the physics presentations and the 9
corresponding articles published in the book of proceedings of the physics conference
(same authors, topics, and event) was also collected, to enable comparison between the
presentations and proceedings articles (PA)1.
implications for production and comprehension. The complex noun groups and
nominalisations, characteristic of the RA or corresponding PA are ill suited for this reason in
the CP. Time is also a limiting factor. In a ten or fifteen minute presentation the content
transmitted will be far less than in a corresponding article.
2. The purpose of scientific research communication, whether spoken or written, is not only
informative but also rhetorical in that it aims to persuade. The relationship with audience or
readership is however very different in the two cases. In the article the authors will usually
adopt a detached stance, impressing upon the readers their prudence and objectivity so as
not to limit the scientific validity of the propositions. In the CP, on the contrary, as the
audience is physically present in the auditorium, it is necessary to set up a feeling of
connivance. Too detached a monologue would be ill-suited to the communicative context
and appropriate interpersonal strategies are important.
3. The semiotic modes employed are also different in the two cases. The CP is an audio-visual
presentation and makes use not only of linguistic semiotic means, but also mathematical
and visual semiotics. While the main channel of communication is arguably the spoken one,
the importance of the visual channel should not be underestimated. In scientific conferences
a large number of visuals (slides, transparencies or Powerpoint displays) accompany the
speaker’s discourse, and audiences are expected to listen and read simultaneously. One of
the tasks of the speaker is to integrate this visual information into the verbal commentary.
7 All of the above features can be expected to have an impact on the language choices
visible in the textual product. In order to examine these choices we will first consider a
bottom up microscopic approach, examining certain discourse surface features with
regard to their textual functions.
combination frequent in the article would be extremely difficult to produce and process
in the live CP environment. The following example is an illustration:
The coupling impedance between the slow space charge wave on the
electron beam and the surface harmonics of the backward TMO1 can be
modified (A14)2
11 Moreover, the traditional impersonality and distance afforded by the passive are not as
relevant in an oral presentation, as speakers adopt a more direct approach when
addressing a live audience. The low proportion of passive constructions was seen to be
correlated with a correspondingly high proportion of personal pronoun (animate)
syntactic subjects followed by an active verb, with speakers tending to express actions
and opinions congruently and taking personal responsibility for their decisions and
interpretations.
12 Considerations relating to author responsibility can also explain why structures involving
extraposition are less frequently found in the CP. In their writing scientists often use
extraposition as a hedging tactic, as it provides a grammatical framework for expressing
stance or evaluation while at the same time, like the passive, respecting the impersonal
tone commonly found in research articles. In the CP, however, as speakers express their
evaluation and comments far more openly, the recourse to hedging is less necessary, as
the following parallel examples illustrate:
Article Presentation
13 The speaker’s choice to orient such clauses as these around personal themes in the CP
makes the presentation much livelier and creates overall very different interpersonal
relations to those of the article.
14 The conference presentation environment also requires the use of certain syntactic
patterns which are little used in scientific writing. Existential constructions, for example,
were found to have an important discourse organising role in the CP, serving frequently
as enumeration or segmentation devices, a role fulfilled to a great extent by
typographical conventions in the article. The greater frequency of structures involving
inversion in the CP was also seen to be related to the specific communicative context.
Inversion was a favoured strategy employed by conference speakers for integrating the
visual and verbal channels of communication. Using inversion enables the speaker to
draw the audience's attention to a visual display, before commenting verbally on it, thus
guiding the audience's interpretation of this visual information: On this axis is electric field
(P2).
15 Pseudo-clefts, which practically never occur in the scientific article, were also found to
play a crucial role in the CP. They serve to generally slow down the discourse flow,
segmenting the discourse into manageable chunks and signalling the salience of
particular items of information. The regular pseudo-cleft (or Wh-cleft) also enables
presenters, through a questioning process implied by the Wh element, to highlight the
originality or novelty of their research in a suitably interactive way:
What this work has shown is that you can describe the FEL with just two parameters
(P12)
What we decided to do was to try to generate a glow-discharge plasma (P8)
16 The use of the reverse RWH-cleft variant of the pseudo-cleft, invariably containing the
demonstrative this or that in theme position in the CP corpus, would also appear to be
motivated by certain characteristics of the discourse environment. With that the
structure not only fulfils a framing function, but also sums up a segment of discourse:
17 With this, the structure has a more cataphoric role, pointing forward to a new element of
visual information displayed on a slide.
3. Rhetorical approach
3.1. Issues involved
20 A more macroscopic approach to genre analysis, and one that has been closely associated
with genre analysis in ESP since the 1980s, is the rhetorical approach, based on the
identification of schemata or recurrent moves. The principal aim of a move analysis
model is to gain insight into the communicative purpose of a discourse genre and into its
specific rhetorical structure as it moves towards this purpose. Move analysis in ESP has
however to the best of our knowledge been applied almost exclusively to written
discourse, with innumerable studies of moves in various written academic genres in the
wake of John Swales’ initial 4-move model (1981) and the revised (1990) Create A Research
Space (or CARS) model of the introductions in research articles (e.g. Hopkins 1985; Peng
1987; Dudley-Evans 1994; Brett 1994; Nwogu 1997; Samraj 2002). From a methodological
viewpoint it was thus important to establish whether move analysis could indeed be
usefully applied to a spoken academic genre. Would it be possible to attribute a clear
rhetorical movement to the CP in the same way as has been done for the RA? Would
existing move models initially developed for written discourse prove to be applicable?
21 We decided to concentrate on the introduction sections to the CP, firstly so as to enable
comparison with the Swalesian CARS model and secondly because of the undeniably
important rhetorical role of introductions in positioning academic research. An analysis
was therefore undertaken of the Introductions to 44 conference presentations in 3
scientific fields.
22 A major methodological problem was however encountered right at the outset: how can
the cut-off points of even the principal sections of the CP be identified – introduction,
methods, discussion, etc.? These are clearly materialised in the RA, but not in a spoken
monologue. The introduction is probably the easiest section to tackle as the beginning is
easily identified. Deciding where an introduction finishes however is less apparent, as
speakers very rarely use structuring metadiscourse markers to signal the transition
(Thompson 2003). We therefore made use of a variety of clues: the content itself, various
frame markers such as Well, So and structures such as pseudo-clefts (So what we decided to
do was…). We also studied research articles in the same speciality and consulted specialist
informants when necessary. Another extremely important clue was the visuals shown
during the presentation, as these, either by their titles or by a shift in the type of visual,
often clearly indicate the beginning of a new section. A dual-rating system was also used
throughout the whole process until agreement between the two analysts was reached.
23 Once the 44 CP introductions had been satisfactorily delimited, the question remained as
to whether existing models of move structure would be applicable to their analysis. The
RA introductions analysed by Swales (1990), in that they were also a research process
genre, produced by scientific researchers for their peers on very similar topics to those of
our corpus, could be presumed to share many features of the CP introductions. However,
although the general rhetorical movement seemed very similar, the communicative
context of the CP, and the fact of it being spoken discourse led to difficulties in applying
the complete CARS model. There were several moves encountered in the CP which were
not specifically catered for in this written model, whilst other RA moves, such as that
relating to the literature review, were far less elaborate. The order of moves posited by
Swales also did not seem to reflect that found in the CP.
24 As guidance therefore to analysing an oral genre, two other models3 of introductions in
spoken academic genres were also consulted: Dubois’ analysis of genre and structure in
biomedical conference presentations (1980) and Thompson’s analysis of introductions in
university lectures (1994). These two models did indeed prove insightful for the analysis
of our own oral data. The division established by Dubois between content orientation and
listener orientation, provided a useful starting point for the dissection of the CP data.
Likewise several categories identified by Thompson, such as those related to the
metatextual function of ‘Setting up the lecture framework’ are also highly relevant to the
CP, as was the more flexible conception of move ordering. However, as Thompson’s
lecture data is didactic discourse, rather than that of research, with the audience being
composed of novices and not peers, the communicative purpose is not the same. The
‘funnel effect’ or rhetorical narrowing characteristic of introductions in research process
genres is absent. Dubois’ analysis, being based on research presentations, could be
expected to provide the greatest similarity with our own data. However Dubois’ analysis
is made within a tagmemic typology of narrative, and as a result is less applicable to the
CP genre overall than the more purposeful rhetorical move analysis proposed by Swales.
The fact that Dubois’ analysis was based on a single scientific discipline, biomedicine, also
meant that certain categories were too discipline-specific for a generic approach.
25 As none of the three models of article introductions analysed seemed entirely
appropriate to our mixed-disciplinary CP corpus, we therefore decided to carry out a
separate move analysis on our data. For all the move categories we took into account not
only the speaker’s monologue but also the information displayed on the visuals. The
model proposed can be seen in figure 1.
26 Move A, Setting up the framework, is where the divergences with the CARS model, designed
for written academic discourse, are most apparent. The two main categories posited here,
Interpersonal framework and Discourse framework, are very much motivated by the
specific enunciative context of the CP: the presence of a live audience generates the need
for a direct contact between the speaker and the audience, and cognitive constraints on
the real-time processing of information also make it useful to give the audience explicit
signalling of the topic and organisation of the talk. The Listener orientation step includes
all remarks addressed by the speaker to the chairman, to the audience or the conference
organisers, thanking them, greeting them and generally making contact.
Acknowledgements, a category not present in the three models consulted, are placed at the
end of the RA and PA, but occur at the beginning of the CP and include
acknowledgements to collaborators, companies or funding agencies. As research in
science is almost exclusively a collaborative effort, this is acknowledged by the individual
speaker and can be seen as a politeness strategy. In the first of the two text-oriented
components, the speaker Announces the topic of the presentation, either verbally and/or
visually, foregrounding the particular presentation within the conference context. The
Structure or scope step gives the audience some indication of how the talk is structured,
and which aspects of the topic will be covered, to what degree of depth. The Outline
Structure step also occurs within Swales’ Move 3 (Occupy niche), but it seemed helpful to
separate the textual moves from the more content-related moves, which are mixed
together in the CARS model.
27 Once the framework (Move A) has been put into place, the two remaining moves
identified for the CP are purely content oriented or ideational and broadly reflect Swales’
rhetorical CARS structure. Within Move B, Contextualising the Topic, we have however also
included a Conference context step. This includes references to other talks given at the
conference concerned and generally precedes the General research context. The latter sub-
move in fact conflates ‘topic generalisations’ and ‘reviewing research’ as identified in the
CARS model, since distinguishing them in the CP context seemed particularly
problematic. There would, moreover, seem to be no full-blown literature review such as
found in research articles. Verbal references to published research in the CP are scarce,
although there are a few scattered literature references on the visuals. This undoubtedly
reflects the different epistemological function of the CP genre. What conference
participants come to hear are results, and the novelty or news value has to be high;
consequently the amount of time devoted to shared background knowledge is restricted.
28 Move C, Research rationale, the final move identified for the CP, collapses the Swalesian
moves of Establish Niche and Occupy niche into one. In the more applied fields analysed
within our corpus, in physics in particular but also to some extent in geology and
medicine, scientists seem less concerned with carving out their particular research niche
than with simply explaining the motivations or origins of the research project. Within the
category of Motivation, we have therefore included the sub-move of Relevance/centrality/
need, as by pointing out the importance of a research project, the scientist can also be
seen to provide an explanation for why the research was undertaken. Other motivations
for the research include the categories of Continuation, and that of signalling Gaps/
problems and Counter-claims, categories also found within the CARS model. In several cases
in our corpus, the problems or gaps evoked are followed by a Response, in the form of
either Question raising or a Research hypothesis and even Previews of results or solutions.
The culmination of the final move is often the description of the specific Research goal at
the end of the introduction.
29 The linear representation presented in the above move model for the CP should not be
considered as rigid. Positing a strict sequence of moves even for a genre as strictly
codified as the RA can be problematic (cf. Samraj 2002). There is considerable flexibility
due not only to disciplinary variation and the specific objectives of the research, but also
because genres themselves are flexible and adapt in response to the evolving needs and
practices of discourse communities (Askehave & Swales 2001; Anthony 1999). This
flexibility is even more apparent in presentations due to the real-time and sometimes
impromptu decisions of the speaker, and because the rules are far less codified. The
position of Outline structure/indicate scope would, for example, appear to be fairly flexible.
References to the General research context can also occur not only within Move B, but also
later within Move C, to reinforce the motivation for the research. Nor are the categories
identified mutually exclusive. Some of the talks analysed contained the majority whilst
others only very few. In a small number of talks we also found a certain amount of
‘spillage’, with elements of the Materials and Methods sections occurring within the
Introductions. Overall however the linear sequence presented does reflect the moves in
the majority of the CP analysed, and our initial findings would seem to indicate that the
CP introduction, like the RA, has a clear rhetorical movement.
30 In order to verify these preliminary results it will be necessary to further refine the
analysis proposed and also to consider the question of obligatory and optional moves
within the CP. Our analysis, however, has shown that despite the complexity of the task, a
rhetorical approach using move analysis can indeed be usefully applied to the CP. By
laying the emphasis on communicative or rhetorical purpose, and on schematic
structure, move analysis goes a long way towards teasing out some important generic
features of the discourse. The fact that the conference presentation is not strictly codified
in the same way as the RA means that giving a conference presentation can often be a
considerable hurdle for novice and NNS presenters. As Shalom (2002) has pointed out, the
introductions to the CP in particular often pose a serious problem for novices. The
comparison with the structural organisation commonly found in the RA introductions has
enabled us to pinpoint some striking differences between the two genres, and it is
apparent that the written article cannot provide an adequate model. The importance of
the interpersonal framework at the outset of the CP is a case in point and can
undoubtedly provide a starting point for further pedagogical applications.
31 However, if move analysis of spoken academic genres has potential, it also has
limitations. The move categories themselves are very slippery concepts. There is no one-
to-one correspondence between linguistic signals and moves and sub-moves in the CP and
applying the categories coherently therefore requires an efficient rating system and the
participation of at minimum two raters. As already noted, these methodological
difficulties are also compounded by the inherent flexibility of spoken discourse genres.
More importantly, however, to the best of our knowledge, move analysis so far has not
taken the multimodality of the discourse into account, and has been based solely on the
linguistic realisation of the textual product. As became obvious, however when
undertaking both the syntactic analysis and the move analysis, the multimodal dimension
of the conference presentation cannot be ignored. The functions and frequencies of some
syntactic structures within the CP, such as inversion for example, can only be
satisfactorily explained by the need to manage a multimodal text and to integrate
information from different semiotics. Likewise some moves and section boundaries are
signalled only via the visual channel, while other verbal information can only be correctly
interpreted by reference to the visuals. To capture the defining features of a multimodal
genre, such as the CP, an analysis of the linguistic realisation can at best provide only a
partial view. The third approach considered will thus tackle some of the issues involved
in a multimodal analysis of the CP.
4. Multimodal approach
4.1. Semiotic Modes
35 Each of the four main semiotic modes outlined above has certain functional specialisms
(Kress et al, 2001). Natural language, for example, whether speech or writing, is
appropriate for the expression of time relations, whereas the visual mode is better
adapted for expressing spatial relations. Language is more suited for evaluating, whereas
visual communication in science is considerably more efficient at showing patterns,
trends or co-variation of several parameters (Larkin & Simon 1987; Krohn 1991; Lemke
1998). Gesture is useful for locating items in the communicative context, or for animating
a static visual. The importance of the visual mode, and the use of gesture, mean therefore
that in the CP these different communicative functions can be distributed among the
three modes. The conference speaker, unlike the RA writer, need not rely solely, or
mainly, on language for certain functions.
36 Other factors that need to be taken into account in a multimodal approach to a genre are
indicated in the inner circle of Figure 2. The degree of articulation of a specific mode, for
example, will vary as a function of the communicative needs of a specific discourse
community. In the community of deaf and dumb people gesture has developed into a
highly articulated and grammaticalised mode. In scientific discourse communities, on the
other hand, the visual mode is highly articulated, with different specialities having their
own visual language. Audiences at scientific conferences have a very well-developed
visual literacy, which enables them to ‘read’ the visual language of their specialism
quickly and efficiently. Another factor to be taken into account as a specific genre unfolds
is the relative foregrounding or backgrounding of the modes involved within the
particular discourse. The interplay between the modes is not fixed but may shift in
relative salience as the discourse progresses. In the RA and to some extent the CP, the
linguistic mode is dominant during the introduction, whereas in the results section, the
visual mode is likely to gain more salience. Tracking such shifts as these is important for
understanding how the complete discourse functions. In the case of the CP the dominance
of the linguistic mode during the introduction can undoubtedly partly explain why novice
researchers or NNS find this part of the presentation so problematic.
37 The precise material conditions of production and reception of a given genre also give
rise to certain constraints and affordances or potentials which have a direct effect on the
semiotic modes employed. In the case of the CP, both the constraints and affordances of
the genre tend to enhance the communicative role of the visual mode. Among the
constraints, a major one is related to the real-time delivery and interpretation of the
discourse. The visual channel, however, allows faster mental processing and is
particularly useful as already noted for explaining trends or displaying spatial
information. It is therefore a highly efficient semiotic mode in this particular
communicative situation. Another constraint concerns the often heterogeneous linguistic
background of audiences at scientific conferences, which makes communication by
language alone potentially unreliable. Visual communication in science is however
universal, and communication via this mode is thus likely to be more effective for such
audiences.
38 The particular affordances or potential linked to the CP environment likewise make
visual communication a particularly prominent semiotic. There are, for example, few
restrictions on the quantity or type of visuals shown. Visuals permanently accompany the
speaker's monologue and much more information can thus be visualised in the CP than in
an article. Such visualised information does not necessarily require verbalisation. In
addition, specific visual strategies are also available for creating meaning in the CP, and
the grammar of visual composition can thus be fully exploited (Kress & van Leeuwen
1996; Rowley-Jolivet 2002 and forthcoming). Many of the cohesive functions fulfilled by
language in the article are accomplished by exploiting the semiotic possibilities of spatial,
temporal, and spatio-temporal visual composition in the CP. In certain disciplines such as
geology, for example, a left-hand and right-hand screen are often used which allows
many logical relations (comparison/contrast, cause/consequence etc.) to be expressed
visually by the juxtaposition of the two visuals. Similarly other visual strategies exist for
communicating the structure of the talk and the links or parallelisms between different
parts. Move categories, as already noted, can be communicated visually rather than solely
by language: a change in type of visual, for example, can indicate a new move or sub-
move.
39 While discovering the particular affordances of the visual semiotic in the CP genre
remains for the moment a monomodal approach, it would nevertheless seem to be a
necessary first step towards a fully multimodal approach. The latter is extremely complex
and time-consuming, as it involves the simultaneous tracking of three modes and their
interactions. The possible combinations are numerous as information can be carried by
any of the three channels separately or by two or more simultaneously. The visual
analysis alone therefore is far from providing a complete picture, but can nevertheless
provide a methodological basis for teaching applications in oral scientific
communication. Learners can be made more aware of the functional specialism of each
semiotic mode, and be guided towards achieving an optimum fit between the type of
information to be communicated and its semiotic expression.
Conclusion
40 The aim of this article was to provide a methodological overview of some of the issues
involved in characterising the scientific conference presentation. The three types of
analysis considered have all provided us with useful insights into certain aspects of the
genre, although each also has its limitations, and leaves certain aspects unexplored.
41 The microscopic analysis of certain syntactic features provides new and useful
information on the relation between generic characteristics and language choices at a
local level – such as the contextual motivation for using pseudo-clefts but not passives in
the CP. The emphasis on sentence level phenomena however does not allow investigation
of the higher level structural organisation of the CP. Move analysis in contrast, which is a
more macroscopic approach, does address the question of rhetorical structure, and our
analysis of the CP introductions has shown considerable differences between the CP and
the RA in this respect and would also seem to hold considerable potential for analyses of a
more pedagogical type.
42 However, neither the move analysis nor the syntactic analysis make it possible to
adequately capture the multimodal dimension, which clearly differentiates the CP from a
corresponding article. In the CP the visual semiotic plays an essential role. As a first step
therefore we have tried to see exactly what the affordances of the visual mode in the CP
genre are, and to gauge how its communicative potential is enhanced compared to the
visuals of the RA. Any method that neglects the vital multimodal nature of the conference
presentation in science will necessarily only present a partial view of the genre. In order
to fully characterise the genre it will be necessary to combine the different elements and
approaches examined. This however remains a formidable task.
BIBLIOGRAPHY
Anthony, L. 1999. “Writing research article introductions in software engineering: How accurate
is a standard model?” IEEE Transactions on Professional Communication 42/1, 38-46.
Askehave, I. & J.M. Swales. 2001. “Genre identification and communicative purpose: A problem
and a possible solution”. Applied Linguistics 22/2, 195-212.
Brett, P. 1994. “A genre analysis of the results section of sociology articles”. English for Specific
Purposes 13/1, 47-59.
Candlin, C.N. & K. Hyland (eds.) 1999. Writing: Texts, processes and practices. London: Longman.
Carter-Thomas, S. & E. Rowley-Jolivet. 2001. “Syntactic differences in oral and written scientific
discourse: the role of information structure”. ASp 31-33, 19-37.
Dubois, B. 1980. “Genre and structure of biomedical speeches”. Forum Linguisticum Vol. V,
140-168.
Dudley-Evans, T. 1994. “Genre analysis an approach to text analysis for ESP”. In Coulthard M.
(ed.), Advances in Written Text Analysis. London: Routledge, 219-228.
Hopkins, A. “An Investigation into the organizing and organizational features of published
conference papers”, Unpublished MA Dissertation, University of Birmingham.
Hyland, K. 2000. Disciplinary Discourses. Social interactions in academic writing. London: Longman.
Kress, G. & T. van Leeuwen. 1996. Reading Images. The grammar of visual design. London: Routledge.
Kress, G., C. Jewitt, J. Ogborn & C. Tsatsarelis. 2001. Multimodal Teaching and Learning. The rhetorics
of the science classroom. London: Continuum.
Krohn, R. 1991. “Why are graphs so central in science?”. Biology and Philosophy 6, 181-203.
Larkin, J.H. & H.A. Simon. 1987. “Why a diagram is (sometimes) worth ten thousand words”.
Cognitive Science 11, 65-99.
Lemke, J. 1998. “Multiplying meaning: Visual and verbal semiotics in scientific text”. In Martin
J.R. & R. Veel. Reading Science. Critical and functional perspectives on discourses of science. London:
Routledge.
Lynch, M. & S. Woolgar (eds.) 1990. Representation in Scientific Practice. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Nwogu, K. 1997. “The medical research paper: Structure and functions”. English for Specific
Purposes 16/2, 119-138.
Peng, J. 1987. “Organisational features in chemical engineering research articles”. ELR Journal 1,
79-116.
Rowley-Jolivet, E. 1999. “The pivotal role of conference papers in the network of scientific
communication”. ASp 23-26, 179-196.
Samraj, B. 2002. “Introductions in research articles: variations across disciplines”. English for
Specific Purposes 21/1, 1-17.
Shalom, C. 2002. “The academic conference: A forum for enacting genre knowledge”. In Ventola
E. et al., The Language of Conferencing. Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang, 51-68.
Simpson, R.C. & Swales, J.M. (eds.) 2001. Corpus Linguistics in North America. Ann Arbor: University
of Michigan Press.
Swales, J. 1981. Aspects of article introduction., Birmingham; UK: The University of Aston, Language
Studies Unit.
Swales, J. 1990. Genre Analysis: English in academic and research settings. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Swales, J. In press. Research genres: Explorations and applications. New York: Cambridge University
Press.
Ventola, E., C. Shalom & S. Thompson (eds). 2002. The Language of Conferencing. Frankfurt am
Main: Peter Lang.
APPENDIXES
Appendix 1: The Corpus
The spoken data used in this study comprise 44 oral presentations given by NS at the
following conferences:
1. EUG VII : European Union of Geosciences, 4-8 April 1993, Strasbourg.
2. International Symposium on Mineralization related to Mafic and Ultramafic Rocks, 1–3
Sept. 1993, Orleans.
3. First International Symposium on Conservative Treatment in Oncology, 17-19 June
1993, Lyon.
4. First Annual European-American Conference on Gastrointestinal Oncology: Cancers of
the Lower Gastrointestinal Tract, 22-24 Sept. 1994, Bordeaux.
5. Euro Electromagnetics. International Symposium on Electromagnetic Environments
and Consequences. May 30 – June 4, 1994, Bordeaux.
6. 15th International Symposium on Plasma Chemistry (ISPC 15). Orléans, 9-13 July 2001.
The comparative corpus of 9 presentations (P) + 9 proceedings articles (A) in physics
(Conference N°5 above) comprises the following:
NOTES
1. Details of the conferences, presentations and articles can be found in Appendix 1.
2. Examples from the proceedings articles are referred to by the letter A followed by the number
of the article, and examples from the conference presentations by the letter P (see Appendix 1).
3. All three models – Swales (1990), Dubois (1980) and Thompson (1994) – are reproduced in
Appendix 2.
4. An interesting exception to this, however, is modelling or simulation papers in science in
which the speaker runs the simulation or ‘virtual experiment’ on his laptop in front of the
audience.
ABSTRACTS
This article will consider three different approaches to a genre analysis of the conference
presentation (CP) in science. We highlight some of the research problems involved in such an
investigation and discuss the advantages and limitations of the three methods in furthering our
understanding of this important spoken academic genre. The first approach is based on a
microscopic view of the discourse and focuses on certain recurrent syntactic features of the CP
that are pragmatically motivated by the communicative context. The second approach is more
macroscopic, and discusses the feasibility of using move analysis as means to exploring the
rhetorical organisation of the introduction section of the CP. However as neither of these
approaches addresses the vital multimodal nature of the CP, we therefore underline in the third
part some of the important features involved in a multimodal analysis.
Cet article envisage trois démarches différentes pour la caractérisation d’un genre scientifique
spécifique : la communication orale de congrès. Nous mettons en évidence certains problèmes
méthodologiques soulevés par une telle analyse, et nous traitons des avantages et des
inconvénients des trois démarches pour appréhender ce genre scientifique important. La
première démarche se fonde sur une analyse microscopique du discours et s'attache à l'étude de
certaines particularités syntaxiques de la communication scientifique orale qui résultent des
motivations particulières du contexte communicatif du congrès scientifique. La deuxième
démarche est macroscopique : il s'agit d'explorer les possibilités d’analyser l’organisation
rhétorique de la communication de congrès à l’aide d’un move analysis. Toutefois, ces deux
démarches n’éclairent pas la dimension multimodale de la communication orale de congrès. Nous
soulignons donc dans une troisième partie certains aspects importants d'une analyse
véritablement multimodale.
INDEX
Mots-clés: communication de congrès, genre, move analysis, multimodalité, rhétorique, syntaxe
Keywords: multimodality, rhetoric, conference presentation, syntax
AUTHORS
SHIRLEY CARTER-THOMAS
Shirley Carter-Thomasestmaître de conférences à l’Institut National des Télécommunications
(GET/INT), Évry. Ses domaines d’intérêt sont l’analyse du discours scientifique et le passage de
l’oral à l’écrit, la pragmatique et la syntaxe. Elle est membre du groupe de recherche OSTERLITS
(UMR 7108) du CNRS. [email protected]
ELIZABETH ROWLEY-JOLIVET
Elizabeth Rowley-Jolivet est maître de conférences à l’Institut Polytechnique de l’Université
d’Orléans. Ses axes de recherche sont l’analyse du discours scientifique oral, la communication
visuelle et la sociologie de la science. Elle est membre du groupe de recherche OSTERLITS (UMR
7108) du CNRS. [email protected]