BPCC 107 Full Textbook
BPCC 107 Full Textbook
BPCC 107 Full Textbook
social psychology
BPCC-107
SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
COURSE COORDINATOR
Dr. Smita Gupta General Editor
Discipline of Psychology
SOSS, IGNOU Dr. Smita Gupta
Discipline of Psychology
SOSS, IGNOU
COURSE PREPARATION TEAM
Unit Writer Editor
BLOCK 1 INTRODUCTION TO (Content, format
SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY Language)
Unit 1 Introduction to Social Psychology Dr. Ari Sudan Tiwari Dr.Smita Gupta
Scientist ‘E’Defence Institute and
of Psychological Research Dr. Mohsin Uddin
Ministry of Defence, Lucknow SOSS, IGNOU
Road, Timarpur, Delhi
Unit 2 Theories of Social Psychology Dr. Ari Sudan Tiwari Dr.Smita Gupta
Scientist ‘E’Defence Institute and
of Psychological Research Dr. Mohsin Uddin
Ministry of Defence, Lucknow SOSS, IGNOU
Road, Timarpur, Delhi
(Also adapted few paragraphs
from BPC-006, Unit 3)
BLOCK 2 SELF IN SOCIAL CONTEXT SELF Dr.Smita Gupta
Unit 3 Self and Its Processes Adapted from BPC-006, and
Block 1, Unit 4 Dr. Mohsin Uddin
Unit 4 Self in Social Context Dr. Ari Sudan Tiwari
Scientist ‘E’Defence Institute
of Psychological Research
Ministry of Defence, Lucknow
Road, Timarpur, Delhi
BLOCK 3 SOCIAL COGNITION
Unit 5 Social Cognition: Understanding Dr. Ari Sudan Tiwari
Social Behaviour- I Scientist ‘E’Defence Institute
of Psychological Research
Ministry of Defence, Lucknow
Road, Timarpur, Delhi
Unit 6 Social Cognition: Understanding Dr. Ari Sudan Tiwari
Social Behaviour- II Scientist ‘E’Defence Institute
of Psychological Research
Ministry of Defence, Lucknow
Road, Timarpur Delhi-110054
Unit 7 Attitude And Attitude Change Dr. Ari Sudan Tiwari
Scientist ‘E’Defence Institute
of Psychological Research
Ministry of Defence, Lucknow
Road, Timarpur, Delhi
BLOCK 4 SOCIAL INFLUENCE Dr. Smita Gupta
Unit 8 Social Influence Dr. Ari Sudan Tiwari and
Scientist ‘E’Defence Institute Dr. Mohsin Uddin
of Psychological Research SOSS, IGNOU
Ministry of Defence, Lucknow
Road, Timarpur, Delhi
Unit 9 Aggression and Social Influence Dr. Ari Sudan Tiwari
Scientist ‘E’Defence Institute
of Psychological Research
Ministry of Defence, Lucknow
Road, Timarpur, Delhi
Unit 10 Interpersonal Attraction and Dr. Ari Sudan Tiwari
Pro-Social Behaviour Scientist ‘E’Defence Institute
of Psychological Research
Ministry of Defence, Lucknow
Road, Timarpur, Delhi
BLOCK 5 GROUP DYNAMICS Dr. Smita Gupta
Unit 11 Group Processes Dr. Ari Sudan Tiwari and
Scientist ‘E’Defence Institute Dr. Mohsin Uddin
of Psychological Research SOSS, IGNOU
Ministry of Defence, Lucknow
Road, Timarpur, Delhi
Unit 12 Leader and Leadership Dr. Mohsin Uddin
Consultant
Discipline of Psychology
IGNOU, Delhi
PRINT PRODUCTION
Mr. Rajiv Girdhar Mr. Hemant Preeda Mr. Suresh Kumar
Assistant Registrar (Publication) Section Officer (Publication) School of Social Science
MPDD, IGNOU, New Delhi MPDD, IGNOU, New Delhi IGNOU, New Delhi
January, 2021
© Indira Gandhi National Open University, 2021
ISBN:
All rights reserved. No part of this work may be reproduced in any form, by mimeography or any other
means, without permission in writing from the Indira Gandhi National Open University.
Further information on the Indira Gandhi National Open University courses may be
obtained from the University’s Office at Maidan Garhi, New Delhi-110 068 or visit our website: http://
www.ignou.ac.in
Printed and published on behalf of the Indira Gandhi National Open University, New Delhi, by Director,
School of Social Sciences.
Laser Typeset by : Tessa Media & Computers, C-206, Shaheen Bagh, New Delhi
Printed at :
Contents
Page No.
The second unit highlights the relationship of social psychology with other
disciplines. The unit also describes the historical background of social psychology.
It discusses the levels of analysis of social behaviour and various theoretical
approaches of social psychology.
8
Introduction to Social
UNIT 1 INTRODUCTION TO SOCIAL Psychology
PSYCHOLOGY*
Structure
1.0 Objectives
1.1 Introduction
1.2 Definition, Nature and Scope of Social Psychology
1.2.1 Definition of Social Psychology
1.2.2 Nature of Social Psychology
1.2.2.1 Social Psychology Applies Scientific Methods of Study
1.2.2.2 Social Psychology Studies Individuals’ Thought, Feeling and
Behaviour
1.2.2.3 Social Psychology Studies Individuals’ Behaviour in Social Contexts
1.2.3 Scope of Social Psychology
1.3 Historical Development of Social Psychology
1.3.1 Early Years of Social Psychology
1.3.2 Formational Years of Social Psychology
1.3.3 Expansion of Social Psychology
1.3.4 Social Psychology in 21st Century
1.3.5 Social Psychology in India
1.4 Let Us Sum Up
1.5 Unit End Questions
1.6 Glossary
1.7 Answers to Self Assessment Questions
1.8 Suggested Readings and References
1.0 OBJECTIVES
After reading this unit, you will be able to:
Define social psychology;
Explain the nature and scope of social psychology;
Describe the historical background of social psychology; and
Illustrate the development of social psychology with reference to India.
1.1 INTRODUCTION
Let us ponder upon some situations, events, issues and people. While going for a
job interview we dress up individually; but when we reach the interview venue
we find that all the candidates are dressed up in almost similar fashion. In another
situation, after the Nirbhaya incidence in Delhi on 16 December 2012, thousands
of people flooded on the roads of Delhi and other parts of the country protesting
against the incidence. Leaders like Mahatma Gandhi, Abraham Lincoln, Nelson
Mandela, Dalai Lama and many others like them influenced thoughts, feelings
and behaviours of the masses in a great manner and led the people’s thought
toward remarkable social, religious, political and ethical revolutions.
* Dr. Ari Sudan Tiwari, Scientist ‘E’Defence Institute of Psychological Research, Ministry of Defence,
Lucknow Road, Timarpur, Delhi
9
Introduction to Social These are few examples from the history of human being where either we are
Psychology
influenced by other people or we influence other people’s behaviours. Social
psychology is the branch of psychology which studies human behaviour in an
array of social situations. The present unit will help you to understand the
definition, scope and nature of psychology. Further, in this unit, you will be
acquainted with the historical background and social psychology with reference
to Indian context.
The scientific social psychology carries out three major activities: description,
explanation and prediction of social behaviours. Social psychology provides a
scientific account of social behaviour based on direct observation rather than on
common beliefs. Social psychology also attempts to explain why people behave
in a particular way in a particular social situation. Such interconnected
explanations of social behaviours lead to formation of theories which help in
predicting social behaviours and managing them in a desirable direction.
10
1.2.2.2 Social Psychology Studies Individuals’ Thought, Feeling Introduction to Social
Psychology
and Behaviour
The varied issues of the field of social psychology include– individuals’ thought,
feeling and behaviour. Cognition is referred as the manner in which people process
information. Social psychology studies cognition that relates to social activities
and that helps us in understanding and predictingour social behaviours. Social
psychology also studies the feelings we experience as an individual in our social
lives. What we think or feel in the social context is finally expressed through our
behaviours in social interactions. Social psychology studies these behaviours in
the forms of cooperation, helping behaviour, conflict, aggression, etc.
Social psychologists study why we are often helpful to other people and
why we may be unfriendly or aggressive at other times.
Social psychologists study what factors lead people to purchase one product
rather than another.
Social psychologists study how men and women behave differently in social
settings.
Social psychologists study what makes some people more likely to engage
in environmentally friendly behaviours than others.
Social psychologists study how someone might choose to risk their life to
save that of a complete stranger.
12
Self Assessment Questions I Introduction to Social
Psychology
1) Define social psychology.
.......................................................................................................................
.......................................................................................................................
.......................................................................................................................
.......................................................................................................................
.......................................................................................................................
During the time of World War II,the studies on conformity were conducted by
Muzafir Sherif (1936) and Solomon Asch (1952). Studies on obedience were
conducted by Stanley Milgram (1974) and Philip Zimbardo (Haney, Banks, &
Zimbardo, 1973).
In the later years, Indian social psychologists continued working in the areas of
prejudice, stereotypes and social attitudes. Large scale surveys were conducted
taking various attitude measures. Adinarayan conducted studies on racial and
15
Introduction to Social communal attitudes (1953) and on caste attitudes (1958). With increasing
Psychology
emphasis on public awareness programmes for health, family planning,
agricultural innovations, attitude change became a major topic of research. During
these years social psychology was growing in popularity in India as evident
from surveys conducted from time to time.
1) Define social psychology and explain the nature and scope of social
psychology.
1.6 GLOSSARY
Social Psychology: Social psychology is defined as the scientific study of how
we feel about, think about and behave toward the people around us and how our
feelings, thoughts, and behaviours are influenced by those people in the social
context.
Social Neuroscience:Social neuroscience is the study of how our socialbehaviour
both influences and is influenced by the activities of our brain.
Classical Conditioning: Classical conditioning presumes that when a neutral
stimulus (conditioned stimulus, CS) is paired with a natural stimulus
(unconditioned stimulus, UCS), neutral stimulus alone acquires the ability to
elicit the response (conditioned response, CR) which naturally occurs
(unconditioned response, UCR) after natural stimulus.
Reinforcement: Reinforcement is the mechanism by which people learn to
perform a particular behaviour because it is followed by something that is
pleasurable or that satisfies a need (or they learn to avoid behaviour that is followed
by unpleasant consequences).
Observational Learning: Observational learning presumes thatpeople often learn
social attitudes and behaviours simply by watching other people, known
technically as “models”.
Social Exchange Theory: Social exchange theory uses the concept of
reinforcement to explain stability and change in social interactions and relations
between individuals.
Social Cognitive Theories: Social cognitive theoriesemphasise that a person’s
behaviour depends on the way he or she perceives the social situation.
Schema: Schema is a mental representation capturing the general characteristics
of a particular class of episodes, events or individuals.
Principle of Cognitive Consistency:Principle of cognitive consistency maintains
that individuals strive to hold ideas that are consistent or congruous with one
another, rather than ideas that are inconsistent or incongruous.
17
Introduction to Social 2) Social psychology is scientific in nature. It applies the scientific method of
Psychology
systematic observation, description and measurementto the study of the
human behaviour in social context. Social psychological studies refer to the
data being collected through direct observation or experiment. Such
experiments and observation are carried out carefully and reported in detail
so that other investigators can repeat and verify the work.
References
Adinarayan, S. P. (1953). Before and after Independence–A study of racial and
communal attitudes in India. British Journal of Psychology, 44, 108-115.
Dalal, A. K., & Mishra, G. (2001). Social Psychology in India: Evolution and
emerging trends. In In A. K. Dalal& G. Misra (Eds.), New Directions in Indian
Psychology (vol. 1: Social Psychology), New Delhi: Sage.
Eagly, A. H., &Chaiken, S. (1993). The Psychology of Attitudes. Fort Worth, TX:
Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (2008). Social Cognition: From Brains to Culture.
Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill.
Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., & Tversky, A. (1982). Judgment under Uncertainty:
Heuristics and Biases. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Kassin, S., Fein, S., & Markus, H. R. (2017). Social Psychology (10th ed.).
Cengage Learning.
20
Introduction to Social
UNIT 2 THEORIES OF SOCIAL Psychology
PSYCHOLOGY*
Structure
2.0 Objectives
2.1 Introduction
2.2 Social Psychology and Other Disciplines
2.2.1 Social Psychology and Other Social Sciences
2.2.2 Social Psychology and Other Branches in Psychology
2.3 Levels of Analysis of Social Behaviour
2.3.1 Individual-Intrapersonal Level Analysis
2.3.2 Interpersonal Interactions
2.3.3 Interaction between Individual and Group
2.3.4 Interaction between Groups
2.4 Theoretical Approaches to Social Psychology
2.4.1 Learning Theories
2.4.2 Cognitive Theories
2.5 Research Methods in Social Psychology
2.5.1 Observational Method
2.5.2 Experimental Method
2.5.2.1 Quasi-experimental Method
2.5.2.2 Experimental Method
2.5.3 Ethical Issues in Research
2.6 Let Us Sum Up
2.7 Unit End Questions
2.8 Glossary
2.9 Answers to Self Assessment Questions
2.10 Suggested Readings and References
2.0 OBJECTIVES
After reading this unit, you will be able to:
Illustrate the levels of analysis of social behaviour;
Explain the relationship of social psychology with other disciplines;
Delineate various theoretical approaches of social psychology;
Discuss the different types of research methods in social psychology; and
Elucidate the various ethical issues of research methods.
2.1 INTRODUCTION
It must be clear to you from the previous unit that, social psychology is the
branch of psychology which studies human behaviour in an array of social
situations. In this unit, you will be explained about the Social psychology as
branch of psychology, its nature, scope and relationship with other disciplines.
You will also be acquainted with the historical background and various theoretical
* Dr. Ari Sudan Tiwari, Scientist ‘E’Defence Institute of Psychological Research, Ministry of Defence,
Lucknow Road, Timarpur, Delhi, (Also adapted few paragraphs from BPC-006, Unit 3) 21
Introduction to Social approaches to social psychology. The unit will also discuss the levels of analysis
Psychology
of social behaviour.
22
Social Psychology and Sociology Theories of Social Psychology
Sociology is the study of human societies and the groups that form those societies.
Although both sociologists and social psychologists are interested in how people
behave in societies and groups, they differ in what they focus on. Sociologists
focus on the group as a single unit, whereas social psychologists focus on the
individual members that make up the group. Some sociologists call themselves
social psychologists and the exchange of ideas and findings between the two
fields that have sometimes been quite fruitful because they bring different
perspectives to the same problems.
Just as any group influences the behaviour of its members, these members, in
turn, may influence the group itself. For instance, individuals contribute to group
productivity and group decision making. Moreover, some members may provide
leadership, performing functions such as planning, organizing, and controlling,
necessary for successful group performance. Without effective leadership,
coordination among members will falter and the group will drift or fail.
There are three general mechanisms by which learning occurs. One is association,
or classical conditioning. Classical Conditioning presumes that when a neutral
stimulus (conditioned stimulus, CS) is paired with a natural stimulus
(unconditioned stimulus, UCS), neutral stimulus alone acquires the ability to
elicit the response (conditioned response, CR) which naturally occurs
(unconditioned response, UCR) after natural stimulus. Pavlov’s dogs learned to
salivate at the sound of a bell because they were presented with food every time.
After a while, they would salivate to the sound of the bell even in the absence of
the meat because they associated the bell with meat. Humans sometimes learn
emotions by association(Pavlov, 1927). After a particularly painful visit to a
particular place, the mere mention of the place may arouse anxiety.
Despite its usefulness in illuminating why relationships change and how people
learn, learning theories have been criticized on various grounds. One criticism is
that learning theories portrays individuals primarily as reacting to or imitating
environmental stimuli. The theory does not account easily for creativity,
innovation, orinvention. A second criticism is that reinforcement theory largely
ignores or downplays other motivations. It characterizes social behaviour as
hedonistic, with individuals striving to maximize profits from outcomes. Thus,
it cannot easily explain selfless behaviour such as altruism and martyrdom. Despite
its limitations, reinforcement theory has enjoyed substantial success in explaining
why individuals persist in emitting certain behaviours, how they learn new
behaviours and how they influence the behaviour of others through exchange.
One way to study cognitive structure is to observe changes that occur in a person’s
cognitions when he or she is under challenge or attack. The changes will reveal
facts about the underlying structure or organization of his or her cognitions. An
important idea emerging from this approach is the principle of cognitive
consistencywhich maintains that individuals strive to hold ideas that are consistent
or congruous with one another, rather than ideas that are inconsistent or
incongruous. If a person holds several ideas that are incongruous or inconsistent,
he or she will experience internal conflict. In reaction, he or she will likely change
one or more ideas, thereby making them consistent and resolving this conflict.
While the paths to these goals may differ, depending on the orientation of a
particular social psychologist, the interaction between theory and research is
well recognised today. “Many psychologists working in an applied field are keenly
aware of the need for close cooperation between theoretical and applied
psychology. This can be accomplished in psychology…..if the theorist does not
look toward applied problems with high brow aversion or with a fear of social
problems, and if the applied psychologist realizes that there is nothing so practical
as a good theory” (Lewin, 1951: 169).
Our discussion will be limited to following research methods:
For more complex behaviours, establishing likely cause is much more difficult.
Thus to find out the course of a particular behaviours, we must look carefully at
the result of many observations, noting the effects of a particular factor. 29
Introduction to Social Observational method can be relatively informal and unstructured or it can be
Psychology
formal and structured. But the object in each case in the same, “to abstract
information from the complex flux of social behaviours that are of potential
significance to the research questions; and to record each instance of such actions
over some period” (Manstead A.S. R. Semin G.R., 2001: 97). The nature of
research setting or topic dictates that observation is conducted in a relatively
informal and unstructured manner with the researcher posing as a member of the
group being observed. A Classical example of research employing this method
is Festinger, Riecken and Schachter’s (1956) study of the consequences of blatant
disconfirmation of strongly held beliefs. The investigators identified a religious
sect which predicted that the northern hemisphere would be destroyed by flood
on a certain date. By joining that sect, members of the research team were able to
observe what happened when the predicted events failed to materialise?
Observational methods of data collection have two main advantages over the
self report methods. They can often be made without disturbing the naturally
occurring behaviours. Even where people know that they are being observed,
enacting behaviour may be quite engrossing.
Nevertheless, there are some types of behaviours that are either impossible to
observe directly (because they took place in the past) or difficult to observe
directly (because they are normally enacted in private). Moreover, social
psychologists are often interested in measuring people’s perceptions, cognitions
or evaluations, none of which can be directly assessed simply through observation.
For these self-report measures or other techniques are often used.
30
Theories of Social Psychology
Table: Terminology in Experimental Research
Source: Atkinson & Hilgard et al (2003) Introduction to Psychology 14th edition. Wadsworth
Asia Pvt. Ltd.
There are two basic types of experiments in social psychology laboratory and
natural laboratory and natural experiments have their particular rules. The
laboratory experiment is of particular interest in social psychological discussions.
Social psychologists use some variations. Two of the most common of these
variations are the quasi-experiments and the true randomized experiments. These
two methods differ with respect to realism of the setting in which data are
collected, and the degree of control that the researcher has over that setting.
31
Introduction to Social of realism. It is sometimes possible to conduct a true randomized experiment in
Psychology
an everyday setting; this is called a field experiment.
There are many types of experimental designs used in social psychology, more
sophisticated and complex than the above. Each design represents a more complete
attempt to rule out the possibility that observed difference between conditions
result from something other than the manipulation of independent variable. A
common design in social psychological experiment is the factorial experiment,
in which two or more independent variables are manipulated within the same
study. The main benefit of a factorial design is that it allows the researcher to
examine the separate and combined effects of two or more independent variables.
The separate effects of each of the independent variable are known as main effects.
Interaction effect is a term used when combined effects of two (or more)
independent variables in a factorial experiment yield a pattern that differs from
the sum of the main effects.
Protection of Participants: The security and safety are the prior concern
of a researcher. No personal harm or violation of the rights of the participants
should take place.
2) Social psychology does not examines the impact of a group on the behaviours
of its individual members. ......................
3) Cognitive theory presumes that the mental activities of the individual are
not important determinants of social behaviour. .......................
2.8 GLOSSARY
Social Psychology: Social psychology is defined as the scientific study of how
we feel about, think about and behave toward the people around us and how our
33
Introduction to Social feelings, thoughts, and behaviours are influenced by those people in the social
Psychology
context.
Social Neuroscience: Social neuroscience is the study of how our social behaviour
both influences and is influenced by the activities of our brain.
Dalal, A. K., & Mishra, G. (2001). Social psychology in India: Evolution and
emerging trends. In In A. K. Dalal& G. Misra (Eds.), New Directions in Indian
Psychology (vol. 1: Social Psychology), New Delhi: Sage.
Eagly, A. H., &Chaiken, S. (1993). The Psychology of Attitudes. Fort Worth, TX:
Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (2008). Social Cognition: From Brains to Culture.
Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill.
35
Introduction to Social Haney, C., Banks, C., & Zimbardo, P. (1973). Interpersonal dynamics in a
Psychology
simulated prison. International Journal of Criminology and Penology, 1, 69-97.
Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., & Tversky, A. (1982). Judgment Under Uncertainty:
Heuristics and Biases. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Kassin, S., Fein, S., & Markus, H. R. (2017). Social Psychology (10th ed.).
Cengage Learning.
LeBon, G. (1908). The Crowd: A Study of the Popular Mind. London: Unwin
(original work published 1896). Online: http://cupid.ecom.unimelb.edu.au/het/
lebon/crowds.pdf
36
Sinha, D. (1952). Behaviour in a catastrophic situation: A psychological study of Theories of Social Psychology
reports and rumours. British Journal of Psychology, 43, 200-209.
Sinha, J. B. P. (1980). Nurturant Task Leader. New Delhi: Concept.
Triplett, N. (1898). The dynamogenic factors in pacemaking and competition.
American Journal of Psychology, 9, 507-533.
37
BLOCK 2
SELF IN SOCIAL CONTEXT
Self In Social Context
INTRODUCTION
This block consists of two units. The first unit deals with self and its processes. It
also discusses the formation and management of impression. Interestingly, it
also discusses the tactics with the help of which you can enhance self presentation.
In this unit we will move ahead continuing our description from the previous
units. But here we are going to explore the cognitive and dynamics of these
processes and would be studying the most pertinent question – to what extent
our social perceptions and person perceptions are accurate? Do these processes
have errors, bias and misinterpretation which affect our real understanding of
social world? In layman’s language, can we trust the information which we have
gathered from impression formation, attributions and social categorisation? If
not what do we about it.
The second unit deals with the concept of society as well as society and their
influences on individual’s behaviour. Society and culture are important
determinant of how our perceptions, attitudes, and behaviours are shaped and
moderated throughout our lives.The type and nature of bonds and relationships
people form in their societies and families are also unique to them. We often
attribute such differences in the societies to the differences in their cultures. In
this unit, you will come to know about the concept of culture, process of
enculturation and acculturation as well as individualistic and collectivistic
societies. By the end of the unit, you will also come to know about the cultural
influences on individuals’ perception and actions.
40
Self and its Processes
UNIT 3 SELF AND ITS PROCESSES*
Structure
3.0 Objectives
3.1 Introduction
3.2 Self Concept
3.2.1 Formation of Self Concept
3.2.2 Self Esteem
3.2.3 Self Efficacy
3.3 Cognitive and Motivational Basis of Social and Person Perception
3.3.1 Impression Formation and Management
3.3.2 Impression Management
3.3.3 Theories of Self Presentation
3.3.4 Self Presentation Tactics
3.3.4.1 Ingratiation
3.3.4.2 Intimidation
3.3.4.3 Self Promotion
3.3.4.4 Exemplification
3.3.4.5 Supplication
3.3.5 Individual Differences in Self Presentation
3.4 Let Us Sum Up
3.5 Unit End Questions
3.6 Glossary
3.7 Answers to Self Assessment Questions
3.8 Suggested Readings and References
3.0 OBJECTIVES
After reading this unit, you will be able to:
Describe the concept of self and self- esteem;
Identify the different features of self- efficacy;
Analyse the dynamics of impression formation; and
Explain the theories and tactics of self- presentation.
3.1 INTRODUCTION
In the previous unit we have studied social cognition – a process of knowing,
understanding and predicting the behaviour of others. Social cognition involves
two basic processes i.e. social perception and person perception. Under social
perception, we had studied various mechanisms of social perception – non-verbal
communications, attributions, impression formation, and implicit personality
theory. In person perception, the mechanisms we studied were physical cues,
schemas, heuristics, construct and social categorisation. Here we concentrated
on the structural and functional aspects of social and person perception. In this
3.2 SELF-CONCEPT
It refers to our understanding about us. It provides a clue to what he or she thinks
about them. Each individual has a self concept of themselves which consists of
those characteristics which they feel are important and related to them self and is
their identity. It is related to our capabilities, nature, personality and other personal
characteristics which help us to define who we are. Further, our self concept is
also dependent on situation, that is, we react differently in different situation.
For example, you might consider as fun loving, adventurous, sports person, good
leader or traditional, less confident and so on. Our identity with any organization,
membership, culture or family also is a part of our self concept.
3.2.3 Self-Efficacy
Self-efficacy refers to the evaluations that people make about their ability to
perform a task within a specific context. The feedback of our potent and abilities
that we get from others reinforces our self-perception and self-efficacy. So, if the
perception of self is positive, more is the confidence level and more will be the
self-efficacy. Therefore, self-perception affects our behaviour and responses.
While some of this process is under our control, much of it is also shaped by the
people in our lives. Positive feedback increases our self-efficacy while negative
feedback of ourselves decreases our self-efficacy. If there is a contradiction on
how others opine for us and what we think about us, then it has a significant
impact on our self-concept as well as self-esteem.
43
Self In Social Context 3.3.1 Impression Formation and Management
In forming impression about others, there are six simple and general principles:
We usually talk as if there is a single self which is stable and well defined. But
social psychologists believe it is more appropriate to think of self as multiple
selves because people display different aspects of themselves in
differentsituations. Schlenker (1980) has termed this as ‘impression management’
and defined it as the conscious or unconscious attempt to control images that are
projected in real or imagined social interactions. When there images deal with
some aspects of self we call the process as self presentation.
3) The different techniques for boosting their image fall into two categories
........................ and ........................ .
45
Self In Social Context Theory of self presentation: Erving Goffman has drawn parallels of this
behaviour to the world of theater and formulated a theory of ‘self presentation
in everyday life’. Goffman (1959/1967) has described social interaction as
a theoretical performance in which each individual acts out “live” – a set of
carefully choosen verbal and non-verbal acts that expresses one’s self. In
self presentation, one of the fundamental rules of social interaction is mutual
commitment i.e. all the members of the interaction use certain face-saving
devices, so as to maintain a face. Through maintaining face is not the goal
of the social interaction it is rather necessary for social interaction to continue.
Incidents that threaten the face of participant also threaten the survival of
the relationship. Therefore we somehow try to avert the embarrassment that
might occur and threaten the relationship by overlooking by helping others
apologise for the social blindness that they commit. Thus for Goffman social
interaction requires its participants to be able to regulate their self presentation
and that it will be perceived and evaluated appropriately by others.
Situated identities theory: C.N. Alexander has put forth another theory
called the ‘situated identities’ theory. There is a pattern of social behaviour
for each social setting and Alexander claimed that people strive to create
the most favourable situated identities for themselves in their social
encounters. For example a college professor might aim for a highly academic
identity when presenting a paper at a seminar, a somewhat more relaxed
identity during lectures and a causal and informal situated identities at a
social gathering and with friends. This is simply a role a person is performing
in different situations.
3.3.4.1 Ingratiation
This is the most common of presentation techniques and is defined as a class of
strategic behaviour illicitly designed to influence a particular other person
concerning the attractiveness of one’s personal qualities. In other words, the
main goal of the ingratiator is to be seen to be likeable. There are number of
ways in accomplishing this and one of the most common tactics is to give
compliment to another person.
46
However a successful ingratiatory knows when to compliment people and also Self and its Processes
has certain amount of credibility and sincerity. He uses his discrimination properly.
Another tactics is to confirm to other person’s opinions and behaviours. We tend
to like people whose beliefs, attitudes and behaviours are similar to our own.
But, there is a danger here, i.e. if the other person (target) suspects ingratiation
this factor does not work.
3.3.4.2 Intimidation
Is to arouse fear in other people. This is in contrast to ingratiation. In intimidation
by creating the image of dangerous person, the intimidator seeks to control and
interaction by the exercise of power. Intimidation is mostly used in relationships
that are non voluntary in which escape is not easily accomplished. Example a
street robber with a threat to kill if money or jewelry is not handed over. Some
times parents do it with their children and also teachers with their pupils.
3.3.4.4 Exemplification
Here the goal is to influence the impressions that others are not conscientious
workers. The person here wants to prove that he has more integrity and moral
worthiness compared to others, and wants to arouse guilty in the target person.
He wants to create an impression of a sufferer.
3.3.4.5 Supplication
Here the person advertises his/ her weakness and dependent on other person. He
is seeking sympathy. This is usually a last resort i.e. what a person is unable to
use any other strategies, he resorts to gaining sympathy. The person is presenting
an image of helplessness hoping to elicit a sense obligation from the target. The
person may use all five self presentation tactics on different occasions. So people
may specialise in one or other tactic and may use it on more than one occasions.
What ever choice or combination, the person’s aim is to create the desired
impressions someone else, there by increasing the chances of obtaining the desired
effect.
High self monitoring persons are particularly sensitivite to the expressions and
the self presentations of others in social situation and they use these as cues in
monitoring, their own self presentation for purpose of impression management.
High self monitoring persons are good at learning what is socially appropriate in
new situations, have good control of their emotional expression and effectively
use these abilities to create the impression they want to display.
In fact they can adapt themselves to any social situation, for example: they can
adopt the mannerism of a reserved, withdrawn and introverted person and then
they can abruptly do about face and portray themselves as equally convincing,
friendly, outgoing and extroverted person. In self-presentation situations, high
selfmonitoring persons are quite likely to seek out social comparisons and
information about the appropriate patterns of behaviour. They put in considerable
effort in attempting to read and understand others and behave accordingly and
guide their self presentation so as to gain approval or power of an interaction.
However self presentation on impression management is not for deceptive
purposes but is an adaptive skill in environments where there is a complex mixture
of people and policies.
Self-presentation and Human Nature: Self presentation is a basic fact of
social life. People influence the images of their ‘selves’ that are projected to
others but where is the ‘real’ self in all this? As already known, each of us
have multiple selves which we present to different people in different ways
but all of them are many aspects of only one ‘true self’ self presentation
factors is selecting certain characteristics and omitting other. Therefore,
presentation of self is an integral part of everyday social interaction.
Self Assessment Questions II
48
3.4 LET US SUM UP
In this unit we have tried to cover the cognitive and motivational basis of social Self and its Processes
and person perception. We tried to show how neural mechanisms do affect
perception etc. We also studied about the impression formation and the processes
involved thereof. A discussion in detail about the processes involved in impression
management was taken up. Following this a detailed analysis of how one presents
oneself was taken up. In these theories of self presentation was discussed and the
many tactics that are used in presenting oneself in the correct light were
considered.
3.6 GLOSSARY
Self Concept: It refers to our understanding about us.
Self-Esteem: Refers to the judgments and evaluations we make of our concept
of self.
Self-Efficacy: The evaluations that people make about their ability to perform a
task within a specific context.
Exemplification: The person here wants to prove that he has more integrity and
moral worthiness compared to others, and wants to arouse guilty in the target
person.
Supplication: Here the person advertises his/ her weakness and dependent on
other person.
Aronson E, Wilson T.D,and Akert R.M (1998) Social Psychology (third edition),
Longman Inc.
References
Aarts and Dijksterhuis 2003 quoted in Taylor, E.S, Letitia Anne Peplau, David
O.Sears (2006), “Social Psychology (12th edition.), Pearson Education, India.
Ajzen 1996 quoted in Crisp, R.J, Rhianomon N Turner (2007) Essential Social
Psychology, Sage Publications, New Delhi.
Albert Bandura, Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control (New York, NY: W. H.
Freeman, 1997).
Anderson 1968 quoted in Crisp, R.J, Rhianomon N Turner (2007) Essential Social
Psychology, Sage Publications, New Delhi.
Barbara M. Byrne, Measuring Self-Concept across the Life Span: Issues and
Instrumentation (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, 1996),
5.
50
Baron.R, Byrne D (2003), Social Psychology, Prentice Hall of India Pvt Ltd., Self and its Processes
New Delhi.
Bond and Atoum 2000 quoted in Baron, R.A Donn Byrne (2006) Social
Psychology (10th edition.) Prentice Hall of India, New Delhi.
Carner and Glass 1978, quoted in Crisp, R.J, Rhianomon N Turner (2007)
Essential Social Psychology, Sage Publications, New Delhi.
Caroll 1996 quoted in Baron, R.A Donn Byrne (2006) Social Psychology (10th
edition.) Prentice Hall of India, New Delhi.
Charles Cooley, Human Nature and the Social Order (New York, NY: Scribner,
1902).
Cooley, (1922) quoted in Crisp, R.J, Rhianomon N Turner (2007) Essential Social
Psychology, Sage Publications, New Delhi.
Cross and John (2003) quoted in Taylor, E.S, Letitia Anne Peplau, David O.Sears
(2006) Social Psychology (12th edition), Pearson Education, India.
Depaulo (1992) quoted in Baron, R.A Donn Byrne (2006) Social Psychology
(10th edition) Prentice Hall of India, New Delhi. Ekman and Friesen (1975)
quoted in Baron, R.A Donn Byrne (2006) Social Psychology (10th edition)
Prentice Hall of India, New Delhi.
Foriester and Liberman (2001) quoted in Taylor, E.S, Letitia Anne Peplau, David
O.Sears (2006), Social Psychology (12th edition), Pearson Education, India.
Gray (2009) quoted in Taylor, E.S, Letitia Anne Peplau, David O.Sears (2006),
Social Psychology (12th edition), Pearson Education, India.
Gross and Miller quoted in Crisp, R.J, Rhianomon N Turner (2007) Essential
Social Psychology, Sage Publications, New Delhi.
Harding, Kunter, Proshanky & Chein 1954, quoted in Crisp, R.J, Rhianomon N
Turner (2007) Essential Social Psychology, Sage Publications, New Delhi.
Heider 1958 quoted in Crisp, R.J, Rhianomon N Turner (2007) Essential Social
Psychology, Sage Publications, New Delhi.
John Bargh & Assou 1996 quoted in Taylor, E.S, Letitia Anne Peplau, David O.
Sears (2006), Social Psychology (12th edition), Pearson Education, India.
Joel Brockner, Self-Esteem at Work (Lexington, MA: Lexington Books,
1988), 11.
Joel Brockner, Self-Esteem at Work (Lexington, MA: Lexington Books,
1988), 2.
51
Self In Social Context Jones and Davis (1965) quoted in Crisp, R.J, Rhianomon N Turner (2007)
Essential Social Psychology, Sage Publications, New Delhi.
Jones and Pillman (1982) quoted in Crisp, R.J, Rhianomon N Turner (2007)
Essential Social Psychology, Sage Publications, New Delhi.
Milgram (1963) quoted in Taylor, E.S, Letitia Anne Peplau, David O.Sears (2006),
Social Psychology (12th edition), Pearson Education, India.
Zalenski and Larsen 2002 quoted in Taylor, E.S, Letitia Anne Peplau, David
O.Sears (2006), Social Psychology (12th edition.), Pearson Education, India.
52
Self and its Processes
UNIT 4 SELF IN SOCIAL CONTEXT*
Structure
4.0 Objectives
4.1 Introduction
4.2 Culture: Meaning and Definition
4.3 Enculturation and Acculturation
4.3.1 Difference in Enculturation and Acculturation
4.3.2 Agents of Enculturation
4.3.2.1 Parents and Siblings
4.3.2.2 Extended Family
4.3.2.3 Peer Relations
4.3.2.4 Education
4.3.2.5 Religion
4.4 Self Across Cultures
4.4.1 Outcomes of Different Self Construals Across Cultures
4.4.2 The Case of Multicultural Identities
4.4.2.1 At Intrapersonal Level
4.4.2.2 At Interpersonal Level
4.4.2.3 At Collective Level
4.5 Social Behaviour Across Cultures
4.5.1 Cross-Cultural Differences in Dynamics of Group Membership
4.5.2 In-group Identification versus In-group Bias
4.5.3 Attribution
4.5.4 Aggression
4.5.5 Person Perception, Attraction and Relationships
4.6 Let Us Sum Up
4.7 Unit End Questions
4.8 Glossary
4.9 Answers to Self Assessment Questions
4.10 Suggested Readings and References
4.0 OBJECTIVES
After reading this unit, you will be able to:
Define and describe culture;
Explain the difference between enculturation and acculturation;
Explain the agents that influence enculturation of an individual to their
society;
Differentiate between individualist and collectivist societies;
Comprehend in-group identification, multicultural identities and intergroup
bias;
Explain how the dynamics of group membership varies across cultures; and
Discuss the cultural influence on aggression, attribution, attraction, person
perception and relationships.
* Dr. Ari Sudan Tiwari, Scientist ‘E’Defence Institute of Psychological Research, Ministry of Defence,
Lucknow Road, Timarpur, Delhi 53
Self In Social Context
4.1 INTRODUCTION
Whenever we visit a foreign country, we find a number of differences between
the lives and lifestyles of the people of that country and that of our own. The
people of that country speak language that is different from ours. They eat foods
that we generally do not eat. They express happiness and grief in ways that are
not similar to ours. The type and nature of bonds and relationships they form in
their societies and families are also unique to them. We often attribute such
differences in the societies to the differences in their cultures. In this unit, you
will come to know about the concept of culture, process of enculturation and
acculturation as well as individualistic and collectivistic societies. By the end of
the unit, you will also come to know about the cultural influences on individuals’
perception and actions.
Triandis (1972): Culture includes some objective aspects, such as tools; and
some subjective aspects, such as words, shared beliefs, attitudes, norms, roles,
and values.
Jahoda (1984): Culture is a descriptive term that captures not only rules and
meanings but also behaviours.
Matsumoto and Juang (2008): Cultureisa dynamic system of rules, explicit and
implicit, established by groups in order to ensure their survival, involving
attitudes, values, beliefs, norms, and behaviours, shared by a group but harboured
54
differently by each specific unit within the group, communicated across Self in Social Context
generations, relatively stable but with the potential to change across time.
After viewing these definitions, it appears that Matsumoto and Juang (2008)
have explained culture in much broader sense by encompassing all essential
characteristics of other definitions. The definition describes the following
components of culture:
Groups and units: There are different levels at which culture is reflected.
When we take it in the perspective of individuals within groups, the units
that reflect culture are specific individuals within the group. However, for a
large group that is comprised of multiple smaller groups, various sections
are the specific units reflecting the culture.
Ensuring survival of the group: The system of rules that exists in a culture
functions as a constraint on behaviour. Absence of the rules may lead to a
situation of chaos. These rules help the smaller units within the group to
coexist with one another by offering and promoting a structure for social
order. The rules also promote balance between the needs and desires of the
groups and units by taking the larger social context and the available
resources into account.
Transmitted from one generation to the next: Culture is not a fashion trend
which is temporarily followed and practised by some people for some time
and which gets vanished with time. Rather culture, comprising of the core
aspects of the system of rules, is transmitted from one generation to the next
and therefore, it is relatively stable over time.
Cultural differences could also influence the type of involvement a parent would
have as guided by the beliefs about their roles as parents and goals of parenting.
LeVine et. al. (1996) observed an emphasis on interaction and active participation
by American mothers (of Boston suburbs), and a focus on child-safety by Kenyan
mothers (of Gusii region). This difference is thought to be a result of the difference
in the perceived goals of parenting and enculturation.
58
4.3.2.2 Extended Family Self in Social Context
Most Western studies, particularly on parenting styles, focus on the nuclear family
and often on the child’s relationship with her mother. Joint families demonstrate
important relationship dynamics with grandparents, uncles, aunts and cousins
almost as much as it does with the parents and siblings. Even in the US,
grandmothers often get more connected into the family when their daughters are
single mothers or teenage mothers (Garcia Coll, 1990).
Children often interact with peers their own gender, creating disconnect from
the other gender. This may carry forward later into adolescence and adulthood as
these individuals are better socialised to and develop skills in interacting with
members of their own sex and not enough skills for opposite sex interactions
(Hanish & Fabes, 2014). A subsystem of peer relations is friendship. Youniss
and Smollar (1989) theorised that close friendships serve functional benefits by
facilitating the acquisition of social competencies such as interpersonal sensitivity,
reciprocity, cooperation, and negotiation that are congruent to the culture.
59
Self In Social Context Salman Akhtar (2009) investigated friendships of immigrant children in Western
countries, providing important insights into the acculturation process. Homo-
ethnic friendships (having friends of one’s own ethnic group) serve maternal
function of pacifying the individual but, on the negative side, impede one’s
individuation. Hetero-ethnic friendships (having friends belonging to ethnic
groups other than one’s own) play paternal role by bolstering the process of
acculturation but tend to lack earnest affective connections. Having exclusively
hetero-ethnic friends, or homo-ethnic friends results into slower psychological
development of the person.
4.3.2.4 Education
Recall that we mentioned about the positive effects of siblings on child
development being conditional, depending on whether there was a school in the
locality. Formal education and informal education become pivotal in the
assimilation of individuals into the society by teaching culturally appropriate
skills and values. John Dewey (1899, 1916) delineated the following roles of
education relevant to the society:
i) Culture Transmission
ii) Minimising inequality
iii) Social adaptability and social change
iv) Acquisition of new knowledge
v) Personal development
Cross-national studies of mathematics achievement and abilities show significant
differences in the same. Geary (1996) assert that secondary, not primary,
mathematical abilities manifest these differences. This would imply that the causal
factors for such difference are cultural and social, not biological. American
students tend to make more miscalculations as compared to East Asian students
(Miura, Okamoto, Kim, Steere, & Fayol, 1993). This may be due to the language
differences in numbers, in Japanese 1 to 10 have unique labels while all numbers
henceforth are combination of these numbers (e.g. 11 is “ten-one”) while in
English, numbers 1 to 19 and decade numbers have unique labels.
Cultural differences in teaching style could also account for the differences in
mathematical and other educational abilities. It was noted that as compared to
the United States, Chinese and Japanese teachers spent greater time with students
and the students spent greater time in the school in terms of days per day and
hours per year. Some cultures majorly opt for a didactic teaching style, where
teachers provide information to the students verbally and students acquire it as
per their level of understanding and memorisation. Alternatively, other cultures
majorly have more dynamic teachers who are actively involved with students,
providing them with a platform where the students can themselves uncover
concepts and theories of the workings of the world. American teaching system
believes in praising the students on correct answers while Indian, Japanese and
Taiwanese culture focus on correcting mistakes of the students.
4.3.2.5 Religion
For long, religion and education as entities were not separate. Religious advocates
would impart values as well as education to children and educational institutes
encouraged religiosity. The religious text of Judaism in ancient Israel, Torah,
instructed and encouraged learning and literacy (Compayreì & Payne, 1899).
The schools, however, only allowed boys. In 622 AD, schools were opened in
the Islamic mosques in Medina (now in Saudi Arabia; Al-Hassani, 2011). Much
earlier, between 1500 and 600 BC, Veda and other Hindu scriptures were the
sources of education in ancient Indian that focused on teaching grammar,
composition, verses, logic and other occupational skills (Gupta, 2007). Gurukulas
were important institutions where Brahmin students studied under a Brahmin
teacher for around twelve years before returning home. While they taught many
life sustaining values, religion and its history dominated the system.
In modern time as well, depending on the level of religiosity in the culture and/
or family, religion plays a major role in socialisation. For Punjabi parents settled
in England, religious practices are important carriers of language and principles
for the next generation (Dosanjh &Ghuman, 1997). Some religions celebrate the
transition to adulthood of individuals by ceremonies such as the Bar Mitzvah in
Judaism and to adolescence by Ramadan fasting participation in Islam. Religious
belief has a strong link in moral development in Africa (Okonkwo, 1997) and
suicide attitudes for Hindus and Muslims in England (Kamal & Lowenthal, 2002),
among other linkages.
Fonerand Alba (2008) find that individuals that convert to Christianity in the
United States have positive outcomes in the acculturation process. Already
belonging to a religion of the majority where one migrates to also helped social
mobility (Cadge & Ecklund, 2007). Stronger religiosity, however, negatively
impacts assimilation in a new culture as they tend to prioritise their own cultures
(Borup & Ahlin, 2011).
3) ……………….. occurs in the culture where we are born and the process of
familiarisation to the culture begins right after the birth. 61
Self In Social Context 4) ……………….. is a dynamic system that describes the average, mainstream
and representative tendencies in a given population.
Culture does not just exist and stay limited to the society and the country we live
in, but we become carriers of it, often perceiving and internalising events and
concepts in context of our personal cultures. Our personal cultures are a part of
our self-concepts. Wehrle and Fasbender (2019) defined self-concept as an
integration of “complex, organised, and yet dynamic system of learned attitudes,
beliefs, evaluative judgments that people hold about themselves.” Sense of self
could be broadly categorised into (Markus & Kitayama, 1991):
62
Self in Social Context
Fig .4.1.: Types of culture and the sense of self they promote
63
Self In Social Context
Motivation to Desire to achieve connected to Achievement orientation
achieve personal goals of striving for related to affiliation
success. Achievement orientation, having
orientation unrelated to social goals - others’
affiliation orientation. expectations and
obligations.
How one perceives oneself (self perception) is an important product of the culture
they grow up in. Members of individualistic societies are able to view themselves
consistently in different contexts on the basis of their perceived skills and
personality traits. This becomes a more difficult task in Eastern societies, where
self perception varies with context. Cross-cultural research on self concept reflects
that Americans focused on more self-evaluative statements while Indians largely
emphasised their social identity (Dhawan, Roseman, Naidu, Komilla, & Rettek,
1995).
Sagie, Elizur, and Yamauchi (1996) found that participants from collectivist
societies, such as Japan, displayed lower personal achievement orientation as
compared to those from individualistic societies, such as Hungary. Collective
64 achievement tendencies were found to be higher in the former. The motivation
to achieve is also related to one’s personal growth in individualist societies but Self in Social Context
in collective societies it tends to have social goals, such as obligatory feelings
and filial piety. In cultures such as that of Turkey, achievement motivation follows
both social and personal elements (Phalet & Claeys, 1993).
Aaker and Williams (1998) talk about ego-focused versus other-focused emotions.
They explained that members of individualist societies (say, in the US) feel ego-
focused emotions, such as pride and anger (also termed as socially disengaged
emotions; Kitayama, Markus, and Kurokawa, 1993) more intensely. Alternatively,
members of collectivist societies (say, in Japan) feel other-focused emotions,
such as respect, friendliness (socially engaged emotions) more intensely. General
and more universal emotions could be felt with different intensities, havedifferent
expressions, and vary in the context of social acceptance across cultures. For
example, a study by Ogarkova, Soriano, and Gladkova (2016) explored anger
metaphors in English, Spanish, Russian languages. English language displayed
more intense, expressive and unmonitored version of anger, while also
demonstrating higher tendency to experience and exhibit the same as compared
to the other two languages. The causation of anger was found to be internal (or
dispositional) as compared to situational, and more socially accepted in English.
Further, indigenous emotions that are unique to collectivist societies are also
related to public facets. Such as amae (Doi, 1973) is an indigenous Japanese
emotion that refers to the dependency on authority figures and yearning for their
acceptance, benevolence, and indulgence. It is noteworthy; however, that Aaker
and Williams found that other-focused emotional appeals worked much better in
persuading members of individualist societies as compared to the use of self-
focused emotional appeals, perhaps because of their novelty. The opposite was
found in collectivist societies because of similar reasons.
66
Self Assessment Questions II Self in Social Context
Culture has much control on how members of a society perceive, bond, or interact
with each other as well as how they interact with the out-group. It would be
functional to be familiar with the terms in-group and out-group at the onset of
this section, as they would be used frequently throughout. Maslow (1968), in his
hierarchical model of needs, mentions the need for belongingness as one of the
important needs, feeling a sense of acceptance in and affiliation to a social group.
In-groups: The groups we identify with or feel that we belong to. For example,
our religious community, nation, family, choir group, football team, etc.
One generally has multiple in-groups. Some group memberships are more
important to us than that of others (Bernstein, 2015). Also, some group
memberships are salient than others and/or become more salient at a particular
time as opposed to another time. For example, stereotypically, Asians are
considered good at mathematics, while women are considered incompetent at
the same. Being conscious of one’s group stereotypes may often reinforce those
stereotypes. Shih, Pittinsky, and Ambady (1999) wanted to find how Asian women
would perform on a mathematical task when their Asian identity was made salient
as compared to when they were reminded of their gender identity. The results
indicated that making the Asian identity (the more “competent” identity) more
salient enhanced the mathematical performance of the participants, while making
the female identity more pronounced, hampered their performance. Mathematical
performance, of course, is not a social behaviour, at least in this context. However,
the example serves the function of understanding how group salience and existing
perceptions about a group could affect our behaviour depending on the situation.
67
Self In Social Context This section will take a gander at how culture can affect behaviour in the social
milieu, exploring the topics of group interaction dynamics, person perceptions,
individualist-collectivist differences, attribution, aggression, and close
relationships.
As indicated earlier in the Unit, collectivist cultures drew starker contrasts between
their ingroup and outgroup members, and also experience greater intimacy with
ingroup members as compared to their individualist counterparts (Triandis, 1988).
The negative effect of this is reflected in the difficulties in communicating with
outgroup members or strangers in collectivist cultures, such as Japan and Korea,
and compared to that in individualist cultures, such as the United States
(Gudykunst, Yoon, & Nishida, 1987). Also, the personalisation of communication
with outgroups in collectivist cultures depends highly on situational factors, while
situational demands do not play as important a role in individualist cultures
pertaining to the same concern.
Attachment theory and styles by Ainsworth et. al. (1978) has been studied in
group context (e.g. Rom & Mikulincer, 2003; DeMarco & Newheiser, 2018). In
simple terms, group attachment anxiety is related to the insecurity of not being
accepted in the in-group, while group attachment avoidance is characterised by
trying to refraining from depending on their in-groups (in spite of feeling
belongingness to the group). Anxious group member, hence, tend to manifest
behaviours that would increase intimacy with their in-group, while avoidant group
members prefer to maintain their distance from the in-group (Smith et. al., 1999).
Behaviours that increase intimacy with the group may include (as defined in
Matsumoto & Juang, 2008):
Conformity: adhering to real or perceived social pressure.
Compliance: explicitly (in behaviours manifested publicly) adhering to social
pressure, although private beliefs may remain unchanged.
While many previous studies suggest that recategorising small groups into larger
groups (for example, girls football team and boys football team into one school
football team) would reduce intergroup bias (e.g., Gaertner et al., 1990), newer
studies suggest the opposite effect (e.g., Hornsey & Hogg, 2000; Turner, & Crisp,
2010). Turner and Crisp confirmed that strong in-group identification would
predict intergroup bias after recategorisation into broader groups. They propose
that the reason for this phenomenon could be an individual’s need to distinguish
oneself and do so by belonging to a positively valued group or viewing the group
positively. If one’s group is merged which other groups that are equivalent on
some important factors, it would trigger the need for positive distinction further,
thus, aggravating the intergroup conflict (Brown & Wade, 1987). This might be
the reason why fascism is described a radical embodiment of nationalism, where
(extreme) nationalism facilitates racism and violence (Turner, 1975, Peters, 2018).
4.5.3 Attribution
Humans have a tendency to find reasons and explanations for their own and
others’ personalities/behaviours and events in their lives or in general. Such
explanations are referred to as attributions. This could explain people’s belief in
astrology and the pleasure we derive from buzz feed personality quizzes. You
could attribute your failure in examinations to the strict grading of the examiner
or to the fever you had that made you underperform. These attributions could be
true. You could attribute your car crashing into the vehicle in front to the other
driver’s sudden braking although it could be due to your own inability to maintain
a distance from the vehicle in front. It could be one of the reasons why we hesitate
to accept self-driving cars because in case of accidents, blaming someone gets
overly complicated – you cannot have a verbal road rage battle with a machine
as well as you can with an equally angry human driver.
69
Self In Social Context
Box 4.1: Errors of Attribution
People tend to attribute one’s own negative behaviours (or failures) to
external factors and positive behaviours (or successes) to internal factors,
i.e. self-serving bias (Bradley, 1978; “I was late to the office because the
traffic was unpredictable heavy”). On the other hand, they attribute most of
others’ behaviours to internal factors, i.e. Fundamental attribution error
(FAE; Jones & Nisbett, 1971; “he was late to the office because he is a lazy
person who does not take his career seriously”).
4.5.4 Aggression
Aggression is an overt expression of anger through behaviours that inflict physical
or psychological harm to another person. Besides genetic factors, environmental
and cultural factors have influence on the overall perception, experience, and
expression of aggression in a culture. In Finland, aggression is viewed as
something one does to gain pleasure and, hence, it is considered more deviant as
compared to Estonia, where aggression is considered a more normal means to
achieve a goal (Terav & Keltikangas, 1998). Further, aggression is more socially
acceptable and considered normal in Hong Kong (as compared to the United
States), when there is a difference in authority levels of two people (Bond, Wan,
Leong, & Giacalone, 1985).
ii) Economic situation. Wealthier societies tend to have lower rates of homicides
(Lim et al., 2005). More importantly, economic inequality, as compared to a
country’s wealth, is a better predictor of homicide rates (Kennedy, Kawachi,
& Prothrow-Stith, 1996; Lim et. al., 2005). Hence, equal distribution of
wealth and resources are important for controlling aggression and violence
in a society.
iii) War. Involvement in war and other violent political feuds creates and
environment of tension and aggression within a society. Countries that were
involved in World War II (combatant countries), had higher homicide rates
after the war was over, as compared to those that were not involved in the
war (combatant countries; Archer and Gartner; 1984). Hence, being involved
in an international conflict has a deep, negative impact on the internal
functioning of a country.
Facial recognition studies indicate that individuals can more accurately recognise
the face of people of their own ethnicities than others (e.g., Ng & Lindsay, 1994;
Bothwell, Brigham, &Malpass, 1989). One of the explanations for this could be
intergroup contact (or lack thereof), individuals tend to spend more time around
people of their own race than others, hence getting used to and better distinguishers
of facial features typical of that race.
Interpersonal attraction, love and relationships have been other interesting areas
of study in cross-cultural research. Croucher, Austin, Fang, and Holody (2011)
explored interpersonal attraction of Hindus and Muslims in India, and found that
both the groups displayed greater attraction (in the physical, social, and task
domains) towards their own religious group than for the other.
Attitudes about love were compared between the United States, Japan, and France
71
Self In Social Context by Ting-Toomey (1991). It was found that love commitment and disclosure
maintenance were rated much higher by American and French participants as
compared to Japanese participants; and the Americans rated relational
ambivalence higher than the Japanese. The domain of conflict expression was
rated higher by Japanese and American participants over French subjects.
Although people tend to be attracted to their in-groups, much of the recent work
demonstrates the benefits of developing close relationships with members of a
different culture. For example, Lu and colleagues (2017) conducted a series of
studies on the effect of intercultural relationships on creativity. Non-Americans
who had worked in the US under J-1 visas who stayed in touch with their American
friends were more innovative and had higher chances of becoming entrepreneurs.
More importantly, people with intercultural dating experience had significantly
higher levels of creativity than those with exclusive intracultural dating
experience. Thus, long-term intercultural friendships and a history of intercultural
romantic relationship have significant positive impact on people.
2) Self - serving bias is an attribution error in which people blame victims for
their suffering. ( )
3) Social identity theory (Tajfel, 1978) is based on the supposition that building
identity in relation to social groups also strengthens and preserves one’s
self identity. ( )
5) The groups we identify with or feel that we belong to are called ‘out- groups’.
( )
4.8 GLOSSARY
Acculturation: adapting to and/or adopting a culture different from the one in
which that person was originally enculturated.
Aggression: overt expression of anger through behaviours that inflicts physical
or psychological harm to another person.
Attribution: evaluative judgment to ascribe the reason/explanation for a particular
event or behaviour as being caused or controlled by another person or situational
factors.
Co-sleeping: when young children and their parents sleep in the same room.
Collectivism: political or cultural ideology that focuses on interdependent self
and “fitting in” with the group.
Compliance: explicitly (in behaviours manifested publicly) adhering to social
pressure, although private beliefs may remain unchanged.
Conformity: adhering to real or perceived social pressure.
Cooperation:group members’ potential to work together to achieve a common
goal.
Culture: totality of equivalent and complementary learned meanings maintained
by a human population, or by identifiable segments of a population, and
transmitted from one generation to the next.
Defensive attribution: tendency to blame victims for their suffering.
Enculturation: transmission of aspects of our own culture from one generation
to the next by different agencies of the culture.
Fascism: an anti-democratic political ideology that encourages extreme
nationalism, denying fundamental rights to individuals of the out-group or that
are considered deviants. 73
Self In Social Context Fundamental attribution error: tendency to attribute others’ behaviours to
internal factors.
Group attachment anxiety: insecurity of not being accepted in the in-group.
Group attachment avoidance: refraining from depending on the in-group.
Hetero-ethnic friendships: Friendships with peers that belong to ethnic groups
different to one’s own.
Homo-ethnic friendships: Friendships with peers of one’s own ethnicity.
Identity redefinition: playing up positive attributes of the target identity so as
to create positive associations and feel better about the said identity.
Just world hypothesis: a cognitive fallacy the world is fair and “good” people
get rewarded, while only the “bad” people get punished.
Myers, D. G. & Twenge, J. M. (2017). Social Psychology (12th ed.). New York,
NY : McGraw-Hill.
References
Aaker, J. L., & Williams, P. (1998). Empathy Versus Pride: the Influence of
Emotional Appeals Across Cultures. Journal of Consumer Research, 25(3), 241–
261. doi:10.1086/209537
Ainsworth, M., Blehar, M., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of Attachment:
A Psychological Study of the Strange Situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Berry, J. W., Poortinga, Y. H., Segall, M. H., &Dasen, P. R.(1992). Cross- Cultural
Psychology: Research and Applications.New York: Cambridge University Press.
Borup, J., &Ahlin, L. (2011). Religion and Cultural Integration. Nordic Journal
of Migration Research,1(3). doi:10.2478/v10202-011-0015-z
Brown, R. J., & Wade, G. (1987). Superordinate goals and intergroup behaviour-
the effect of role ambiguity and status on intergroup attitudes and task-
performance. European Journal of Social Psychology, 17, 131-142.
Cadge, W., & Howard Ecklund, E. (2007). Immigration and Religion. Annual
Review of Sociology, 33(1), 359-379.doi:10.1146/annurev.soc.33.040406.131707.
Compayreì, G., & Payne, W. H. (1899). The History of Pedagogy. Boston: D.C.
Heath & Co.
Conroy, M., Hess, D. R., Azuma, H., &Kashiwagi, K.(1980). Maternal strategies
for regulating children’s behavior. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 11(2),
153-172.
Dewey, J. (1899). The School and Society: Being three lectures. Chicago: The
University of Chicago Press.
Dhawan, N., Roseman, I. J., Naidu, R. K., Komilla, T., &Rettek, S. I. (1995).
Self-concepts across two cultures.Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 26(6),
606–621.
Doi, T. (1973). The Anatomy of Dependence. Tokyo:Kodansha.
Dosanjh, J. S., &Ghuman, P. A. S. (1996). The cultural contextof child rearing: A
study of indigenous and BritishPunjabis. Early Child Development and Care,
126, 39–55.
Ernst, C, & Angst, J. (1983). Birth Order. New York: Springer-Verlag
Foner, N. and Alba, R. (2008), Immigrant Religion in the U.S. and Western Europe:
Bridge or Barrier to Inclusion? International Migration Review, 42: 360-392.
doi: 10.1111/j.1747-7379.2008.00128.x
Gaertner, S. L., Mann, J. A., Dovidio, J. F., Murrell, A. J., &Pomare, M. (1990).
How does cooperation reduce intergroup bias? Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 59, 692–704.
Ghosh, R. (1983). Sarees and the maple leaf: Indian women in Canada. In Kurian,
G., and Srivastava, R. P. (1983). Overseas Indians: A study in adaptation (pp.
90-99). New Delhi: Vikas.
Isaac, R., Annie, I. K., & Prashanth, H. R. (n.d.). Parenting in India. In H. Selin
(Ed.), Parenting Across Cultures (Vol. 7, pp. 39-45). Springer Netherlands.
doi:10.1007/978-94-007-7503-9.
Garcia Coll, C. T., Sepkoski, C., & Lester, B. M. (1981). Culturaland biomedical
correlates of neonatal behavior. Developmental Psychobiology, 14, 147–154.
Gudykunst, W. B., Yoon, Y., & Nishida, T. (1987). The influence of individualism
collectivism on perceptions of communication in ingroup and outgroup
relationships. Communication Monographs, 54(3), 295-306. doi:10.1080/
03637758709390234.
Hanish L.D. &Fabes R.A. (2014) Peer socialization of gender in young boys and
girls. In: Tremblay R.E., Boivin M., & Peters R.DeV. (Eds). Martin C.L., (Section
Ed). Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development. http://www.child-
encyclopedia.com/gender-early-socialization/according-experts/peer-
socialization-gender-young-boys-and-girls.
Hong, Y., Zhan, S., Morris, M. W., & Benet-Martínez, V. (2016). Multicultural
identity processes. Current Opinion in Psychology, 8, 49–53. doi:10.1016/
j.copsyc.2015.09.020
78
Hornsey, M. J., & Hogg, M. A. (2000). Subgroup relations: A comparison of Self in Social Context
mutual intergroup differentiation and common ingroup identity models of
prejudice reduction. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 242-256.
Jones, E. E., & Nisbett, R. E. (1971). The actor and the observer:Divergent
perceptions of the causes of behavior.In E. E. Jones, D. E. Kanouse, H. H. Kelley,
R. E. Nisbett, S. Valins, & B. Weiner (Eds.), Attribution: Perceivingthe Causes
of Behavior (pp. 79–94) Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.
Kamal, Z., & Lowenthal, K. M. (2002). Suicide beliefs andbehavior among young
Muslims and Indians in the UK. Mental Health, Religion and Culture, 5(2), 111–
118.
79
Self In Social Context Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., Kurokawa, M., &Negishi, K.(1993). Social
orientation of Emotions: Cross-cultural Evidenceand Implications. Unpublished
manuscript, Universityof Oregon.
LeVine, R. A., LeVine, S. E., Dixon, S., Richman, A.,Leiderman, P. H., & Keefer,
C. (1996). Child Care and Culture: Lessons from Africa. Cambridge, UK:
CambridgeUniversity Press.
Lim, F., Bond, M. H., & Bond, M. K. (2005). Linking Societal and Psychological
Factors to Homicide Rates Across Nations. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology,
36(5), 515–536. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022105278540
Lu, J. G., Hafenbrack, A. C., Eastwick, P. W., Wang, D. J., Maddux, W. W., &
Galinsky, A. D. (2017). “Going out” of the box: Close intercultural friendships
and romantic relationships spark creativity, workplace innovation, and
entrepreneurship. Journal of Applied Psychology, 102(7), 1091–1108.
doi:10.1037/apl0000212
McEvoy, M., Lee, C., O’Neill, A., Groisman, A., Roberts-Butelman, K., Dinghra,
K., &Porder, K. (2005). Are there universal parenting concepts among culturally
diverse families in an inner-city pediatric clinic? Journal of Pediatric Health
Care, 19(3), 142–150.doi:10.1016/j.pedhc.2004.10.007
Miura, I. T., Okamoto, Y., Kim, C. C., Steere, M., et al.(1993). First graders’
cognitive representation of numberand understanding of place value. Cross-
nationalcomparisons: France, Japan, Korea, Sweden, and theUnited States.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 85(1),24–30.
80
Moghaddam, F. M., Ditto, B., & Taylor, D. M. (1990). Attitudesand attributions Self in Social Context
related to psychological symptomatologyin Indian immigrant women. Journal
ofCross-Cultural Psychology, 21, 335–350.
Morris, M. W., & Peng, K. (1994). Culture and cause: Americanand Chinese
attributions for social and physicalevents. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology,67(6), 949–971.
Morris, M. W., Chiu, C., & Liu, Z. (2015). Polycultural Psychology. Annual
Review of Psychology, 66(1), 631–659. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-
010814-015001
Noguchi, K., Kamada, A., &Shrira, I. (2013). Cultural differences in the primacy
effect for person perception. International Journal of Psychology, n/a–n/a.
doi:10.1002/ijop.12019
Outlook Web Bureau. (2018, April 27). People Behave Themselves Abroad, But
Once In India They Spit On, Litter Public Places: New SDMC Mayor. Outlook.
Retrieved from https://www.outlookindia.com/website/story/people-behave-
themselves-abroad-but-once-in-india-they-spit-on-litter-public-pla/311408
Phalet, K., & Claeys, W. (1993). A comparative study ofTurkish and Belgian
youth. Journal of Cross-CulturalPsychology, 24, 319–343.
Reitz, J. G. Breton, R., Dion, K.K., & Dion, K.L. (2009). Multiculturalism and
Social Cohesion: Potentials and Challenges of Diversity. London: Springer.
Rosin, H., & Spiegel, A. (2017, June 15). The Culture Inside [Audio Podcast].
Retrieved from https://www.npr.org/programs/invisibilia/532950995/the-culture-
inside
Sagie, A., Elizur, D., & Yamauchi, H. (1996). The structure and strength of
achievement motivation: A cross-cultural comparison. Journal of Organizational
Behavior, 17(5), 431–444. doi:10.1002/(sici)1099-1379(199609)17:5<431::aid-
job771>3.0.co;2-x
Sanchez, D. T., Shih, M. J., & Wilton, L. S. (2014). Exploring the identity
autonomy perspective (IAP): An integrative theoretical approach to multicultural
and multiracial identity. In V. Benet-Martínez & Y.-Y. Hong (Eds.), Oxford library
of psychology. The Oxford handbook of Multicultural Identity (pp. 139-159).
New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press.
Sinha, D. (1988). Basic Indian values and behavior dispositions in the context of
national development. In D. Sinha & H.S.R. Kao (Eds.), Social Values and
Development: Asian Perspective. New Delhi: Sage
Sinha, J. B. P., Sinha, T. N., Verma, J., & Sinha, R. B. N. (2001). Collectivism
coexisting with individualism: an Indian scenario. Asian Journal of Social
Psychology, 4(2), 133–145. doi:10.1111/j.1467-839x.2001.00081.x
Smith, E. R., Murphy, J., & Coats, S. (1999). Attachment to groups: Theory and
management. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 94-110.doi:
10.1037/0022-3514.77.1.94
82
Stewart, S. M., Bond, M. H., Zaman, R. M., McBride-Chang,C., Rao, N., Ho, L. Self in Social Context
M., & Fielding, R. (1999). Functionalparenting in Pakistan. International Journal
of BehavioralDevelopment, 23(3), 747–770.
Suh, E. M., & Oishi, S. (2002). Subjective Well-Being Across Cultures. Online
Readings in Psychology and Culture, 10(1). http://dx.doi.org/10.9707/2307-
0919.1076
Tajfel, H., Billig, M. G., Bundy, R. P., &Flament, C. (1971). Social categorization
and intergroup behaviour. European Journal of Social Psychology, 1(2), 149-
178. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2420010202
Turner, R. N., & Crisp, R. J. (2010). Explaining the relationship between ingroup
identification and intergroup bias following recategorization: A self-regulation
theory analysis. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 13(2), 251–261.
doi:10.1177/1368430209351702
83
Self In Social Context Verkuyten, M. (2009). Self-esteem and multiculturalism: An examination among
ethnic minority and majority groups in the Netherlands. Journal of Research in
Personality, 43(3), 419-427.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2009.01.013
Ye, D., Ng, Y. K., & Lian, Y. (2014). Culture and Happiness. Social Indicators
Research, 123(2), 519-547.
84
Self in Social Context
BLOCK 3
SOCIAL COGNITION
85
Social Cognition
BLOCK 3 INTRODUCTION
This block comprises of Four units which deals with the social cognition. It
should be mentioned here that social cognition is the way by which we process
social information.The first unit deals with the concepts of schema and heuristics
and the unit explains the modes of social thought. The unit also discusses about
the sources of errors that affects our social cognition. With the help of this unit
you will come to know about the role played by our cognitive processes in social
interaction with others.
The second unit discusses about the process of social perception and describes
the ways in which we perceive others in different social situations. It will also
deal with the theories of attribution, in order to know whether the behaviour of a
person originated from his or her internal dispositions or there were some external
situational factors that caused the particular behaviour. The unit will also explain
the process by which an overall impression of others interacting with us in social
situations is formed. Lastly, we will illustrate the errors we commit in attribution
process.
In the third unit of this block, we will discuss the meaning and definition of
attitude. We will further explain the components, types and functions of attitude.
We will also describe the process and theories of attitude formation and change.
We will also discuss the issue pertaining to relationship between attitude and
behaviour. Lastly, we will also try to understand the concept, process and relevance
of persuasion.
In the fourth and last unit of this block, we will discuss the relationship between
attitude and behaviour. We will also understand the concepts of stereotype,
prejudice and discrimination. By the end of this unit, you will also come to know
about the sources of prejudice and the disguised forms of discrimination. Lastly,
you will also be acquainted by the ways of reducing stereotype, prejudice and
discrimination.
86
Social Cognition:
UNIT 5 SOCIAL COGNITION: Understanding Social
Behaviour- I
UNDERSTANDING SOCIAL
BEHAVIOUR- I*
Structure
5.0 Objectives
5.1 Introduction
5.2 Schema
5.2.1 Types of Schema
5.2.1.1 Person Schemas
5.2.1.2 Self Schemas
5.2.1.3 Group Schemas
5.2.1.4 Role Schemas
5.2.1.5 Events Schemas
5.2.2 Impact of Schema
5.3 Modes of Social Thought Processing
5.3.1 The Continuum Model of Processing
5.3.2 Automatic vs. Controlled Processing
5.4 Heuristics: The Mental Shortcuts
5.4.1 Availability Heuristics
5.4.2 Representativeness Heuristics
5.4.3 Anchoring and Adjustment Heuristics
5.5 Sources of Errors in Social Cognition
5.5.1 Cognitive-Experiential Self Theory
5.5.2 Paying Attention to Inconsistent Information
5.5.3 Negativity Bias
5.5.4 Planning Fallacy
5.5.5 Potential Costs of Thinking Too Much
5.5.6 Counterfactual thinking
5.5.7 Magical Thinking
5.6 Let Us Sum Up
5.7 Unit End Questions
5.8 Answers to Self Assessment Questions
5.9 Glossary
5.10 Suggested Readings and References
5.0 OBJECTIVES
After reading this unit, you will be able to:
Understand the meaning of social cognition;
Describe meaning, types and impact of schema;
Explain the concept and relevance of heuristics; and
Discuss the sources of errors in social cognition.
* Dr. Ari Sudan Tiwari, Scientist ‘E’Defence Institute of Psychological Research, Ministry of Defence,
Lucknow Road, Timarpur, Delhi 87
Social Cognition
5.1 INTRODUCTION
Human beings are social animals. They think, feel and act by involving
themselves, others and larger collectives throughout every moment of the day.
The enormous yet seemingly natural tasks of social perception, social memory,
and social decision-making in which they engage; and the by-products of such
tasks constitute the study of social cognition. Social cognition is defined as the
process by which we interpret, analyze, remember and use information about
the social world. In the other words, social cognition is the way by which we
process social information. More specifically, while studying social cognition
social psychologists attempt to answer following important questions of social
lives of human being:
5.2 SCHEMA
Our social interactions are largely guided by our expectations regarding the people
involved in the interactions, roles played by them in the specific situations, norms
guiding behaviours of people involved in the interaction and the likely events
and actions in the situation. Such expectations originate from our previous
experiences and knowledge of people, roles, norms and events of similar kinds.
Social psychologists refer it as schemas. Schemas are defined as cognitive
structures containing broader expectations and knowledge of the social world
that help us systematically organise social information.
Schemas contain not only some precise and explicit illustrations, they also include
our inferences and assumptions about of the persons, events, situations, etc.
Schemas help us to predict the likely behaviours of people occupying specific
roles in a social interaction and sequence of actions in a particular social event.
Further, schemas influence the process of encoding, storage and retrieval of social
information.They also guide us in making inferences about the information which
is not available to us in a particular social situation. By all its functions, a schema
significantly reduces the efforts weput forth in processing the social information.
88
5.2.1 Types of Schema Social Cognition:
Understanding Social
Behaviour- I
Social psychologists have categorised schemas into different types: person
schemas, self-schemas, group schemas, role schemas and event schemas.
Schemas are integrated and stable in nature: Schemas are developed and
strengthened with our experiences in particular social situations and further they
are stored in the form of integrated structures of associated components. During
our social interactions even a single accessed component of a schema is capable
of activating the whole schema, as strong associative links exist among the
components of the schema (Fiske & Dyer, 1985). Once schemas are developed
and are recurrently activated during our social encounters they become relatively
stable part of our social thought process and further they resist change even
when we are encountered with the evidences inconsistent with the existing
schemas.
92
5.4.1 Availability Heuristics Social Cognition:
Understanding Social
Behaviour- I
Some schemas are more frequently used in our social interactions than others. A
schema which is most recently used is more readily available to us to guide our
social interactions. Schwarz et al. (1991) proposed a different explanation to
availability heuristics in terms of ease of retrieval. They argued that schemas
consistent with the examples which are easier to remember are more readily
available and therefore, used in our social thoughts. Thus they emphasised the
ease of remembering a particular example associated with certain schema than
the number of times the schema is used.
Suppose that you are asked in an exam to provide the population of Delhi. If you
do not know that population but you know the population of Haryana, you might
use the population of Haryana as an anchor and thinking that Delhi must be
somewhat smaller than Haryana, adjust the population of Haryana downward to
produce your guess. In most cases of social judgements, we generally use
ourselves as an anchor.
94
5.5.6 Counterfactual Thinking Social Cognition:
Understanding Social
Behaviour- I
Counterfactual thinking is a tendency in which people think contrary to what
actually occurred. People think about the already occurred events by framing
some possible alternatives in terms of “What if?” and the “If I had only...” For
example, a cricketer thinks that “what could have happened if played in that
match!”
5.9 GLOSSARY
Social cognition: The process by which we interpret, analyze, remember and
use information about the social world.
Self schemas: Cognitive representations about us that organize and process all
related information.
Group schemas: Also called stereotypes, are schemas regarding the members
of a particular social group or social category and indicate that certain attributes
and behaviours are typical of members of that group or social category.
96
Role schemas: Indicate that certain attributes and behaviours are typical of Social Cognition:
Understanding Social
persons occupying a particular role in a group and are often used to understand Behaviour- I
and to predict the behaviours of people who occupy roles.
Branscombe, N. R., & Baron, R. A. (2016). Social Psychology (14th ed.). Boston:
Pearson/Allyn& Bacon.
Dreben, E. K., Fiske, S. T., & Hastie, R. (1979). The independence of evaluative
and item information: Impression and recall order effects in behavior-based
impression formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1758-
1768.
Fiske, S. T., & Dyer, L. M. (1985). Structure and development of social schemata:
Evidence from positive and negative transfer effects. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 48, 839-852.
Jones, E. E., & Nisbett, R. (1972). The actor and observer: Divergent perceptions
of the causes of behavior. In E. E. Jones, D. E. Kanouse, H. H. Kelley, R. E.
Nisbett, S. Valins, & B. W. Weiner (Eds.), Attribution: Perceiving the Causes of
Behavior. Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.
Nisbett, R. E., Caputo, C., Legant, P., &Maracek, J. (1973). Behavior as seen by
the actor and as seen by the observer. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 27, 154-164.
Wilson, T. D., & Schooler, J. W. (1991). Thinking Too Much: Introspection Can
Reduce the Quality of Preferences and Decisions. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 60, 181-192.
99
Social Cognition
UNIT 6 SOCIAL COGNITION:
UNDERSTANDING SOCIAL
BEHAVIOUR- II*
Structure
6.0 Objectives
6.1 Introduction
6.2 Person Perception
6.2.1 Impression Formation
6.2.1.1 Trait Centrality
6.2.1.2 First Impression
6.3 Theories of Attribution
6.3.1 Heider’s Naive Psychology
6.3.2 Correspondent Inference Theory
6.3.3 Covariation Model
6.3.4 Attribution of Success and Failure
6.4 Errors and Biases in Attribution
6.4.1 Fundamental Attribution Error
6.4.2 Actor-Observer Bias
6.4.3 Self-Serving Bias
6.4.4 Ultimate Attribution Error
6.5 Let Us Sum Up
6.6 Unit End Questions
6.7 Answers to Self Assessment Questions
6.8 Glossary
6.9 Suggested Readings and References
6.0 OBJECTIVES
After reading this unit, you will be able to:
Explain the concept of person perception and impression formation;
Discuss the various theories of attribution;
Illustrate the process of impression formation; and
Elucidate the ways in which people generally commit errors in person
perception.
6.1 INTRODUCTION
The present unit of the block will explain the process of social perception and
describe the ways in which we perceive others in different social situations. We
will also illustrate the theories of attribution in order to know whether the
behaviour of a person originated from his or her internal dispositions or there
were some external situational factors that caused the particular behaviour. We
will also understand the process by which an overall impression of others
* Dr. Ari Sudan Tiwari, Scientist ‘E’Defence Institute of Psychological Research, Ministry of Defence,
100 Lucknow Road, Timarpur Delhi-110054
interacting with us in social situations is formed. Lastly, we will illustrate the Social Cognition:
Understanding Social
errors we commit in attribution process. Behaviour- II
Asch (1946) presented empirical evidence to the view that when we form
impression of a person some traits play more important role than others. Asch
(1946) presented a list of traits of an imaginary person to one of his two research
101
Social Cognition groups. The list included seven traits: intelligent, skilful, industrious, warm,
determined, practical and cautious. The list which was presented to the second
research group differed in the manner that the trait “cold” replaced the trait
“warm”. After being presented with the list of traits, participants of both the
research groups were asked to write a short description of the impression they
formed of the imaginary person and also to rate the person on another list of
characteristics: generous, wise, happy, good-natured, humorous, sociable, popular,
humane, altruistic and imaginative.
The findings revealed that when the traits “warm” and “cold” shaped the overall
impression formed by the research participants to a great extent. In the “warm”
trait condition, the imaginary person was evaluated as happy, successful, popular
and humorous. While in “cold” trait condition, he was perceived as self-centred,
unsociable and unhappy. Furthermore, a considerable qualitative difference was
observed in the overall impression of the imaginary person as written in the
description of the person by both “warm” and “cold” trait groups. This evidence
was substantiated when the basic research procedure was replicated with a minor
innovative change that in the list of traits the trait of “warm-cold” was replaced
by “polite-blunt”. Results indicated that difference in the impression formed in
polite vs. blunt trait conditions was significantly lesser than that of warm vs.
cold. The findings suggested that different traits vary in their centrality value in
order to form impression of others. The trait which has greater influence on
overall impression is considered to have higher trait centrality value.
2) Person perception is the area of social psychology which studies the process
by which we form ......................... of other people with whom we interact
in ourreal or virtual social surroundings.
Fritz Heider (1944, 1958) proposed that in regular social interactions people try
to find out the causes behind the behaviour of other people by using commonsense
reasoning. The process and method of finding out the causes of behaviour is
performed as”naive scientists” and is similar to the scientific method.Therefore,
Heider argued that in order to understand the process by which people do causal
attribution social psychologists are required to focus on commonsense reasoning
employed by common people.
Heider proposed that while doing causal attribution, people are primarily focused
on understanding whether the behaviour is attributed to the person’s internal
state, referred to as dispositional attribution; or to the environmental factors,
referred to as situational attribution. For example, attributing a person’s aggressive
behaviour to his or her internal states or characteristics, such as irritability, bad
temper, hostility is an instance of dispositional attribution. On the other hand,
judging the aggressive behaviour originating from the situational factors, such
as being aggressive under provocation; refers to situational attribution. As a
perceiver, our decision to attribute behaviour to the personal dispositions or to
the situational factors is based on our evaluation of the strength of situational
pressures on the actor. Under strong situational pressure, we generally go with
situational attribution.
BEHAVIOUR THAT IS
Thus, correspondent inference theory states that we are most likely to conclude
that others’ behaviour reflects their stable traits and dispositional factors (i.e.,
we are likely to reach correspondent inferences about them), when that behaviour
is freely chosen, yields distinctive, non-common effects and is low in social
desirability.
105
Social Cognition Suppose, while going toward your office you notice a road accident. There may
be at least two potential causes to which the accident may be attributed: internal
causes (personal attributes of the person involved in the accident, such as rough
driving), external causes (abrupt driving by others, sudden exposure to damaged
road). Kelley (1967) proposed that while employing the principle of covariation
to determine whether the behaviour was caused by the internal causes or external
causes, people focus on three types of information: consensus, consistency and
distinctiveness.
Consensus is the extent to which people react to a given stimulus or event in the
same manner. It refers to whether all persons behave in the same way or only a
few people behave in that way. For example, whether all persons driving on that
side of road meet an accident (high consensus), or is that person only who has
encountered with an accident while driving on that side of road (low consensus)?
Consistency refers to the extent to which the person behaves in the same way at
different occasions and situations. If theperson meets an accident on many
different occasions, his/her behaviour is (similar) highin consistency. If s/he has
been never met a road accident earlier, his/her behaviour is low in consistency.
The causal attribution for the behaviour depends on the particular combination
of consensus, consistency and distinctiveness information that people associate
106
with that behaviour. People usually attribute a behaviour to the internal causes Social Cognition:
Understanding Social
(personal characteristics of the person, the driver) when the behaviour is low in Behaviour- II
consensus, low in distinctiveness and high in consistency. In contrast, people
usually attribute a behaviour to the external causes (rough driving by other drivers,
the context/damaged road) when the behaviour is high in consensus, high in
distinctiveness and high in consistency.
In order to decide that which of these four factors was the actual reason behind
the success or failure, perceivers firstly determine the locus of control of the
success or failure. That is, whether the reason of success or failure was within
the actor (internal or dispositional attribution) or it was caused by some
environmental factors (external or situational attribution). Secondly, the perceiver
determines the degree of stability of the success or failure. That is, whether the
reason behind the result was an enduring characteristic of the actor/environment
(stable) or it was varying (unstable). The perceiver can make a final attribution
of success or failure only after deciding the internality-externality and stability-
instability aspects of the causes.
Weiner (1986) proposed that the four factors of success or failure can be arranged
in the form of a matrix along the dimensions of internality-externality and stability-
instability of the causes. For example, ability is usually considered as an internal
and stable factor. Ability is primarily interpreted as an internalcharacteristic of
the individual and it is considered as a stable property which does not
variesquickly. On the contrary, effort is an internal and unstable property. Effort
is exerted by the individual (internal) and also, the same individual may exert
different amount of efforts at different occasions and at different tasks (unstable).
Task difficulty is an objective characteristic of the task (external) that remains
constant for a particular task (stable). Luck or chance is an external and unstable
factor.
Performance of a person is attributed to internal or external causes after comparing
his or her performance with that of others. Extraordinary performances, regardless
of good or bad, are generally attributed to internal causes. We are more likely to 107
Social Cognition evaluate a student as exceedingly able or extremely motivated who secures very
high grades in an extraordinarily tough examination. Likewise, a student with
unusually poor performance is perceived as weak in ability or very low in
motivational aspect. On the contrary, an average performance is generally
attributed to external causes. A mediocre performance of a student in an
examination is attributed either to the tough competition or to misfortune.
While being asked to evaluate the true attitude of the essay writer towards the
Castro government in Cuba, the participants viewed the writer’s attitude consistent
with the opinions expressed in the essay, regardless of the condition that the
writer had choice to take his or her position in the essay (choice condition) or not
(no-choicecondition). Experimenters further reported that although the research
participants didnot completely ignored the fact that the writers of no-choice
condition were assigned the position to take, they attached less importanceto it
and overestimated the attitudinal disposition of the essay writer. Thus, the impact
of the no-choice condition (situational or external factor) was under estimated
and the choice component (dispositional or internal factor) was over
estimated.This error results from a failure by the observer to fully apply the
108 subtractive rule.
6.4.2 Actor-Observer Bias Social Cognition:
Understanding Social
Behaviour- II
Actor-observer bias refers to the tendency to attribute other’s behaviour to internal/
dispositional factors, while attributing our own behaviour to situational/
environmental factors (Jones & Nisbett, 1972). For example, a student who fails
in an examination justifies his or her result to tough question paper, very strict
evaluation, not getting sufficient time for preparation, some sudden engagements
in family, etc. However, he or she explains similar results of other students by
lack of their ability, carelessness, indiscipline, etc. It has been observed that in
clinical settings the clinical practitioners tend to view their clients’ problem related
to their internal stable dispositions, while the clients justify their problems by
the situational factors.
Arguably, actors and observers view each others’ performances with distinct
perspectives. When we are actors, we are not able to see our own behaviours.
Rather the situational factors influencing our behaviour are more readily noticed.
However, when we are an observer the person’s behaviour is more prominent
than the environmental and contextual factors influencing the behaviour. Such
differential perspective of actors and observers lead to situational attributions
for actors and dispositional attributions for observers.
5) Recency
Self Assessment Questions II
1) False
2) True
3) False
4) True
5) True
6.8 GLOSSARY
Impression Formation: The process by which we organise diverse information
into a unified coherent manner to form an impression of the other person.
Trait Centrality: Phenomenon that some traits of a person are weighted more
heavily and have large impact than others on the overall impression we form of
that person.
Recency Effect: The most recent information we receive exerts the strongest
influence on the impressions we form of others.
Consensus: The extent to which other people react to a given stimulus or event
in the same manner as the person we are considering.
Consistency: Whether the person behaves in the same way at different times
and in different settings.
Distinctiveness: The extent to which the person reacts in the same manner to
other, different stimuli or events.
Attribution of Success and Failure: The process to find out causes of success
and failure of ours and that of others.
Self-Serving Bias: Tendency to accept credit for success and deny responsibility
for failure.
Ultimate Attribution Error: Tendency to make attributions that protect the group
we belong to.
Branscombe, N. R., & Baron, R. A. (2016). Social Psychology (14th ed.). Boston:
Pearson/Allyn& Bacon.
Dreben, E. K., Fiske, S. T., & Hastie, R. (1979). The independence of evaluative
and item information: Impression and recall order effects in behavior-based
impression formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1758-
1768.
Fiske, S. T., & Dyer, L. M. (1985). Structure and development of social schemata:
Evidence from positive and negative transfer effects. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 48, 839-852.
Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1991). Social Cognition (2nd ed.). New York:
McGraw-Hill.
Jones, E. E., & Nisbett, R. (1972). The actor and observer: Divergent perceptions
of the causes of behavior. In E. E. Jones, D. E. Kanouse, H. H. Kelley, R. E.
Nisbett, S. Valins, & B. W. Weiner (Eds.), Attribution: Perceiving the Causes of
Behavior. Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.
113
Social Cognition Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgement under uncertainty: Heuristics
and biases. Science, 185, 1124-1131.
Wilson, T. D., & Schooler, J. W. (1991). Thinking Too Much: Introspection Can
Reduce the Quality of Preferences and Decisions. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 60, 181-192.
114
Social Cognition:
UNIT 7 ATTITUDE AND ATTITUDE Understanding Social
Behaviour- II
CHANGE*
Structure
7.0 Objectives
7.1 Introduction
7.2 Meaning and Definition of Attitudes
7.3 Structure of Attitudes
7.4 Types of Attitudes
7.5 Functions of Attitudes
7.6 Attitude Formation
7.6.1 Mere Exposure
7.6.2 Personal Experience
7.6.3 Classical Conditioning
7.6.4 Operant Conditioning
7.6.5 Observational Learning
7.6.6 Genetic Factors
7.7 Attitude Change
7.7.1 Balance Theory
7.7.2 Cognitive Dissonance Theory
7.7.2.1 Forced Compliance Behaviour
7.7.2.2 Decision Making and Cognitive Dissonance
7.7.2.3 Effort Justification
7.7.3 Persuasion
7.7.3.1 Dual Process Models of Persuasion
7.7.3.2 Factors Affecting Persuasion
7.7.3.3 Resistance to Persuasion
7.8 Relationship between Attitude and Behaviour
7.8.1 Attitude Specificity
7.8.2 Attitude Accessibility
7.8.3 Self Awareness
7.8.4 Attitude Certainty
7.8.5 Attitude Strenght
7.9 Stereotype, Prejudice and Discrimination
7.9.1 Stereotype
7.9.2 Prejudice
7.9.2.1 Sources of Prejudice
7.9.3 Discrimination
7.9.4 Reducing Stereotype, Prejudice and Discrimination
7.9.4.1 Social Learning Approach
7.9.4.2 Increased Intergroup Contact
7.9.4.3 Recategorisation: Developing Common Social Identity
7.9.4.4 Feeling of Guilt Originated from Prejudice
7.9.4.5 Learning to Negate Stereotypes
7.10 Social Distance
7.11 Measurement of Attitude
7.12 Let Us Sum Up
* Dr. Ari Sudan Tiwari, Scientist ‘E’Defence Institute of Psychological Research, Ministry of Defence,
Lucknow Road, Timarpur, Delhi 115
Social Cognition 7.13 Unit End Questions
7.14 Glossary
7.15 Answers to Self Assessment Questions
7.16 Suggested Readings and References
7.0 OBJECTIVES
After reading this unit, you will be able to:
Explain the meaning of attitude;
Describe components, types and functions of attitudes;
Elucidate the process of attitude formation; and
Discuss the process of attitude change.
7.1 INTRODUCTION
Attitude has been a core issue of study in social psychology since its inception.
Attitude is generally used to explain our feelings, thoughts and behaviours for
other people, issues, events, situations, etc. In our everyday life also, attitude has
been one of the most used word. Often we say that:
“I do/don’t like Rohan.”
“I have positive or negative feelings and ideas about dogs as a pet.”
“Anand, as a colleague, has an attitude problem.”
“I favour capital punishment for the sexual offences.”
“I favour atomic non-proliferation at the global level.”
All these statements refer to some or other aspects of attitude. In this unit we will
understand the meaning and definition of attitude. We will further explain the
components, types and functions of attitude. We will also describe the process
and theories of attitude formation and change. We will also discuss the issue
pertaining to relationship between attitude and behaviour. Lastly, we will also
understand the concept, process and relevance of persuasion.
However, there are many other attitudes that function at the unconscious level.
These attitudes are called as implicit attitude. Contrary to the explicit attitudes,
implicit attitudesare under control of automatically activated evaluation and are
executed in behaviour without the awareness of the person holding that attitude.
Thus, implicit attitudes automatically affect behaviours, without conscious thought
and below the level of awareness.
Generally, this means that familiarity, in fact, may not breed contempt. Familiar
faces, ideas and slogans become comfortable old friends. This mechanism is
explicitly evident in the advertisement slogans. Repeated exposure to these slogans
leads us to like the advertised item. This liking is further translated into buying
behaviour. However, studies have shown that the mere exposure effect is most
powerful when it occurs randomly over time and too many exposures actually
may decrease the effect (Bornstein, 1989). Bornstein (1989) further argued that
repeated exposure increases liking when the stimuli are initially neutral or positive.
Whereas, repeated exposure to initially negative stimuli may increase the negative
emotion.
Not only such negative emotions, but positive emotions and likings can also be
developed through this process. Advertisers repeatedly present their brands
associated with those celebrities who are thought to induce positive emotion
among the target audience. Assumptions of classical conditioning suggest that
this leads to liking of that brand which was initially neutral and was consistently
paired with a positive stimulus. Many researchers have further demonstrated
that attitudes can be formed through the mechanism of classical conditioning
even by the exposure to the stimuli that are below the threshold of individual’s
conscious awareness, known as subliminal conditioning (Krosnick, Betz, Jussim,
& Lynn, 1992).
In the process of socialisation, parents give rewards to their children in the form
of verbal praise or candies when they express right views. Similarly, children
receive punishments, as well, when they show wrong attitudes. Our parents,
basic family, peers, school, workplace colleagues, etc. are those people and
institutions whose even soft rewards (praise) and punishments (scolding or
neglect) matter a lot to us. When we become members of these groups we learn
to express attitudes similar to those held by them in order to maximise our rewards
and minimise punishments from them. Most of our basic religious and political
120 attitudes are formed in this way. When we become member of a new social
network many a times our old attitudes do not correspond to the attitudes of the Attitude And Attitude Change
new group. Therefore, in order to fulfil the desire to fit in with others in the new
group and get reward for holding the same attitudes we tend to change the old
attitude and form the new attitude similar to the newly joined group (Levitan &
Visser, 2008).
Balance theory proposes that since balanced states are favoured over unbalanced
states, people are motivated to change an unbalanced state to a balanced state.
Heider argued that by this way people restore consistency in the relationships.
Heider (1958) demonstrated this in a study where participants were exposed to
the triads showing that “Jim doesn’t like Bob, but he likes the poem that Bob
wrote”. Responding to the situation, about 80% of participants felt the requirement
of some change in the relationships. Majority of participants suggested Jim to
change the sentiment relationship with Bob. About one third participants suggested
Jim to change attitude toward the poem. About 5% suggested a change in the
unit relationship between Bob and the poem stating that the poem was actually
not written by Bob.
Cognitive dissonance theory assumes that in our daily social life whenever we
notice incoherence among our thoughts, attitudes and actions, an automated
response of psychological distress is generated. Although the degree of dissonance
may vary according to the relevance and importance of opinions, attitudes and
actions and corresponding to the degree of incongruence between belief and
behaviour, we are strongly tend to resolve the dissonance and furthermore, the
greater the dissonance the more you will be motivated to resolve it. It is argued
that dissonance is resolved in any of three basic ways:
124
The cognitive dissonance theory has been very extensively researched and applied Attitude And Attitude Change
in number of significant social behaviours; such as forced compliance behaviour,
decision making and effort justification.
The results led the researchers to conclude that an incentive of $1 was sufficient
for showing behaviour inconsistent with the belief which led the participants
who were paid $1 to experience greater dissonance. Consequently, this dissonance
was resolved by modifying the belief that the tasks were interesting and enjoyable.
However, a payment of $20 provided a significant reason for believing task as
really enjoyable and therefore, no or little dissonance was created in such
participants.
Brehm (1956), based on his study, proposed that dissonance in such conditions
can be reduced if the person in dissonance enhances the attractiveness of the
alternative he or she has chosen and simultaneously by attaching less attraction
or advantages to the rejected option. Brehm (1956) referred it as ‘spreading apart
the alternatives’.
7.7.3 Persuasion
Whenever we switch on our television sets we are exposed to numerous
advertisements that suggest buying different kinds of products, ranging from
apparels to sun glasses, chocolates to toothpastes, tour packages to electronic
gadgets, etc. There are ample research evidences indicating substantial influence
of such advertisements on our buying behaviour. These advertisements actually
change our attitudes towards certain products and brands. Persuasion refers to
changing attitudes by such external communication either in person or through
mass media, either in text or through multi media.
At the outset, if the persuasive communication takes the central route the
effectiveness of the persuasive attempt is majorly affected by the strength and
quality of the argument furthered by the persuader. If content of the persuasive
argument is strong and rich in quality the persuasive attempt is successful.
However, in case of peripheral route several properties of the source of the
message become critical to its effectiveness. Primarily, persuasive attempt is
successful when the source of the message is physically attractive, similar(in
terms of shared attitudes, appearance, or social categories) to the audience and
has credibility in the issues pertaining to the particular attitude.
The Source
The foremost important factor in effectiveness of persuasion is the communicator.
Often we see that similar arguments presented by different people have varying
impacts on the audience. Credible sources are more persuasive than those who
are low on the dimension of credibility. Credibility of the source increases with
our perception of the communicator as an expert of the field and his or her
trustworthiness. Hovland and Weiss (1951) initially took ratings of attitudes of
research participants towards nuclear submarines. One week later all the research
participants were asked to read an identical message regarding nuclear submarines.
However, one group of participants were told that the source of the message was
the famous scientist Robert J. Oppenheimer, presumably a high credible source.
On the other hand, remaining participants were told that the source of the message
was Pravda, the newspaper of the Communist Party of the then Soviet Union.
Researchers arguably presumed that this source would be a low credibility source
for the participants belonging to the United States. Immediately after reading the
message ratings of attitudes of research participants towards nuclear submarines
was taken again. Results indicated greater attitude change in those receiving
message from a presumably credible source than those who received message
from a low credible source.
127
Social Cognition Persuaders who are attractive and high on likeability are more successful in
changing the attitudes of their audience. The reason behind hiring attractive
models to appear in advertisements and to promote sell is the basic principle that
we like those attractive models and therefore, agree to buy the product. Some
researchers have argued that people speaking rapidly persuade more effectively
than those who speak slowly. Presumably, people speaking rapidly present an
impression that they have expertise and know everything of what they are talking
about (Miller, Maruyama, Beaber & Valone, 1976).
The Message
Emotion embedded in the content of the message is also an important factor in
determining the effectiveness of persuasion. Good feelings either induced by
the message or otherwise present in the environment, when persuasion is
attempted, enhance persuasion. Dabbs and Janis (1965) reported that students
participating in the experiment were more influenced by the persuasion when
they were enjoying peanuts and Pepsi while reading the message. In fact, when
the audience is in positive mood the message is processed through the peripheral
route and therefore, the content of the message is ignored resulting into more
impulsive decisions.
Some messages are designed in such a way that they present opposing arguments.
Contrarily, other messages are designed in such a way that they present only one
sided argument. Studies indicate that two sided messages are more effective in
persuasion as compared to one sided messages. Walster and Festinger (1962)
argued that the two sided messages do not appear to be deliberately framed to
change the attitude and therefore, such persuasive attempts face least resistance
from the audience. On the other hand, one sided messages seem to be deliberately
framed to change the attitude and therefore, audience to such messages show
enhanced resistance leading to less effective persuasion.
The Channel
Some persuasive attempts merely present the verbal messages to the audience.
On the other hand, persuasive messages may also be presented to the audience in
an interactive and experiential manner. Studies indicate that although the mere
reception of the message may lead to substantial degree of persuasion, the extent
of persuasion decreases as the significance and relevance of the issue increases.
When the issue is more relevant and important to the audience interactive and
experiential way of persuasion is more successful.
The Audience
People vary in their susceptibility to persuasion. Some people are easy to persuade,
while others show great resistance to the persuasion efforts. Janis (1954) has
argued that the people who have low self esteem are more easily persuaded than
those having high self esteem. Studies have shown that when audience are
obstructed from paying attention to the message they become more susceptible
to persuasion (Allyn & Festinger, 1961). Furthermore, people in formational
age, adolescents and early adults, are more susceptible to persuasion as compared
to the older audience (Krosnick & Alwin, 1989).
Reactance
All of us have a strong need of personal freedom to take a position or to have a
view on various issues. When a skilled persuader exerts pressure on us to change
our views or attitudes it threatens our freedom leading to increased level of
annoyance. Consequently, we not only resist the attempts of persuasion many
times we form a strong attitude opposite to the direction desired by the persuader
(Brehm, 1966). When an individual views a persuasive attempt as a direct threat
to his or her image as an independent person, this tendency of reactance becomes
stronger and the individual is strongly motivated to protect his or her attitude
from persuasion. Studies have indicated that in situations when reactance is
activated moderate or weak arguments are more successful in persuasion as
compared to the stronger ones.
Forewarning
There are number of situations where before being exposed to the persuasive
message we know in advance that the message has been intentionally designed
to change our attitude. For example, whenever we switch on our television sets
we know that the advertisements aired during the commercial breaks are
intentionally designed to enhance the possibility of buying the product by the
viewers. Similarly, when we listen to the speakers in a political campaign we
know that the speakers would argue for voting for particular political party. Studies
have been reported indicating that when the audience knows that a message is
intentionally designed to change the attitudes, known as forewarning, the
individual is less susceptible to the persuasive message (Johnson, 1994). When
we know about the intention of the message in advance we have enough time to
formulate arguments to guard our attitude from the persuasive message. Therefore,
in such situations we are cognitively better armed to protect our views.
129
Social Cognition Selective Exposure
Once attitudes are formed they become part of our self and therefore, we have a
strong tendency to protect them. We generally attend the information that are
consistent to our existing attitudes and purposefully avoid the information that
challenges our views. While watching television we change the channel during
commercial breaks to avoid any impact of persuasive attempts. Such selective
exposure and avoiding the information contradictory to our views ensure that
our attitudes are intact and persist for a longer time.
Counterarguments
Studies suggest that when we actively argue against the persuasive message
inconsistent to our attitude our susceptibility to the persuasion is weakened (Eagly,
Chen, Chaiken, & Shaw-Barnes, 1999). This is particularly true for the attitudes
which were initially formed on the basis of strong reasoning and extensive
arguments. When we argue against the message contrary to our attitude it further
provides reasons for holding the attitudes resulting into strengthening of the
existing attitude.
2) If content of the persuasive argument is strong and rich in quality, then the
persuasive attemptis successful. ( )
130
7.8.1 Attitude Specificity Attitude And Attitude Change
In many cases, our general attitudes fail to predict our specific behaviours. For
example, we might, in general, like psychology as a discipline. However, when
it comes to social psychology, one of its specific branches, we may not like it.
Similarly, in LaPiere’s study the attitude reported in the questionnaire was
regarding Chinese couple in general; however, the behaviour observed was toward
a specific Chinese couple. Furthermore, despite of being prejudiced and having
negative attitude toward a particular community in general, one may have
friendship with one or more specific members of that community.
Although the words stereotype, prejudice and discrimination are used in similar
ways in general conversations, they are theoretically explained in different ways
by social psychologists. Social psychologists argue that stereotype, prejudice
and discrimination represent three different components of attitude.
7.9.1 Stereotype
Stereotypes are beliefs that some traits and characteristics are shared by the
members of a particular social group. Stereotypes function as cognitive framework
and influence the way in which information relevant to the stereotype is processed.
Gender stereotype is one of the most prevalent stereotypes across societies. Based
on compilation of findings of various studies on gender stereotype, it is concluded
that females are stereotypically believed as ‘warmand dependent’, whereas; males
are perceived as ‘competent and independent’. Stereotypically associated feminine
traits are warm, emotionally sensitive, kind, submissive, oriented to aesthetics,
mild, etc. On the other hand, traits like competent, emotionally stable, confident,
tough, independent, non-conformist, leader, aggressive, etc. are stereotypically
believed as traits possessed by males. Das (2011) has reported that Indian
television advertisements have portrayed women mostly as young characters, in
relationship or family roles, less frequently as prominent characters, more
frequently in advertisements related to female oriented or beauty products, mostly
in home settings and not often as professionals.
7.9.2 Prejudice
Prejudice is defined as a feeling, primarily negative, toward a person exclusively
on the basis that the person is member of a particular social group. Thus, a person
prejudiced toward a particular social group expresses negative emotions for the
members of that particular group. Gordon Allport (1954) has referred prejudice
as “an antipathy based upon a faulty and inflexible generalization”. This indicates
that although prejudice is expressed toward a particular individual, the negative
emotions are actually targeted to the whole group. Similar to stereotypes/schemas,
prejudice too influences the way by which prejudiced person processes
information related to the particular social group and information consistent with
the prejudice is more readily attended, encoded and retrieved than the information
which is inconsistent.
Some studies have also reported that prejudiced people differentiate social groups
based on a belief that the groups have some common essence among all the
group members which may be biologically influenced (Yzerbyt, Corneille, &
Estrada, 2001). Prejudice is further referred to as an implicit or covert association
between a person’s being member of a particular social group and the evaluative
emotional response a prejudiced person expresses toward that person. This
suggests that in-group and out-group categorisation of our social world
automatically activates emotional evaluation of the people belonging to the social
groups and results into our corresponding responses toward them without being
consciously aware of it.
133
Social Cognition develop prejudice toward members of that social group. Tamborini et al. (2017)
have reported that the research participants who were more exposed to the news
coverage of the 2015 Paris terrorist attacks showed increased prejudice and
reduced pro-social intentions toward the members of the social group they thought
responsible for the incidence.
Sherif, Harvey, White, Hood, and Sherif (1961) very efficiently demonstrated
that how competition for resources can induce and intensify conflict between the
groups. The researchers conducted an innovative field experiment commonly
called as the Robbers Cave Experiment, a classic study in the field of social
psychology. Two groups of boys (12 boys randomly assigned to each group) of
similar socio-economic background were taken for a summer camp to a place
near rural Oklahoma. At the camp location, both groups kept disconnected from
each other. Boys of both the groups extensively enjoyed various activities, such
as hiking, swimming, etc. and the members of both the groups very quickly
developed in-group affiliation and attachment. They assigned names for their
respective groups; Rattlers and Eagles, and also made their flags and T-shirts
along with their group symbols stencilled on them. It further enhanced in-group
affiliation and identification.
In the second phase of the study, the two groups were introduced to each other
and were engaged in a series of inter-group competitions for which various
trophies and prizes were on stake. This initiated very intense competition between
the two groups which very soon resulted into positive evaluation of in-group and
negative evaluation of out-group, very heated verbal conflicts, attack on each
other’s camps, etc.; and finally into development of strong prejudices toward
each other.
However, similar to the attitudes prejudices are also not always overtly expressed
in behaviours. Modern legal provisions, influences of democratic social norms,
fear of retaliatory consequences, etc. prevent people to be overtly engaged in
discriminatory behaviours towards the target social groups. Therefore, prejudices
are expressed more often in disguised forms so that our prejudices are hidden
and not known to others. Some of such disguised forms of discrimination are
discussed below.
This common in-group identity model argues that when people from different
groups recreate their social boundaries to form a common social identity, their
earlier negative attitudes toward each other turn into positiveones.Sherif et al.
(1961) suggested the waysby which the social boundary can be recreated. In the
final phase of the Robber’s Cave study, researchers obstructed the water supply
which was common for both the groups and could be restored only with
cooperative efforts of both the groups. This led the boys of the two groups to
collaborate to achieve the common, superordinate goal. Researchers reported
that the conflict between the two groups further reduced and members of both
the groups started cooperating in other activities as well, resulting into
development of friendships among boys across the groups.
136
7.9.4.4 Feeling of Guilt Originated from Prejudice Attitude And Attitude Change
138
Attitude And Attitude Change
7.14 GLOSSARY
Attitude: Feelings, thoughts and behaviours for other people, issues, events,
situations, etc.
Implicit attitudes: Operateat conscious level and the person holding the attitude
is aware of its cognitive processing and impact on behaviour.
Forced compliance behaviour: Situations in our social life when we are forced
(many times under pressure of social norms, etiquettes or obligations) to behave
publicly in a way that is inconsistent with our belief that we personally or privately
have.
139
Social Cognition Effort justification: Tendency to justify efforts by appraising the achieved goal
in a more positive way and by attaching more subjective value to those goals,
consequently leading to the reduced dissonance.
Elaboration-likelihood model: When the target person is willing and has high
processing capacity to process the content of the message, the persuasive
communication takes the central/systematic route and the cues relevant to the
content or quality of the message are paid greater attention.
Heuristic-systematic model: When the target person is less motivated and has
low processing capacity to process the content of the persuasive communication
takes the peripheral/heuristic route and the cues irrelevant to the content or quality
of the message are paid greater attention.
Forewarning: A situation when people are aware about the intention of the
message in advance provides enough time to formulate arguments to guard our
attitude from the persuasive message.
Attitude accessibility: The attitudes that are more easily accessible; more strongly
influence the person’s behaviour.
Stereotype: Belief that some traits and characteristics are shared by almost all
the members of a particular social group.
Realistic conflict theory: The theory suggesting that the social groups engaged
in conflict for various resources view each other in extremely negative manner,
often as enemies.
Social categorisation: Refers to the tendency to categorise the social world into
two categories; “us” (in-groups) and “them”(out-groups).
Common in-group identity model: Argument that when people from different
groups recreate their social boundaries to form a common social identity, their
earlier negative attitudes toward each other turn into positive ones.
Superordinate goal: A goal common for the conflicting groups that can be
restored only with cooperative efforts of the groups.
References
141
Social Cognition Allport, G. W. (1935). Attitudes. In C. Murchison (Ed.), Handbook of Social
Psychology (pp. 173–210). Worcester, MA: Clark University Press.
Allport, G. W. (1954). The Nature of Prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Allyn, J., & Festinger, L. (1961). The effectiveness of unanticipated persuasive
communications. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 62(1), 35-
40.
Aronson, E., & Mills, J. (1959). The effects of severity of initiation on liking for
a group. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 59, 177-181.
Dabbs, J. M., Jr., & Janis, I. L. (1965). Why does eating while reading facilitate
opinion change? An experimental inquiry. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 1(2), 133-144.
Das, M. (2011). Gender Role Portrayal in Indian Television Ads. Sex Roles, 64,
208-222.
Eagly, A. H., Chen, S., Chaiken, S., & Shaw-Barnes, K. (1999). The impact of
attitudes on memory: An affair to remember. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 64-89.
Katz, D. (1960). The functional approach to the study of attitudes. Public Opinion
Quarterly, 24, 163-204.
Kawakami K., Dovidio, J. F., Moll, J., Hermsen, S., &Russn, A. (2000). Just say
no (to stereotyping): Effects of training in the negation of stereotypic associations
on stereotype activation. Journal and Personality and Social Psychology, 78,
871-888.
Krosnick, J. A., & Alwin, D. F. (1989). Aging and susceptibility to attitude change.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 416-425.
Krosnick, J. A., Betz, A. L., Jussim, L. J., & Lynn, A. R. (1992). Subliminal
conditioning of attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 152-
163.
LaPiere, R. T. (1934). Attitudes vs. actions. Social Forces, 13, 230-237.
Levitan, L. C., & Visser, P. S. (2008). The impact of the social context on resistance
to persuasion: Effortful versus effortless responses to counter-attitudinal
information. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 640-649.
Miller, N., Maruyama, G., Beaber, R. J., &Valone, K. (1976). Speed of speech
143
Social Cognition and persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34(4), 615-624.
Only 23 of 152 Oz attacks racist, Ministry tells LS. Indian Express. Retrieved 25
February 2010.
Petty, R. A., Tormala, Z. L., Brinol, P., & Jarvis, W. B. G. (2006). Implicit
ambivalence from attitude change: An expiration of the PAST model. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 21-41.
Petty, R.E., Haugtvedt, C.P. and Smith, S.M. (1995) Elaboration as a Determinant
of Attitude Strength: Creating Attitudes That Are Persistent, Resistant, and
Predictive of Behaviour. In R.E. Petty and J.A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude Strength:
Antecedents and Consequences (pp. 93-130). Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah.
Sherif, M., Harvey, D. J., White, B. J., Hood, W. R, &Sherif, C. W. (1961). The
Robbers’ cave experiment. Norman, OK: Institute of Group Relations.
Sinclair, S., Dunn, E., &Lowery, B. S. (2005). The relationship between parental
racial attitudes and children’s implicit prejudice. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 41, 283-289.
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior.
In S. Worchel& W. G. Austin (Eds.), The Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations
(2nd ed., pp. 7–24). Monterey, CA: Brooks-Cole.
Tajfel, H., Billig, M., Bundy, R., &Flament, C. (1971). Social categorization and
intergroup behaviour. European Journal of Social Psychology, 1, 149-178.
Tamborini, R., Hofer, M., Prabhu, S., Grall, C., Novotny, E. R., Hahn, L.
&Klebig,B.(2017).The impact of terrorist attack news on moral intuitions and
144
outgroup prejudice. Mass Communication and Society,20,800-824. https://DOI: Attitude And Attitude Change
10.1080/15205436.2017.1342130
Wilson, T., Lindsey, S., & Schooler, T. Y. (2000). A model of dual attitudes.
Psychological Review, 107, 101-126.
Yzerbyt, V. Y., Corneille, O., & Estrada, C. (2001). The interplay of subjective
essentialism and entitativity in the formation of stereotypes. Personality and
Social Psychology Review, 5, 141-155.
145
BLOCK 4
SOCIAL INFLUENCE
Social Cognition
INTRODUCTION
The present block consists of three units. The first unit discusses about the others’
influence on our behaviour in a social setting. While influencing our behaviour
this ‘other’ person may or may not be interacting with us. The three types of
social influences on our behaviour are: conformity, adherence to social norms or
following majority; compliance, acceding to direct request from others; and
obedience, following orders given by some authority. The present unit will explain
compliance, conformity and obedience as processes of social influence. Further,
through this unit you will also come to know about the various factors affecting
conformity and the ways to resist conformity. The unit will also explain you the
concept of compliance and the various strategies for gaining compliance. At the
end of the unit you will be explained about the concept and relevance of obedience.
The second unit of this block discusses about the concept and various theoretical
approaches of aggression. It also explains the various factors that affect aggression
as well as the strategies and techniques to reduce aggression. The unit also tries
to explain the nature, causes and steps to reduce bullying behaviour.
In the third and last unit of this block, you will come to know about the concept
and process of interpersonal attraction. You will also understand the various factors
of interpersonal attraction and the concept of pro-social behaviour. In the end of
the unit, we will also discuss about the various motivational factors of pro-
social behaviour and the factors affecting pro-social behaviour.
148
Attitude And Attitude Change
UNIT 8 SOCIAL INFLUENCE*
Structure
8.0 Objectives
8.1 Introduction
8.2 Conformity
8.2.1 Solomon Asch: Pioneer of Research on Conformity
8.2.2 Impact of Conformity
8.2.3 Factors Affecting Conformity
8.2.4 Reasons for Conformity
8.2.5 Disadvantages of Conformity
8.2.6 Resisting Conformity
8.2.7 Minority Influence
8.3 Compliance
8.3.1 Principles of Strategies Used in Compliance
8.3.2 Strategies for Gaining Compliance
8.3.2.1 Techniques Based on Friendship or Liking
8.3.2.2 Techniques Based on Commitment and Consistency
8.3.2.3 Techniques Based on Reciprocity
8.3.2.4 Techniques Based on Scarcity
8.4 Obedience
8.4.1 Milgram’s Experiment
8.4.2 Reasons for Destructive Obedience
8.4.3 Resisting Destructive Obedience
8.5 Let Us Sum Up
8.6 Unit End Questions
8.7 Glossary
8.8 Answers to Self Assessment Questions
8.9 Suggested Readings and References
8.0 OBJECTIVES
After reading this unit, you will be able to:
Explain compliance, conformity and obedience as processes of social
influence;
Describe reasons for displaying conformity, various factors affecting
conformity and the ways to resist conformity;
Discuss the concept of compliance and explain various strategies for gaining
compliance; and
Explain that concept and relevance of obedience.
8.1 INTRODUCTION
Whenever we are in a social setting, our thoughts and behaviours are affected by
elements which are external to our body and mind. Suppose you are driving your
* Dr. Ari Sudan Tiwari, Scientist ‘E’Defence Institute of Psychological Research, Ministry of Defence,
Lucknow Road, Timarpur, Delhi 149
Social Influence bike at night on a lonely road of your colony, you will drive in a quite carefree
manner. But when you drive on a heavy traffic road, you stay vigilant and follow
certain traffic rules. Maybe you want to avoid accident or you want to avoid
traffic inspector. Whatsoever the reason may be but your thoughts and your
behaviour get affected due to a social setting. Interestingly, sometimes your
behaviour is affected just by thinking about someone even though that someone
may not be present there. For example, although you get a lonely road in daylight,
yet you follow the traffic rules because you assume that a traffic inspector might
be present at the next crossroad. This form of influence is known as symbolic
social influence.
Social influence may be of many forms. It may be a request from someone that
affects your behaviour (compliance) or it may be social norms that change your
behaviour (conformity) or it may be an order from some authority that has brought
some change in your behaviour (obedience).
8.2 CONFORMITY
In almost every social setting that you encounter in your daily lives, there are
some rules about the “accepted behaviour” in that setting. How one should behave
and what one should not do is stated by those rules. These rules are known as
social norms. In some settings, these norms are formal, explicitly stated and
clearly mentioned in written form. For example, “Please form a Queue” sign
board in a post office. However, in many other settings norms are informal, implicit
and unstated. A widely accepted norm in most of the cultures around Indian sub-
continent is that after marriage, a girl should leave her parent’s house to live with
her husband’s family. Whatsoever the case may be, the norms play a very
important function of removing uncertainties and chaos from a social situation.
Norms restrict a person to behave in a predictable manner and hence reducing
uncertainties. That is why even though norms place restrictions on people, yet
people follow them.
Our tendency to conform to the social norms is so dominant that we are under a
social pressure to be similar to the people in our surrounding. Not just the norms,
but people around us also provide us a standard set of behaviour and opinions
against which we evaluate our own behaviour and opinions. They may or may
not be the correct standards, yet we use them to judge our own behaviour. For
example, people standing at the back in a political rally may not be able to listen
to the speech of the leader. Yet they clap just by seeing that the others standing
there are clapping.
Asch (1956, 1959) further conducted researches on conformity with some well
thought and innovative modifications in his classic study layout. He introduced
research accomplices who gave the correct answer or an answer which was
between the correct one and the one which was given by the majority. In another
study, the research accomplice gave more incorrect answer than that chosen by
the group. Thus, the studies were planned in such a way that the consensus of the
group was broken. Findings suggested that the real participant showed lesser
conformity to the group under all three conditions. This indicated that the unbroken
agreement of the group is the key component or force behind conformity and
once this unanimity is broken anyhow, the impact of group pressure is reduced
and it becomes much easier to resist. Asch further introduced some innovation in
his basic research design. He asked his research participants to write down their
answers on a paper and not to speak them out loudly. Interestingly, since the
participants were not required to openly show their disagreement with group,
the incidence of conformity reduced significantly. This finding indicated the
difference between public conformity and private acceptance suggesting that at
a number of occasions even if we explicitly act as per the social norms, we
actually donot alter our personal views.
Generally as the size of the group increases more pressure we feel to conform
to the group. However, relation between size of the group and the level of
conformity has been inconclusive. Some studies claim that conformity
increases only up to three to four members and after that the group influence
becomes either constant or even decreases (Asch, 1956). Some other studies
claim that conformity increases with the group size up to eight members
and ahead of that (Bond & Smith, 1996).
Apart from their classification as formal and informal, norms can also be
classified as descriptive and injunctive. Descriptive norms explain what
people generally do in a given situation; whereas, injunctive norms tell us
that what should be done in a given situation. For example, people do not
play loud music in a funeral is a descriptive norm; whereas, prescribing not
to smoke near a petrol pump is an injunctive norm. According to normative
focus theory (Cialdini, Reno & Kallgren, 1990), we conform only to those
norms which we consider relevant to us. The theory argues that norms steer
our behaviours mainly when we think about them and view them as relevant
to our behaviour. Contrary to this, the effects of social norms are reduced
when we do not think about them or view them as irrelevant. Furthermore,
Aarts and Dijksterhuis (2003) suggested that there are certain situational
norms that guide our behaviour in specific environment. For example, we
speak gently in a hospital and shout loud in a stadium. These norms affect
out behaviour very strongly and in an automatic manner, without our
conscious awareness.
Whatsoever the reason behind conforming is, it has both positive and negative
effects. Conformity helps to reduce uncertainty from people’s behaviour. Due to
conformity, we can predict others’ behaviour in a social setting and hence can
behave accordingly. While driving on road, we know that everyone will drive on
left side and will stop at red light signal. Similarly, in an emergency situation,
people follow others to escape from the situation. In case of fire, people follow
others to reach the nearest fire exit. Disadvantages of conformity include blind
adherence to certain norms like gender norms. Gender Norms are those norms
which describe appropriate acceptable behaviour for men and women in a
particular culture. This can place limits on the opportunities and career aspirations
of women (Eagly, 2007). Due to this only, trans-genders face number stereotypical
behaviours against them. Conformity is the most prominent cause behind
continuation of number of superstitious behaviours since generations. Other
negative effects of conformity include uncontrolled behaviour of crowd. In a
crowd, people follow others and ultimately do something so extreme which they
would have never done had they been alone.
Need to maintain individuality: Just like the desire to be liked and desire
to be right, there is a desire for individuation characterising, the desire to be
distinguished from others in some respect (Maslach, Santee & Wade, 1987).
Higher the desire for individuation, lower will be the need to conform and
vice versa. Studies have been reported which indicated that the need for
individuation varies in different cultures. This need is generally found to be
higher in individualistic cultures and lower in collectivistic culture. Hence,
accordingly, the need to conform will be lower in individualistic cultures
and higher in collectivistic cultures (Bond & Smith, 1996).
They must be consistent in their opposition to the majorities. They must not
appear divided.
They must not be rigid or dogmatic. Minorities that repeats same proposition
over and again are less persuasive than those that display a degree of
flexibility.
Minorities that argue for a position that is consistent with current social
trend are more influential.
1) Apart from their classification as formal and informal, norms can also be
classified as ………………………. and ……………………….
8.3 COMPLIANCE
Compliance is a form of social influence where we accede or give acceptance to
direct request form some other person. In our daily life, we encounter many
persons whose success in their profession depends upon their ability to make
others comply. Salespersons, advertisers, insurance agents, politicians,
professional negotiators, etc. are some examples of such compliance professionals.
Not only these professionals but we also indulge in lot of events of making others
comply. Consider your mother going to market for the weekly shopping. While
bargaining with the vegetable vendor, both mother and vendor are involved in
compliance strategies. Whenever you try to finalise any plan for party with your
friends, all of you try to convince others with their ideas and finally you reach at
some conclusion.
154
8.3.1 Principles of Strategies Used in Compliance Social Influence
Scarcity: There is a greater chance for us to comply with those requests that
focus on scarcity.
Reciprocity: We are more likely to comply with the requests of those who
had previously given us a favour.
Social validation: We are more likely to comply with the request for
behaviours which are in line with our social norms and beliefs.
Flattery: Persons trying to gain compliance usually praise their target so that
they build a positive image of themselves and hence increase chances of gaining
compliance.
Lowball Technique: In lowball procedure, a deal is first offered to the target, but
once the target accepts this deal, the deal is made less lucrative. Studies have
shown that this strategy is successful in gaining compliance (Cialdini, Cacioppo,
Bassett & Miller, 1978). Here also, the target has option to reject the deal once
requester introduces changes in it. Yet prior commitment of the target makes
them accept the changed deal also. For example,whenever you choose for an
insurance scheme, the terms and conditions of the product are disclosed after
you agree to buy the product.
Playing Hard to Get: In Playing Hard to Get technique the object of request is
portrayed as rapidly exhausting and the target person has to work really hard to
get that object. “Limited Stock” displays on the shops use this tactic to gain
compliance.
8.4 OBEDIENCE
This is a form of social influence in which a direct order by a person compels
others to behave in a particular manner. However, the person giving order can
use other means also to influence the behaviours, such as request instead of
order (Yukl & Falbe, 1991). Thus, this form of social influence is less frequent
than other two forms discussed earlier. Obedience, as a social influence, can
sometimes be highly destructive. For example, military troop obeying their
command can be brutal towards their target.
157
Social Influence
Fig. 9.1: Apparatus used in Milgram’s Obedience Experiment (Retrieved from https://
www.simplypsychology.org/milgram.html)
Procedure: In each pair, the learner (fake participant) had to perform a simple
task of learning (reciting the second word of the previously memorized pair after
listening to the first word of that pair). The participant in the teacher role (real
participant) had to read out those words to the learner and to punish the learner
on errors (by giving an electric shock through the apparatus). The real participant
was seated in front of the apparatus. The experimenter (who was conducting the
experiment) was present with him. The assistant (learner) was seated on the
chair receiving the shock. He was visible to the real participants. During the
experiment, the learner deliberately made many errors. Every time he made an
error, the real participant had to give a shock to the learner. If he hesitated, the
experimenter pressured him to continue with a graded series of urge like: “The
experiment requires you to continue”; “It is essential that you continue”; “You
will have no other choice, you must go on”.
Results: Results indicated that 65% of the total participants proceeded through
the complete series till 450 volt. Few participants protested and asked the
experiment to stop but later yielded to the experimenter’s demand to proceed.
Participants continued even after the assistant acted to become unconscious at
the level of 300 volts. In such cases, the participants were asked to consider no
response from the learner as an error and hence to continue the punishment to
the learner. Other studies have also reported similar results for different cultures
and with children and adults also (Kilham& Mann, 1974; Shanab&Yanya, 1977).
158
8.4.2 Reasons for Destructive Obedience Social Influence
History of human race has numerous examples showing that destructive obedience
can become extremely detrimental to the mankind. Many dictators have been
responsible for death of thousands of people through assassinations, massacre,
etc. Psychologists have studied the reasons that lead people to obey the destructive
commands of such dictators.
People obeying the order are relieved of the guilt feeling by the fact that
they are just following the command. Hence they do not hesitate in obeying.
In Milgram’s experiment, participants were told that they will not be
responsible for the learner’s well-being.
People giving commands; usually wear some uniform or some insignia which
is a symbol of their authority and power. It is a general norm of our society
to obey the seniors. Hence most cannot disobey the commands. The
experimenter in the Milgram’s experiment wore a white coat which gave
the participants a feeling that the experimenter is a doctor.
Orders are gradually increased in their relative strength to harm others. For
example, initially the order may be given to a police team to just arrest a
group of protesters. But later on, the orders may be escalated to brutal beating
of the protesting group. In Milgram’s experiment as well, participants were
initially instructed to give mild shock to the learner and the magnitude of
shock was increased in a graded manner.
Obedience is a form of social influence which is least used because the person
using this can use other, more effective, forms of influencing behaviour.
Experiments of Stanley Milgram presented a very surprising phenomenon that
people obey the destructive commands even from persons who do not have
legitimate authority.
160
Social Influence
8.6 UNIT END QUESTIONS
1) Present an account of conformity with the help of Solomon Asch’s classical
experiment.
3) Describe the conditions under which people resist to conform. Also bring
out the conditions under which, minority can influence majority.
8.7 GLOSSARY
Social influence: Attempts to bring change in the behaviour, attitudes or feelings
of others people.
Conformity: Tendency to get influenced by the social norms and to think, feel
or act in the way majority think, feel or act.
Descriptive norms: Norms that explain that what people generally do in a given
situation.
Normative focus theory: Theory stating that we conform only to those norms
which we consider relevant to us.
161
Social Influence Door-in-the-face technique: A compliance technique in which a larger deal is
offered first by the requesterandonce the target rejects this deal, a smaller and
actual deal is presented before them.
162
Asch, S. E. (1951). Effects of group pressure upon the modification and distortion Social Influence
of judgment. In H. Guetzkow (Ed.),
Baron, R. S., Vandello, U. A., & Brunsman, B. (1996). The forgotten variable in
conformity research: Impact of task importance on social influence. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 915-927.
Cialdini, R. B. (1994). Influence: Science and Practice (3rd ed.). New York:
Harper Collins.
Cialdini, R. B., Cacioppo, J. T., Bassett, R., & Miller J. A. (1978). A low-ball
procedure for producing compliance: Commitment then cost. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 463-476.
Cialdini, R. B., Reno, R. R., & Kallgren, C. A. (1990). A focus theory of normative
conduct : Recycling the concept of norms to reduce littering in public places.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 105-1026.
Maslach, Christina & T. Santee, Richard & Wade, Cheryl. (1987). Individuation,
Gender Role, and Dissent: Personality Mediators of Situational Forces. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology. 53. 1088-1093.
McLeod, S. A. (2017, Feb 05). The Milgram shock experiment. Retrieved from
https://www.simplypsychology.org/milgram.html
164
Social Influence
UNIT 9 AGGRESSION AND SOCIAL
INFLUENCE*
Structure
9.0 Objectives
9.1 Introduction
9.2 Aggression: Meaning and Forms
9.3 Theoretical Approaches to Aggression
9.3.1 Biological Approaches
9.3.2 Drive Approaches
9.3.3 Social Learning Approaches
9.3.4 Frustration-Aggression Hypothesis
9.3.5 Excitation Transfer Theory
9.3.6 General Aggression Model (GAM)
9.4 Causes of Aggression
9.4.1 Personal Factors
9.4.1.1 Frustration and Provocation
9.4.1.2 Personality and Aggression
9.4.1.3 Bio-chemical Influences
9.4.1.4 Displaced Aggression
9.4.1.5 Age and Aggression
9.4.1.6 Gender and Aggression
9.4.2 Interpersonal, Social and Cultural Factors
9.4.2.1 Exposure to Mass Media and Aggression
9.4.2.2 Social Rejection
9.4.2.3 Influence and Need Fulfilment
9.4.3 Situational and Environmental Factors
9.4.3.1 Weapon Effect
9.4.3.2 Environmental Discomfort
9.5 Reducing Aggression
9.5.1 Social Learning: Punishment and Modelling
9.5.2 Catharsis
9.5.3 Cognitive Strategies
9.6 Bullying Behaviour
9.6.1 Motives behind Bullying Behaviour
9.6.2 Steps to Reduce Bullying Behaviour
9.7 Let Us Sum Up
9.8 Unit End Questions
9.9 Glossary
9.10 Answers to Self Assessment Questions
9.11 Suggested Readings and References
* Dr. Ari Sudan Tiwari, Scientist ‘E’Defence Institute of Psychological Research, Ministry of Defence,
Lucknow Road, Timarpur, Delhi
165
Social Influence
9.0 OBJECTIVES
After reading this unit, you will be able to:
Define aggression;
Describe various theoretical approaches of aggression;
Explain various factors that affect aggression;
Discuss various strategies and techniques to reduce aggression; and
Explain the nature, causes and steps to reduce bullying behaviour.
9.1 INTRODUCTION
Aggression is one of the most primitive adaptive behaviour that has been used
not only by human being, but also by other species. In the primitive societies,
survival was dependent upon the ability to overpower the physical might of others
by being hostile and aggressive towards them. However, technological
advancements have brought about different more fatal, though easier, ways of
expressing aggression. Now, there are many sophisticated weapons that are easily
available and can be smoothly operated in interpersonal aggressive and hostile
moments. Moreover, many weapons of mass destruction have been developed
by nations and the world always faces fear that these weapons may be used by
any country out of individual insanity and stupidity of dictatorial heads of some
of these nations. Thus, aggression, anger and hostility form an extreme form of
social behaviour and therefore, social psychologists have attempted to explore
various aspects of aggression, such as the process of expression of aggressive
behaviours, factors that affect aggression, strategies and techniques to reduce
aggression, etc.
169
Social Influence
Stage One: Input Variables
Affect
Cognition Arousal
Fig. 9.1: General aggression model (Source: Bushman & Anderson, 2002)
At the first stage, the GAM assumes two types of input variables responsible for
instigating expression of overt aggression. Factors coming under situational
variables include frustration, provocation, exposure to in vivo or in vitro aggressive
models, environmental elements causing discomfort to the person, etc. Variables
pertaining to individual differences are related with the personal dispositions for
aggressive behaviours, individual’s attitudes, beliefs, intentions and temperament
and skills for performing aggressive acts. GAM further assumes that personal
and situational variables function in an additive or interactive way and therefore,
aggression is directly linked with the number of personal and situational variables
present in a given incidence.
The second stage pertains to the routes through which personal and situational
factors affect the person’s appraisal and decision processes leading to either
aggressive or nonaggressive outcomes. GAM argues that the personal and
situational factors influence the person’s present internal state constituted by his
170
or her affect (hostile feelings and hostile gestures and facial expressions), cognition Aggression and Social
Influence
(hostile thoughts, beliefs and attitudes) and arousal (physiological arousal and
excitement) which further moderate the likelihood of aggression. Different input
variables affect different present internal state variables, but present internal state
variables also influence each other in interactive and reciprocal ways. Affect can
influence cognition and arousal. For example, feeling angry can encourage hostile
thoughts and increase arousal. Similarly, cognition and arousal can influence
affect. For example, interpreting a situation in a hostile manner can increase
anger, which in turn can increase arousal. GAM does not assume any specific
sequence of the present internal state variables and suggest that any of the three
variables can occur first and then influence the other two.
The third and final stage of GAM is the outcome stage where the person interprets
the situation and based on his or her appraisal of the current situation and
restraining factors, decision on being involved in aggressive or nonaggressive
behaviour is taken. GAM argues that based on situational appraisal and
reappraisal, the person engages either in thoughtful action; leading to
nonaggressive behaviour, or in impulsive action; leading to aggressive behaviour.
GAM has further explained the process of social learning by which exposure to
real or virtual aggressive models increases the likelihood of aggressive behaviours.
Recurring exposure to these stimuli strengthens the aggression related knowledge
structures; such as beliefs, attitudes, schemas and scripts relevant to aggression
and with increasing strength of these knowledge structures, these are more
activated by situational or person variables. Thus, repeated exposure to aggressive
models actually “primes” the person for aggressive behaviour.
4) Aggression is defined as
…………………………………………………………………….
Furthermore, low self-control has been reported as one of the strongest predictor
of aggression. Social psychologists have argued that persons high in narcissism
react with extremely high levels of aggression when other people behave against
their wish, putting their unrealistically inflated self-image in danger and
consequently, their egos are threatened.
172
9.4.1.3 Bio-chemical Influences Aggression and Social
Influence
The GAM, discussed in an earlier section, provides an explanation for the effect
of exposure to media violence on aggressive behaviour (Bushman & Anderson,
2002). The GAM argues that consistent and prolonged exposure to media violence
modifies our cognitive schemas, beliefs, attitudes and scripts and strengthens
the knowledge structures related to aggression. As these knowledge structures
become stronger, they lead people to a hostile expectation bias, a belief that
other people generally behave in aggressive way. This belief further leads us to
easily become aggressive and violent, even in response to general environmental
conditions.
174
9.4.2.3 Influence and Need Fulfilment Aggression and Social
Influence
9.5.2 Catharsis
Catharsis, a concept originated from psychoanalytic approach, is the process in
which a strong emotion is vented out at some displaced target in milder form.
Catharsis of aggression is expression of one’s aggressive intent at a target that is
in some way associated with the original source of aggression in some relatively
non-harmful manner. There is an assumption that catharsis reduces the possibility
of overt aggressive behaviour in its harmful form. Studies have shown that non-
harmful behaviours, such as going for vigorous sports, aggressing at the
photograph of an enemy or shouting in foul language in an empty room, that are
a substitute to the actual aggressive behaviour, have proved to reduce anger and
aggression. However, such effects have been found to be only temporary and
when the person is placed in the actual social setting, the aggressive behaviour
may recover again. Furthermore, contrary to the belief that safer aggressive acts
such as watching televised violence, attacking an inanimate object or verbal
aggression may reduce the chances of aggressive behaviour, studies have shown
that aggression may actually increase due to involvement in minor aggressive
behaviours by reducing the inhibition for such behaviours.
4) Type B behaviour pattern are extremely high on competitive drive and they
are highly irritable and aggressive in social interactions. ( )
9.9 GLOSSARY
Aggression: Behaviour aimed at producing physical or psychological harm to
another person.
Frustration: An event that obstructs the attainment of the goal by any obstacle
to the goal directed behaviour.
The TASS model: An argument that some traits enhance the person’s sensitivity
by reducing threshold for responding to the situation.
Myers, D. G. & Twenge, J. M. (2017). Social Psychology (12th ed.). New York,
NY: McGraw-Hill.
Kassin, S., Fein, S.& Markus, H. R. (2017). Social psychology (10th ed.). Cengage
Learning.
References
Anderson, C. A. & Bushman, B. J. (2002). Human aggression. Annual Review of
Psychology, 53, 27-51.
Anderson, C. A., Bushman, B. J. & Groom, R. W. (1997). Hot years and serious
and deadly assault: Empirical tests of the heat hypothesis. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 73, 1213-1223.
Dabbs, J. M., Carr, T. S., Frady, R. L. & Riad, J. K. (1995). Testosterone, crime,
and misbehavior among 692 male prison inmates. Personality and Individual
Differences, 18, 627-633.
Dollard, J., Doob, L., Miller, N., Mowerer, O. H. & Sears, R. R. (1939).
Frustration and Aggression. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
182
Aggression and Social
UNIT 10 INTERPERSONAL ATTRACTION Influence
10.0 OBJECTIVES
After reading this unit, you will be able to:
Explain the concept and process of interpersonal attraction;
Describe various factors of interpersonal attraction;
Define pro-social behaviour and explain its types;
Explain various motivational factors of pro-social behaviour; and
Discuss various factors affecting pro-social behaviour.
* Dr. Ari Sudan Tiwari, Scientist ‘E’Defence Institute of Psychological Research, Ministry of Defence,
Lucknow Road, Timarpur, Delhi 183
Social Influence
10.1 INTRODUCTION
Human beings have been defined as social animal. The term ‘animal’ has been
used because of the biological processes associated with us. But the term ‘social’
has a lot to say about nature of human beings. Have you ever thought if the
human were made to grow and live in isolation? Obviously, you cannot imagine
that how our life would have been if we didn’t have so many people around us.
Human infants are born with an inbuilt motivation and ability to seek contact
with their social world. Interaction with others is so essential that lack of it can
cause a lot of psychological disorder. Think about the punishment of kala pani.The
prisoners of kala pani suffered a state of social exclusion in the Cellular Jail of
Andaman and Nicobar Islands. This made them face many psychological
disorders. Some of them even died during it.
Social psychologists have been pondering upon the universality of the need to
affiliate and associate with other people. On one hand, some social psychologists
184
claim that need to affiliate is a very strong one and almost all people display Interpersonal Attraction and
Pro-Social Behaviour
them (Baumeister & Twenge, 2003). On the other hand, a different
viewpointclaimsthat there is no suchuniversal need to affiliate to others. Rather
they claim thatsomepeopleavoid close relationships; termed as dismissing-
avoidant attachment style (Collins & Feeney, 2000). However, some recent studies
(Carvallo & Gabriel, 2006) have supported the view that all the people have a
strong need to affiliate. They may hide it, claim otherwise, or display it in some
other ways depending upon their culture; but they cannot deny its existence.
Direct Influence
In India, we frequently come across elections, either for central legislature,
provincial legislature or some other local body elections. During such periods,
various candidates visit us for canvass. No matter how ill named, rude and
notorious those candidates are in their real lives, but when it comes to elections
they talk nicely, behave gently and project themselves as your only well-wisher.
Similarly, the sales person in a showroom is very gentle to us or an insurance
agent talks very nicely to us. A general thumb rule is that we tend to like a person
who is nice to us and makes us feel good rather than those who is a contrary
(Ben-Porath, 2002).
Indirect Influence
One even more surprising influence of emotions on our likes and dislikes come
from the principles of classical conditioning. When we are in a positive mood
we evaluate a person or a thing present at that time in a positive way and when
we are in a negative mood we evaluate a person or a thing present at that time in
a negative way (Berry & Hansen, 1996). Application of this principle has been
frequently seen in the advertisements where a positive mood is induced before
presenting the target product.
These principles are more prominent with a stranger or with a person or a thing
we do not know very well, such as a guy from some other section of your class
185
Social Influence with whom you have a casual relation or a product you have not used before.
With a well acquainted person there is a pre-existing opinion whether you like
him or her or do not like. Also, these principles do not work in absolutism. There
are many other factors, such as your belief systems, cultural biases, stereotypes,
etc., which guide your likes and dislikes towards a person. For example, you will
never positively evaluate a stranger belonging to a community towards which
you have cultural biases and negative stereotypes; no matter how positive your
mood is or how nicely he or she is talking to you.
It has been found that people associate qualities like interesting, sociable,
dominant, exciting adjusting, skilled, successful, masculine/feminine, etc. with
attractive men and women (Dion & Dion, 1991). Although, these associations
may be incorrect, misguidingand illogical; yet, researchers have found that
attractiveness is usually associated with popularity, high self-esteem and good
interpersonal skills (Diener, Wolsic & Fujita, 1995). Though, attractiveness may
not have any direct influence on these qualities; the attractive people have usually
been treated well by others.
Just like the cues of physical attractiveness, there may also be some other
superficial and observable characteristics that influence the onset of
communication and attraction. The first one of them is clothing. Both neatness
and colour of the cloths have effects on attractiveness of the person leading to
attraction of other interacting persons. Furthermore, people react positively to
youthful walking style, firm handshake, animated behaviour and modest persons.
Person’s physique also triggers various emotional reactions and differential
attraction. For example, round and fat body indicates sad and sloppy person;
hard and muscular body indicates good health and lack of intelligence; thin and
angular body indicates intelligence and fearfulness (Ryckman, Robbins, Kaczor
& Gold, 1989). Apart from this, various other visible characteristics such as any
form of physical disability, mental illness, perceived age, beard and eyeglasses,
etc. influence attraction.
However, the genesis of attraction between two individuals is not so simple that
in can be predicted by using a single mathematical formula. For example, if you
have a new classmate with the views similar to that of yours on sports, music and
religious practices. But the only difference you find that you love to be serious
and quiet most of the time; whereas he or she is extremely talkative. Considering
such situations, Rosenbaum (1986) proposed repulsion hypothesis which states
that similar attitude does not increase attraction. Instead, it is dissimilarity which
causes repulsion between two people. By considering a mathematical formula as
used above, it becomes impossible to segregate the effect of similarity and
dissimilarity.
Social psychologists have been exploring that why similarity causes attraction
and dissimilarity causes repulsion. Why does similarity elicit positive feelings;
whereas dissimilarity evokes negative feelings? Various propositions have been
put forward to answer this question. Applying balance theory (Heider, 1958) to
friendship development, Newcomb (1961) has argued that people have a natural
tendency to organise their likes and dislikes in a symmetrical way. Similarity of
thoughts, ideas, attitudes, etc., between two persons constitutes a state of balance
which is emotionally pleasing to both the persons. Similarly, discovering
dissimilarity in some respect causes imbalance which is emotionally unpleasant.
An imbalanced state drives individuals to restore balance by any of the three
methods: causing one of them to change or to misperceive the dissimilarity or by
developing dislikes each other.
The third approach to explain the effect of similarity and dissimilarity on attraction
is adaptive response proposed by Gould (1996). This theory is evolved from
evolutionary perspective which presumes that anything that is not similar to us
is a potential danger to us. Thus, it is an inherent tendency in the human being to
accompany people with similarity and to stay away from dissimilar people.
2) Newcomb (1961) has argued that people have a natural tendency to organise
their likes and dislikes in a ……………………… way.
Thus, the difference between altruism and helping behaviour is drawn from the
distinction in the motivation behind the behaviour and the mere outcome of the
behaviour is not that important. A person may get rewarded for a behaviour
entirely motivated to relieve the suffering of the victim. However, since the
behaviour was not executed in expectation of any reward in return, the behaviour
may be called altruism.
191
Social Influence
10.8 THE BYSTANDER EFFECT: A FIVE STAGE
MODEL OF HELPING BEHAVIOUR
Often we come across the situations where by standers help the victims who are
most of the time strangers to them. However, several stories in media are also
reported about incidences in which bystanders remain passive and do not
intervene. We find many incidences of road rage or molestation at a public place
in which the victims do not get help from the bystanders.
Latané and Darley (1968) proposed a model suggesting that in such emergency
situations, where immediate help is warranted, a bystander passes through five
different stages and at every step he or she has to take a decision before helping
the person in need. The model assumes that help is provided by the bystander
only when he or she notices something unusual in the situation; perceives,
interprets and construes the situation as an emergency and assumes responsibility
for helping. Bystander must understand that he or she has the knowledge and
skill required to give help and decides how to help and finally decides actually to
the victim. The model, as shown in Fig. 6.1, suggests that each stage of the
helping behaviour has a decision process and the bystander must take a ‘yes’
decision at each stage in order to help the victim. A ‘yes’ decision at one stage
does not guarantee that help will be provided; it simply allows the person to
move to the next stage of the model. However, a‘no’ decision at any of the five
stages will lead to failure to help.
No Yes
Decided to implement Help is provided
help
Yes
No
How to help is decided
Help is not provided
Yes
No
Responsibility to help is
assumed
Yes
No
Situation perceived as an
emergency
Yes
No
Something unusual in
the situation is noticed
Fig. 10.1: Five stage model of helping behaviour (Latané & Darley, 1968)
192
1) Noticing something unusual in the situation: First of the five stages of Interpersonal Attraction and
Pro-Social Behaviour
helping behaviour in emergencies is noticing that there is something unusual
or unexpected in the situation. For every social situation we generally have
some schemas or cognitive structures that contain broader expectations and
knowledge of the situation that help us systematically organise and process
the information. Any given situation is noticed as unusual when it there is
something remarkably distinguishing from our long held schema driven
expectation in the situation. Every day we see numerous motor vehicles
running on the roads. However, hearing somebody’s cry from a passing car
may lead us to notice something unusual in the situation. In case we fail to
notice unusual in the situation, we will not move to the next stage of the
decision process and help will not be provided.
5) Deciding to finally implement help: Although the first four stages are
mandatorily required to pass through, they alone do not lead a person to
helping behaviour. The person has still to take the final decision whether he
will implement the helping behaviour or not. This final decision to execute
helping behaviour is dependent on the person’s evaluation about the rewards
and costs in helping. There are potential rewards for helping (gratitude from
the victim, monetary reward, recognition by peers, etc.) and for not helping
(avoiding potential danger, arriving for an appointment on time, etc.).
Similarly, there are costs for helping (possible injury, embarrassment,
inconvenience, etc.) and for not helping (loss of self-esteem). Presumably,
help is finally provided to the victim when the person finds that the rewards
owing to the helping behaviour are greater than the costs. However, in case
of greater costs involved in helping, likelihoodofpeople providing help to
the victim is reduced.
10.9.4 Gratitude
When a person receives recognition and appreciation for his or her pro-social
act, it is more likely that he or she will help again to the same person, as well as
to other people at subsequent occasions. It is believed that being recognised and
appreciated enhances the helper’s self-efficacy and self-worth which further lead
the person to enhanced tendency of pro-social behaviours.
2) People are more likely to provide help to the strangers than to those who are
close to them, such as family members, friends. ( )
4) Such behaviours which are merely motivated by the desire to reduce the
suffering of the person are called altruism. ( )
5) When a person receives recognition and appreciation for his or her pro-
social act, it is more likely that he or she will not help again. ( )
195
Social Influence
10.10 LET US SUM UP
More than interpersonal relation, attraction is a prolonged relationship based on
liking between two persons. Though it is common among human being, different
people differ in their tendency to get attracted towards others and number of
internal, external and interpersonal factors affect interpersonal attraction. The
tendency to affiliate and associate with others is one important factor affecting
interpersonal attraction. Different individuals have different intensity of desires
to affiliate with others and for relations. Many external events, such as natural
disasters or other common threats, may also influence the need to affiliate.
Presence of positive affect has very prominent effect on our evaluation of other
persons and things around us, leading to have a facilitate development of
interpersonal attraction. Persons who are physically closer and have chance to
meet repeatedly are expected to develop attraction towards to each other;
presumably because repeated exposure to any new stimulus results in positive
evaluation of that stimulus. Furthermore, good and attractive physical appearance
also has a significant influence on the initiation of interpersonal communication
and attraction. Social psychologists argue that similarity and reciprocity evokes
positive feelings between people leading to develop interpersonal attraction.
196
Interpersonal Attraction and
10.11 UNIT END QUESTIONS Pro-Social Behaviour
2) Define pro-social behaviour and explain its types. Also discuss various
motivational factors behindpro-social behaviour.
10.12 GLOSSARY
Interpersonal attraction: A prolonged relationship between two persons based
on liking for each other.
Need to affiliate: Tendency to affiliate with others and to form relations.
Mere exposure effect: Persons who are physically closer and have chance to
meet repeatedly are expected to develop attraction towards to each other.
Myers, D. G. & Twenge, J. M. (2017). Social Psychology (12th ed.). New York,
NY : McGraw-Hill.
Kassin, S., Fein, S., & Markus, H. R. (2017). Social Psychology (10th ed.).
Cengage Learning.
References
Andreoletti, C., Zebrowitz, L. A. & Lachman, M. E. (2001). Physical appearance
and control beliefs in young, middle-aged, and older adults. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 969-981.
Batson, C. D., Duncan, B. D., Ackerman, P., Buckley, T. & Birch, K. (1981). Is
empathic emotion a source of altruistic motivation? Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 40, 290-302.
Berry, D. S.& Hansen, J. S. (1996). Positive affect, negative affect, and social
interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 796-809.
Carvallo, M., & Gabriel, S. (2006). No man is an island: The need to belong and
dismissing avoidant attachment style. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
32, 697-709.
Diener, E., Smith, H. & Fujita, F. (1995). The personality structure of affect.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 130-141.
Fischer, P., Greitemeyer, T., Pollozek, F. & Frey, D. (2006). The unresponsive
bystander: Are bystanders more responsive in dangerous emergencies? European
Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 267-278.
199
Social Influence Fultz, J., Shaller, M. & Cialdini, R. B. (1988). Empathy, sadness, and distress:
Three related but distant vicarious affective responses to another’s suffering.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 14, 312-325.
Gould, S. J. (1996). Full House: The Spread of Excellence From Plato to Darwin.
New York: Harmony Books.
Hunt, A. McC. (1935). A study of the relative value of certain ideals. Journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 30, 222-228.
Monin, B. (2003). The warm glow heuristic: When liking leads to familiarity.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 1035-1048.
Park, J.& Banaji, M. R. (2000). Mood and heuristics: The influence of happy
and sad states on sensitivity and bias in stereotyping. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 78, 1005-1023.
200
Rowe, P. M. (1996, September). On the neurobiological basis of affiliation. APS Interpersonal Attraction and
Pro-Social Behaviour
Observer, 17-18.
Ryckman, R. M., Robbins, M. A., Kaczor, L. M. & Gold, J. A. (1989). Male and
female raters’ stereotyping of male and female physiques. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 15, 244-251.
201
BLOCK 5
GROUP DYNAMICS
Group Dynamics
INTRODUCTION
The fourth block of this course consists of two units. In the first unit, you will
come to know about the concept of group and its various types. We will also
discuss the various stages of group formation. Further, the unit will also define
various elements of a group and how they affect behaviour in a group as well as
the various group processes. The unit will also explain decision making in a
group and describe cooperation, competition and conflict in a group.
The second unit discusses about the meaning, characteristics and nature of leaders.
It will also deal with the different types of leadership style and the role of leaders
in conflictual situation.
204
Group Processes
UNIT 11 GROUP PROCESSES*
Structure
11.1 Objectives
11.2 Introduction
11.3 Groups: Definition and Concept
11.4 Types of Groups
11.4.1 Perceived Bond among Group Members
11.4.2 Entitativity
11.4.3 Primary versus Secondary Group
11.4.4 Formal versus Informal Group
11.5 Components of Group
11.5.1 Status
11.5.2 Roles
11.5.3 Norms
11.5.4 Cohesiveness
11.6 Stages of Group Formation
11.7 Group Effectiveness
11.8 Group Processes
11.8.1 Social Facilitation
11.8.2 Social Loafing
11.8.3 Deindividuation
11.9 Group Decision Making
11.9.1 Group Polarisation
11.9.2 Group Think
11.10 Cooperation and Competition in a Group
11.10.1 Determinants of Cooperation and Competition
11.11 Conflict in a Group
11.11.1 Causes of Conflict
11.11.2 Outcomes of Conflict
11.11.3 Conflict Resolution Strategies
11.12 Communication
11.12.1 Types of Communication
11.12.2 Communication Style
11.12.3 Barriers to Effective Communication
11.13 Let Us Sum Up
11.14 Unit End Questions
11.15 Glossary
11.16 Answers to Self Assessment Questions
11.17 Suggested Readings and References
* Dr. Ari Sudan Tiwari, Scientist ‘E’Defence Institute of Psychological Research, Ministry of Defence,
Lucknow Road, Timarpur, Delhi 205
Group Dynamics
11.1 OBJECTIVES
After reading this unit, you will be able to:
Define Groups and its various types;
Describe various stages of group formation;
Define various elements of a group and how they affect behaviour in a group;
Define various group processes;
Explain decision making in a group; and
Discuss the concept and relevance of cooperation, competition and conflict
in a group.
11.2 INTRODUCTION
Living in a social world, we are rarely alone. At homes, we are surrounded by
our family members, in school we are surrounded by classmates/teachers, in
office we are surrounded by our colleagues,on the road we are surrounded by
our fellow travellers, in the park we are surrounded by our friends and strangers.
These people range from one whom we don’t know at all to those whom we
know very well. Few of them interact with us frequently, few of them sometimes
and few of them rarely. Whatsoever the level of interaction, they all influence
our behaviour. In this unit, we will be discussing about the persons whom we
know and with whom we have a considerable amount of interactions are somehow
connected to us and we are an intact part of group. Being intact suggests that we
are dependent on each other for fulfilment of some of our basic needs, like survival
needs, social needs, etc. Hence, these people are the ones who play a major role
in shaping our core personality and are the cause behind most of our actions.
A group must be further differentiated from a team where the members have
complementary skills, affect each other’s activities, attain a positive synergy
by coordinated effort and are collectively responsible for the performance.
206
For example, ten students doing combined study is an example of group. Group Processes
Here each individual studies the syllabus separately. On the other hand, a
group formed in the company to complete a project is an example of team.
Here, the person analysing the data cannot do the job until the data has been
entered by the other person, who in turn is dependent on the person
responsible for collection of data. Moreover, in the second example whole
group is accountable for the completion of the project; whereas, in the first
Affinitive needs: Provided by the group through friendship between
individuals within it.
The groups can provide, with or without the conscious knowledge of their
members, a means of fulfilling many of the needs of the individuals in them,
such as:
In a common-bond group, members have face to face interaction and also a strong
bond existsamong them. Players of a team constitute such a common-bond group.
11.4.2 Entitativity
Entitativity is the extent to which groups are perceived as a coherent whole
(Campbell, 1958). Low entitativity groups are a collection of people who happen
to be at same place at same time with a common purpose. Group of interviewees
for a particular job is an example of such group. A group which includes members
of a family is an example of high entitativity group. Groups high on entitativity
have following characteristics:
Frequent interaction among members (either face to face or virtually on
phones, internets, etc.).
Common goals.
Members are highly similar in some ways and are significantly different
from the non-members.
Group is a consequence of its members.
One thing that should be noted here is that, it is not the group size but the
behavioural features such as acceptance of authority, sharing of resources,
adherence to norms, etc. that affects the entitativity of a group (Lickel, Rutchick,
Hamilton & Sherman, 2006). High entitative groups are more stereotyped than
those low on entitative (Yzerbyt, Corneille & Estrade, 2001).
11.5.1 Status
When a teacher enters into the classroom, all students stand in the respect of the
teacher. No significant decision in the family is taken until the head of the family,
generally parents in the Indian families, approve the decision. These examples
characterise status of teachers and parents in their respective groups.
The status of a member may be defined by his/her physical attributes (Judge &
Cable, 2004), usefulness to the purpose of the group and behaviour in accordance
with the norms of group (Haslam, 2004). It may be an official position or rank in
a group or it may be an implicit feature such as elders and new entrants in a
group. Often, status is associated with many privileges in the group, such as
access to resources of the group, respect, salary, influencing other group
members,group responsibilities, making decisions in a group, etc. (Buss, 1999).
It has been found that people with low status are more conforming to the group
norms than those with high status (Jetten, Hornsey &Adarves-Yorno, 2006). By
being more susceptible to group influence, new members, having low status, of
the group attempt to increase their acceptability in the group.
11.5.2 Roles
Different members in a group are designated to perform different functions in
the group. For example, in a cricket team the captain is responsible to lead the
team on the ground, the coach arranges and manages smooth and strategic practice
to the team, the physiotherapist ensures physical fitness of the players and the
manager is assumed to be responsible for the issues pertaining to team travels
and its interaction with media. All these people actually play different roles for
the team or the group of players and officials representing a country at the national
or international level.
Roles define the expected set of behaviours from different members of a group
occupying specific positions in the group. Roles may be officially assigned to a
group member or they may be gradually acquired by a group member without
any formal assignment. A person may fulfil one role in a group while other role
209
Group Dynamics in some other group (Slater, 1955). Often the roles are internalised and gradually
become integral.
A clear recognition of one’s goals, or role clarity, helps to avoid chaos in the
group. Sometimes, however, members may be confused about the things that are
expected of them, such as their level of authority or their responsibility. This is
called role ambiguity and is typically experienced by new members. Some groups
that are pursuing very dynamic and uncertain tasks have very lower degree of
role differentiation and therefore, avoiding the situation of role ambiguity is
extremely challenging in such groups.
11.5.3 Norms
Norms are the implicit rules within a group, guiding how its members should or
should not behave. Norms are generally agreed-upon informal rules that guide
members’ behaviours. They represent shared ways of viewing the world. Norms
differ from formal rules in that they are not formal and written. However, norms
have powerful influence on group behaviour. If each individual in a group
separately decides how to behave in each interaction, no one would be able to
predict the behaviour of any group member and consequently, chaos would reign.
Therefore, norms function as a guide to members’ behaviours and reduce
ambiguity in groups. Groups do not establish norms about every possible situation
but only with respect to things that are significant to the group. Norms might
apply to every member of the group or to only some members. Norms that apply
to particular group members usually specify the role of those individuals. Norms
vary in the degree to which they are accepted by all members of the group: some
are accepted by almost everyone, others by some members and not others.
People are guided by the norms of a group only if they value being the member
of that particular group. In contrast, they may even act contrary to the norms ofa
group with which they do not identify themselves (Jetten, Spears & Manstead,
1997; Moreland & Levine, 2001).
11.5.4 Cohesiveness
Cohesiveness is the force that keeps a group intact (Ellemers, de Gilder &Haslam,
2004). Cohesiveness refers to how strongly group members desire to remain in
their groups. Highly cohesive groups are ones in which the members like one
another, accept the group’s goals, and work towards meeting them. In very un-
cohesive groups, the members dislike one another and may even work at cross-
purposes. In essence, cohesiveness refers to a “we” feeling, a sense of
belongingness to the group. Group cohesion tends to get strengthened under
conditions of high external threat or competition. Cohesiveness generally tends
to be greater when group members spend more time together. Lesser sub-
210
groupings are evident in high cohesive than in low cohesive groups.A group Group Processes
high on this dimension has following qualities (Hogg, 2007; Mullen & Cooper,
1994):
Perceived homogeneity.
More supportive and cooperative for in-groups than with the out-groups.
More oriented towards group goals than toward individual goals.
Have high morale.
Perform better that the groups low on cohesiveness dimension.
Highly adamant towards members of out-groups.
Display more intra-group attraction and commitment towards group goal in
presence of an out-group member.
Self Assessment Questions I
Fill in the blanks-
1) ………………… are the needprovided by the group through friendship
between individuals within it.
3) Norms are the ……………….. within a group, guiding how its members
should or should not behave.
Forming: The members of the group might be unknown to one another when
they meet for the first time.Therefore, initially the group is quite similar to a
crowd. This stage is marked by uncertainties regarding the group purpose, goals,
members, procedures, etc.
Storming: At this stage, there is a lot of conflict regarding various roles and
status of various members and the methods of achieving the group goals. By the
end of this stage some sort of hierarchy develops among the members and some
clarity is obtained regarding how the target has to be achieved.
211
Group Dynamics Norming: At this stage, group members finalise various norms which regulate
their behaviours and roles. Furthermore, division of responsibilities takes place
at this stage. Members devise some scale to measure their progress. Finally, by
the end of this stage the group gets a clear identity.
Performing: This is the execution stage of the group. Since the norms, status,
roles, etc. have been finalised, members fully devote their efforts to achieve the
group goals. For some groups with a single specific purpose this might be the
last stage.
Adjourning: Only few groups have this final stage. Here group is disbanded
after its purpose is achieved. For example,a committee of juries formed to
investigate any case of corruption gets disbanded after submitting its report.
The above mentioned stages are merely a general illustration for the sequential
functioning of a group and therefore, the sequence may be different for different
groups. In a political committee, norms and status of various members may be
pre-decided before the constitution of the group. Similarly, a group may undergo
two stages simultaneously or it may switch back and forth between various stages
or even it may skip any of the stages.
By influencing the group process, inputs have both direct and indirect effects on
group effectiveness. Inputs include resources, such as personnel, task, tools and
time. Groups composed of more competent people, having appropriate knowledge,
skills, and motivation, will on average be more effective than groups with less
competent members. Work groups that are functionally diverse have a larger
stock of ideas to draw upon, and differences in assumptions that allow them to
generate more creative solutions. Members who have expertise in a wide variety
of disciplines have the potential to be highly creative, bringing together old ideas
212 in new ways (Hargadon & Sutton, 1997).
Group Processes
11.8 GROUP PROCESSES
How a person acts in presence of someone else may be completely different
from what he would have been doing, had he been alone. This ‘someone’ may be
a co-worker of her/his group, an audience or a mere stranger. For instance, while
dining out in some restaurant, you may refrain from doing many things which
you would have done when dining at home. There are several influences of group
on an individual called as group processes. Some of them have been mentioned
in the sub headings below.
Price, Harrison and Gavin (2006) observed that there are two factors which
contribute to social loafing: feeling of being dispensable to the group and feeling
of unfairness in the group. When a member feels that the contributions made by
her/him are not essential to the group, then she/he is more likely to loaf.
Conversely, if a member has skills and knowledge relevant to the performance
of group task, she/he is more likely to actively participate in the group activities.
Similarly, if a member feels that she/he may be treated unfairly, even after making
significant contribution, then she/he is more likely to loaf. Also, if a member is
dissimilar from the other members on factors such as age, sex, ethnicity, etc. she/
he is more likely to feel unfairly treated and thus more likely to loaf.
213
Group Dynamics Following techniques may be implied to reduce social loafing:
11.8.3 Deindividuation
Deindividuation is a psychological state characterized by reduced self-awareness
and personal identity, brought on by external conditions such as being an
anonymous member of a large crowd. This leads the members of a crowd to
perform behaviours which they would have never performed when they were
alone.Deindividuation leads to greater obedience of the norms of the crowd
(Postmes & Spears, 1998).
Previously in one of the above section, you were informed about the differences
between group and crowd, where, crowd is merely a collection of people, who
happen to be at the same place at the same time with or without any common
purpose. But this does not mean that crowd cannot have a norm. Consider a
crowd gathered to watch a cricket match. Here the crowd norm is to cheer their
team. Being in a crowd makes a person anonymous and hence they feel less
responsible for their acts. Thus, they are more likely to resort to wild, unrestrained
and anti-social actions (Zimbardo, 1970). Hooliganismis a special case of
deindividuation, where the fans of English soccer team displayed extremely wild
and unsocial behaviour in the matches involving English team.
There are two factors which contribute to this phenomenon of group polarisation.
First, in order to prove themselves a worthy and loyal, the members of group
hold a view which is in line with the group’s overall image and simultaneously
extreme than others. For example, in a terrorist group extreme ideas to create
chaos will be considered more worthy. Hence members will tryto hold views
which are more extreme than others. Second, due to persuasion, a group’s initial
preference gets strengthenedleading to extreme decisions.
Reward Structure: A reward structure that considers only group effort, promotes
cooperation in the group. Such a reward structure endorses interdependence
among the members. Contrarily, if the reward structure considers only individual
efforts, it will promote competition among the members of the group. Suppose
in the above example of cricket match, if the condition is set in such a way that
the Man of the Match award will be given to the player of winning team only
then players will first try to achieve the team target instead of focussing on their
individual performance.
Initially the poor performance of the group followed by the negative feedback
may force the members to blame each other for the group failure. This
ultimately generates conflict among them. Studies have been reported
showing that more the amount of negative feedback received by the group,
higher the level of conflict reported (Peterson & Behfar, 2003).
Perceived inequity with respect to work share and reward distribution also
leads to conflict among the group members.
Feeling that the other group does not respect norms of our group may also
lead to conflict.
11.12 COMMUNICATION
Communication is a process of interpersonal interaction in which thoughts, ideas,
emotions and understanding between sender(s) and receiver(s) are exchanged
(Guo and Sanchez, 2005). This simple definition of communication directs
attention to three important issues:
Communication involves transmission and reception of messages.
Communication involves people, at least two: one to transmit the message
(sender) and another to receive the message (receiver).
Though verbal and non verbal communications are separate, both operate at the
same time. The verbal part of a message conveys content or information. The
non verbal component indicates how the verbal message should be interpreted
and thus it is meta-communication. Whenever verbal and non verbal messages
contradict each other, people are more likely to believe the non verbal.
219
Group Dynamics 11.12.3 Barriers to Effective Communication
Anything that filters, blocks or distorts the message or the information during
the process of “encoding-sending-decoding” between the sender and the receiver
is considered as barriers of communication. Barriers of communication can be
broadly classified into two types (Longest, Rakich, & Darr, 2000):
4) The tendency of a group to shift towards more extreme decisions than those
initially held by its members is known as group polarisation. ( )
A group passes through various phases of development which turn it into a full-
fledged functional entity. These phases are forming, storming, norming,
performing. Few groups are disbanded after the purpose of their formation is
solved. Such groups have an additional phase of adjourning.The effectiveness
of a group has to be viewed in terms of achievement of productivity goals, ability
to adapt to changes, satisfaction of personal needs of the members, including a
220
sense of status and socialized power and maintenance of the required level of Group Processes
communication, co-operation and sense of accomplishment.
Since, members of a group frequently interact with other members of the group;
their behaviour is quite different from what it would have been in isolation.
According to drive theory of social facilitation, presence of others when we
perform increases our arousal level which may either improve or impair our
performance. Similarly, distraction conflict theory of social facilitation states
that others’ presence leads to cognitive overload resulting into altered
performance. When group is involved in some task, a tendency among the
members is to rely on others’ efforts and contribute less. This propensity is known
as social loafing. This reduces the groups’ overall productivity.
Since groups are formed for some specific motives, they are frequently involved
in various decision-making processes. However, often we mistakenly consider
that the decisions taken by groups are always better than those taken by an
individual. Sometimes group may be victim of group polarization and thus they
may take highly extreme decisions. In this condition, the group members lean
towards group’s initial position so intensely that the final decision is an extreme
one. Another tendency of a highly cohesive group is to consider their group to be
always correct and hence they display a tendency of group think where they
think that the group’s decision can never be wrong. Other vulnerabilities in group’s
decision can be improper sharing of information.
Recurrent interaction among the group members may cultivate tendencies like
cooperation, competition and conflicts. Cooperation occurs when group works
in unison to achieve the common goal of the group. Competition in the group is
observed when members give priority to their individual interests over the group
goals. In the case of competition, one member can achieve the target only if the
other members fail to attain that. Conflict is a realisation of fact that others’
interests are incompatible to that of ours which may or may not be true.
Communication is a relevant process and affects the group interactions and
processes.
6) Explain various causes and outcomes of conflict in a group. How can conflict
be resolved?
Formal and informal groups: A formal group has explicitly stated rules and
functions,definite roles for its members and clearly specified norms. In contrast,
an informal group does not have any explicitly stated rules and regulation.
Norms: The implicit rules within a group guiding how its members should or
should not behave.
Cohesiveness: The force that keeps a group intact.
Group effectiveness: Viewed in terms of achievement of productivity goals,
ability to adapt to changes and satisfaction of personal needs of the members in
order to sustain the group as a team.
Group think: Assumption of the embers of a highly cohesive group think that
their decisions cannot be wrong and all the members must support the decision
taken by the group.
Cooperation: Behaviour in which whole group works together to attain the shared
222 goal.
Competition: Behaviour in which individual tries to excel his/her own Group Processes
performance and simultaneously hindering others in completing their tasks.
Super-ordinate goals: The goals, usually superior to the conflict interest, which
both sides in a conflict need to achieve.
References
Baron, R. A. (1990). Attributions and organizational conflict. In S. Graha& V.
Folkes (Eds.), Attribution theory: Applications to achievement, mental health,
and interpersonal conflict (pp. 185–204). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
223
Group Dynamics Brickner, M., Harkins, S., & Ostrom, T. (1986). Personal involvement: Thought
provoking implications for social loafing. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 51, 763-769.
Buss, D.M. (1999). Evolutionary Psychology: The New Science of the Mind.
Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Campbell, D. T. (1958). Common fate, similarity, and other indices of the status
of aggregates of persons as social entities. Behavioral Science, 4, 14-25.
Dasgupta, N., Banji, M. R. & Abelson, R. P. (1999). Group entiativity and group
perception: Association between physical features and psychological judgment.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 991-1005.
DeDreu, C. K. W., & Van Lange, P. A. M. (1995). Impact of social value orientation
on negotiator cognition and behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
21, 1178-1188.
Ellemers, N., de Gilder, D., & Haslam, S. A. (2004). Motivating individuals and
groups at work: A social identity perspective on leadership and group performance.
Academy of Management Review, 29, 459-478.
224
Jetten, J., Spears, R., &Manstead, A. S. R. (1997). Strength of identification and Group Processes
intergroup differentiation: The influence of group norms. European Journal of
Social Psychology, 27, 603-609.
Judge, T. A., & Cable, T. A. (2004). The effect of physical height on workplace
success and income: Preliminary test of a theoretical model. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 89, 428-441.
Keltner, D., & Robinson, R. J. (1996). Extremism, power, and the imagined basis
of social conflict. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 5,101-105.
Lickel, B., Rutchick, A. M., Hamilton, D. L., & Sherman, S. J. (2006). Intuitive
theories of group types and relational principles. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 42, 28-39.
Mullen, B., & Cooper, C. (1994). The relation between group cohesiveness and
performance: An integration. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 210-227.
Price, K. H., Harrison, D. A., & Gavin, J. H. (2006). Withholding inputs in team
contexts: Member composition, interaction processes, evaluation structure, and
social loafing. journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 1375-1384.
Williams, K. D., & Karau, S. J. (1991). Social loafing and social compensation:
The effects of expectations of co-worker performance. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 61, 570-581.
Yzerbyt, V. Y., Corneille, O., & Estrada, C. (2001). The interplay of subjective
essentialism and entitativity in the formation of stereotypes. Personality and
Social Psychology Review, 5, 141-155.
Zimbardo, P.G. (1970). The human choice: Individuation, reason, and order versus
deindividuation, impulse, and chaos. In W.J. Arnold & D. Levine (Eds.), Nebraska
Symposium on Motivation (Vol. 17, pp. 237–307). Lincoln, NE: University of
Nebraska Press.
226
Group Processes
UNIT 12 LEADER AND LEADERSHIP*
Structure
12.0 Objectives
12.1 Introduction
12.2 Concept and Role of Leader
12.3 Characteristics of leader,
12.4 Styles of leadership,
12.5 Social categorization,
12.6 Role of a Leader in Conflict Resolution
12.7 Let Us Sum Up
12.8 Unit End Questions
12.9 Glossary
12.10 Answers to Self Assessment Questions
12.11 Suggested Readings and References
12.0 OBJECTIVES
After reading this Unit, the learner will be able to :
Describe the Characteristics of leader;
Explain types of leadership;
Summarize the social categorization; and
Explain role of a leader in conflict resolution.
12.1 INTRODUCTION
Imagine that you were living in the 18th century, when social issues such as the
Sati system troubled the entire India, then how would you have enacted to stop
the system? That was the time when Raja Ram Mohan Roy retired from the East
India Company and was always interested in reforming the society. He worked
very hard to improve people’s conditions and did a great job in the field of social,
religious and educational work. His active contribution and leadership skills
towards prohibiting the Sati system is best recalled.
Who do you consider are really great leaders? The names that always come to
your mind are all political leaders: Nelson Mandela, Mahatma Gandhi, Indira
Gandhi and Narendra Modi. Some names such as Adof Hitler and Kim Jong-un
also come to our mind.
Many leaders are visionaries, some use their power of authority and others force
us to do as they want. It is not difficult to see the burden in all of this for making
enormous changes in our lives. It is interesting fact that the study of leader and
leadership has held a central role for over 75 years in social psychology.In unit
11 you have learned about Group Dynamics. In this unit, you will learn about the
characteristics of leader, types of leadership, social categorization and role of a
leader in conflict resolution.
* Dr. Mohsin Uddin, Consultant, Discipline of Psychology, IGNOU, Delhi 227
Group Dynamics
12.2 CONCEPT AND ROLE OF LEADER
The leader is the person who has the greatest impact on the group actions and
beliefs. She or he is the individual who initiates action, gives orders, makes
decisions, addresses conflicts between group members, provides inspiration,
serves as a model, and is at the heart of any of the operational activities.
On the other hand, within certain groups, there is no formal leader at all.
Friendship/ Board or Commotions groups show a trend of informal leadership.
In group discussions, one member may be more fluent and convincing than others
and may therefore have more effect on decisions making and conflicts resolution.
Many tasks in the group are handled by a leader. The fundamental roles of the
leader differ from the form of formal or informal group being headed. If we go
through the different opinions expressed by the various social and organizational
psychologists, we will certainly infer that leaders typically perform any or all of
the following roles in group decision making and during conflicts resolution.
To encourage the members for better communication.
To inspire the members of the group to obey the law.
Acting as a bridge between the group and other formal and informal
institutions.
Monitoring and enhancing the efficiency of the group.
To cultivate interpersonal relationships between group.
To settle disputes between members of the group.
To give the group strategic direction.
In different locations and places to represent the groups.
Expressing and engaging with the group goal.
Secure productivity of group effort.
Recruit new members and trained the members in core competencies.
Developing team spirit.
ii) Communication
Leadership and successful communication are intertwined. Leader need to be
able to communicate in a variety of ways, from transmitting information to
coaching the group members. In addition, through occupations, social identities,
and more, a wide range of member must be able to listen to, and communicate
with others. The quality and efficacy of communication within group directly
affects the effectiveness of group cohesiveness, groupthink and plan, too.
iv) Self-esteem
To the degree to which a person is at peace with himself and has a good overall
assessment of her or his self-worth and skill, any individual is considered
appropriate to be a leader. Leaders with high self-esteem respect their followers
more.Higher levels of self-confidence can affect those with high self-esteem and
this affects their image in the eyes of their followers. Self-esteem can also explain
the correlation between certain physical attributes and the emergence of leaders.
v) Intelligence
Either aleader is appointed or elected, intelligence is one of the important attribute
of leadership.
vi) Empathy
A leader has enough open-mindedness to understand their followers’ motives,
dreams, expectations, and challenges so that they can build a profound emotional
touch with them.
vii) Maturity
A leader should have a high degree of emotional quotient. She or He needs to
maintain her/his temper of calmness. She or He must be highly tolerant. She or
He should be open minded to explore fresh ideas. She/ He should be able to look
at situations logically.
xi) Passion
Leaders should feel a passion for their work and show it. The buy-in of members
would be stronger if their leader indicates that they care for the job they do.
xii) Influence
Great leaders will positively affect their workers. Earn the organisation ‘s
confidence and appreciation so that you can inspire them to work hard, stay
optimistic and show ingenuity.
2) Participative Style
Participative leadership is a form of leadership in which all group members
working together for taking decisions. Participative leadership is also known as
democratic leadership, where everyone is supposed to participate.
7) Transformational leadership
Transformational leadership, unlike other leadership types, is more about
facilitating change in organisations, groups, oneself and others. Transformational
leaders inspire others to do more than they initially expected and sometimes
much more than they considered feasible. They set more difficult goal and
generally achieve a higher result. Statistically, transformational leadership tends
to have more committed and satisfied followers. This is mostly so because
followers are motivated by representatives of change.
8) Transactional Leadership
Transactional Leadership is a theory or style first explored by sociologist Max
Weber in 1947 and subsequently extended by Bernard M. Bass, who also played
a leading role in the growth of Transformational Leadership.It is leadership that
requires an exchange process in which members receive immediate, benefits
for carrying out the instructions of the leader.
9) Bureaucratic Leadership
Bureaucratic leadership refers to organisational leadership through a highly
formalised set of procedures, protocols, and systems. Laws , policies and
hierarchies here set up a straightforward set of objectives as well as an explicit
chain of command. At each level of a bureaucracy, organisational members are
obliged both to their immediate subordinates and to a wider ecosystem of rules
and procedures. Through channelling defined laws, implementing current systems,
and ruling over particular parts of the hierarchy, bureaucratic leaders lead.
231
Group Dynamics 11) Socio-emotional Leadership
A socio-emotional leadership style involves a leader, who, by driving away fears,
reducing tension, resolving conflicts, and settling arguments and disagreements,
is concerned with raising the morale of his or her group at a high level.
For instance, when we meet a new teacher, we could classify his as men, a middle-
aged man, an Asian, an academic, and so on. Social categorization is a natural
feature of social cognition; it provides a mental shortcut in that it helps us to
infer an individual’s properties based on the properties of others in the same
community.
232
Social categorization is the distinction between oneself and others and between Leader and Leadership
one’s own group and other groups, and it is such a natural and spontaneous
phenomenon that we are not aware of it often.
A third perspective on the origins of stereotyping and prejudice begins with this
basic fact that people generally divide the social world into two distinct categories
Us and Them–social categorization Ingroup is US and the Outgroup is THEM
(Charles Sumner, 1906, Page 12). Persons in the Ingroupare perceived favorably
while those in the latter are perceived more negatively. This tendency to make
more favorable and flattering attributions about members of one’s group than
members about the other group is known as the ultimate attribution error.
Evidence from research suggests that by identifying with certain special classes,
people seek to boost their self-esteem. And so the final outcome is inevitable, as
each group seeks to see itself better than the rivals. Thus, dividing our group into
two opposing groups is one of the significant causes of stereotyping and
discrimination.
At work and in groups, nobody likes conflict. It can fester and affect group morale,
interpersonal relationships , and work efficiency if not addressed properly and in
due time. A lot of research has been done to identify measures for better dispute
resolution that are time-efficient and impactful.
Researcher Kenneth Thomas and Ralph Kilmann developed a model for conflict
resolution in the 1970s. Following them, it was called the Thomas-Kilmann model.
The word ‘conflict’ is defined as the situation under this model in which the
concerns of people can not be contrasted with others. If two or more individuals
or groups care about items that are conflicting with each other, then conflict is
the result.
233
Group Dynamics 1) Competing
The first Thomas-Kilmann conflict mode, which is competing, is assertive and
non-cooperative. It refers to answering only one’s own issues at the detriment of
the other’s concerns. It is a power-oriented mode that uses whatever dynamic of
power seems necessary to achieve a desirable outcome for itself.
The willingness of a person to argue, their place in the hierarchy, or their financial
power matter the most. It strictly means standing up for your own convictions
and actually striving to win. Competing is defensive.
2) Accommodating
According to the Thomas-Kilmann model, the Accommodating Mode is both
accepting and cooperative. That’s the opposite of being competing. While
welcoming, the individual in question neglects their own interests or values to
tackle the issues of the other party. The self-sacrifice element is emphasised in
this mode. Usually, accommodating requires selfless comprehension, generosity,
or charity. At times, accommodating would require you to obey the instructions
of the other person if you would not like to do so, or adhere to the opinion or
decisions of the other person.
3) Avoiding
Avoiding is both unassertive and uncooperative in the Thomas-Kilmann model.
The person does not want to tackle his or her own problems or others’ problems.
This essentially suggests that they do not want to participate in the dispute at all.
At times, avoidance may be seen as a diplomatic move involving bypassing or
ignoring the issue. It may also mean putting the problem away until the time is
right, or simply standing back from an unpleasant or unsafe situation.
4) Collaborating
In the Thomas-Kilmann conflict model, working together is the most
advantageous outcome. It is assertive as well as cooperative. The complete
opposite of avoiding is this mode. Collaborating requires a voluntary attempt to
work together with the opposition to find a perfect solution that solves the
collective issue entirely. Collaborating requires deep-diving into a dilemma to
define the critical criteria of the members or group involved. A desire to understand
the ‘why’ of the dispute could take the form of collaborating between two or
more individuals. It means trying to look for interesting solutions to interpersonal
problems and enriching yourself from the experiences of the other members.
5) Compromising
In the Thomas-Kilmann conflict model, the last outcome falls on both the
assertiveness and cooperativeness scales on the average point. The aim here is to
find a mutually satisfactory and robust solution that satisfies both individuals in
some way.
234
To sum up the modes of conflict resolution: Leader and Leadership
12.9 GLOSSARY
Leader: The leader is the person who has the greatest impact on the group actions
and beliefs.
Consultative: Consultative leaders regularly ask members for input and take
members complaints seriously.
Hopps, J.G. and Pinderhughes, E.B. (1999). Group work with overwhelmed
clients, New York : Free Press. Krech, Crutchfield and Ballachy (1962). Individual
and Society, McGraw Hill, New York.
Krech, Crutchfield and Ballacy (1962). Individual and Society, McGraw Hill,
New York.
237