SchimmentiDevelopmentalrootsofdissociationpre Print
SchimmentiDevelopmentalrootsofdissociationpre Print
SchimmentiDevelopmentalrootsofdissociationpre Print
net/publication/301717630
CITATIONS READS
65 1,835
1 author:
Adriano Schimmenti
Kore University of Enna
232 PUBLICATIONS 8,156 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by Adriano Schimmenti on 29 January 2017.
Note: This is a pre-print version of the article. The final version can be found in the
Sciences, UKE – Kore University of Enna, Cittadella Universitaria, 94100, Enna, Italy.
E-mail: [email protected].
DEVELOPMENTAL ROOTS OF DISSOCIATION 2
ABSTRACT
The current study was aimed to test a developmental model of dissociation.
This model is based on clinical observations and research findings
concerning the role of childhood emotional neglect in the development and
maintenance of dissociative symptoms. Seven hundred ninety-two adult
volunteers completed questionnaires on parental bonding, theory of mind,
alexithymia, and dissociation. Significant associations were found between
the investigated variables, and a multiple mediation analysis showed that the
relationship between childhood emotional neglect and dissociation was
totally mediated by theory of mind and alexithymia. The findings of this
study support the view that childhood experiences of emotional neglect may
foster difficulties mentalizing as well as problems with affect regulation,
with these two factors interacting to generate excessively activated
dissociative processes. This may suggest that individuals who were exposed
to emotional neglect during their childhood and who currently suffer from
dissociative symptoms may greatly benefit from clinical interventions aimed
to foster mentalized affectivity.
mechanism: a process by which behaviors, thoughts, memories, and feelings split from
one another (Kluft, 1990a; O’Neil, 2006; Vaillant, 1994). Dissociation is already
available in the early stages of development (Porges, 2001), and it allows an individual
2009). However, it has been postulated that experiences of abuse and neglect in the
neglectful parenting styles can indeed lead the child, whose capacities for self-
regulation and cognitive processing of affective states are not fully developed, to
and unbearable mental states arising from such negative interpersonal experiences
(Kluft, 1990b, 2000; van der Kolk, 2014; Putnam, 1997; Schimmenti & Caretti, 2016).
At this point, one’s entire psychic functioning might become organized around
Decades of clinical and empirical research have shown that child abuse and
neglect are strongly related to dissociative symptoms (Kluft, 1985, 1990a, 2013;
Dalenberg et al., 2012). In fact, an excessive activation of dissociation has been linked
to the dysregulation of affect metabolism derived from child maltreatment (Lanius et al.,
2010), which may indeed generate significant alterations in the network of cortical and
DEVELOPMENTAL ROOTS OF DISSOCIATION 5
subcortical interactions that foster the development of self-awareness and the ability to
organize mental and affective states (Schimmenti, 2012). These alterations might
involve atypical development of the right brain, the hippocampus, the prefrontal cortex,
sensitive to parental care. It is rather clear that such alterations may undermine the
might help explain why many psychiatric disorders, and not only dissociative disorders,
have been empirically linked with dissociation. For example, high levels of dissociation
have been found to be related to post-traumatic stress disorder (Carlson, Dalenberg, &
& Dogan, 2004), personality disorders (Zanarini, Frankenburg, Jager-Hyman, Reich, &
Fitzmaurice, 2008), substance use disorders (Najavits & Walsh, 2012), and even
processes were usually identified with childhood experiences of sexual abuse. This
individuals with multiple personality disorder who had suffered from child sexual abuse
in the context of attachment relationships were described (e.g., Bowman, Blix, & Coons,
1985; Coons, 1986; Kluft, 1987, 1990a; Malenbaum & Russell, 1987; Ross, Norton, &
more distinct personalities alternately take control of an individual. In this disorder, the
effects of extreme dissociation are clearly represented; a person’s entire identity, not
only its functions, is split and fractionated into separate parts with their own distinct
personality.
On the one hand, a renewed interest in the study of multiple personality disorder
childhood sexual abuse on an individual’s development (Kluft, 1990a, b); on the other
hand, focusing on the effects of a single and specific form of child maltreatment (sexual
paradoxically slowed down the understanding of the complex relationships between the
overly activated dissociative processes (Chefetz, 2015; Liotti, 1999). As a matter of fact,
in many cases child sexual abuse co-occurs with other adverse experiences, such as
parental loss, financial hardship, social isolation, violence between parents, rejection,
neglect, parentification, physical abuse, and psychological abuse (Bifulco & Moran,
1998).
which other forms of child maltreatment, including sexual abuse from parents, can
develop and generate psychologically venomous fruits (Schimmenti & Bifulco, 2015).
that a child needs comfort or attention; it covers a number of parental behaviors that
imply a failure or refusal to provide needed psychological care (e.g., rejection of the
child, failure to express positive feelings to the child, hindering the development of
DEVELOPMENTAL ROOTS OF DISSOCIATION 7
autonomy, withholding love, and denying the child opportunities for interacting; see
Glaser, 2002; Minty, 2015). In this sense, emotional neglect can be conceptualized as a
field in which the child’s affective needs are disowned; as a result, such needs are
subdued to parental demands, desires, conflicts, fears, and projections (Schimmenti &
considerations. Indeed, research has shown that increased dissociation scores are more
related to the quality of child–caregiver interactions than to child abuse. For example,
Ogawa, Sroufe, Weinfield, Carlson, & Egeland (1997) found in a sample of 168 late
adolescents aged 18–19 years that the psychological unavailability of the caregiver
during the adolescents’ first two years of life, lack of caregiver attention, and
It is noteworthy that the psychological unavailability of the caregiver was the single
childhood, surprisingly, did not add to the prediction in the study by Ogawa and
followed a group of 56 late adolescents from infancy to age 19 and found that observed
prediction. In the study by Dutra and colleagues, the other forms of abuse were not
It could be useful here to highlight that such findings do not suggest that
emotional neglect is the only factor leading to high levels of dissociation; they suggest
DEVELOPMENTAL ROOTS OF DISSOCIATION 8
instead that among the many types of child maltreatment, those related with the
attune with the child’s needs are the most critical variables to take into account for
predicting increased levels of dissociation. Such findings are consistent with the more
general consideration that while physical and sexual abuse in childhood may be
episodic, emotional neglect and lack of parental responsiveness may characterize the
entire developmental environment (Schimmenti & Bifulco, 2015; Infurna et al., 2016).
Children need to perceive positive feelings of being cared for, accepted, and protected
in their family environments in order to grow up secure and healthy (Bowlby, 1988),
and the lack of such positive feelings may constitute a developmental trauma for the
child (Schimmenti & Caretti, 2016), which could undermine his or her developing sense
neglect and dissociation are still under debate. Recent theoretical contributions in the
psychoanalytic field have suggested that lack of mentalization and affect dysregulation
are crucial constructs to take into account in the understanding of this relationship. For
example, Fonagy and colleagues (2002) observed that childhood trauma may inhibit the
internal states, i.e., the capacity to organize the understanding of one’s own and others’
experience in terms of mental state constructs (beliefs, feelings, desires, intentions, and
Ensink, Fonagy, Berthelot, Normandin, & Bernazzani, 2015). As a result, the normal
acquisition of an adequate theory of mind (i.e., the ability to attribute and interpret one’s
own and other people’s mental and emotional states, such as beliefs, intents, desires,
perspectives, pretending, knowledge; see Baron-Cohen, Leslie, & Frith, 1985) might be
hindered for these children. Theory of mind is critical for mentalizing processes,
because it constitutes the basic capacity allowing one to interpret reality at both
cognitive and affective levels. Notably, research has shown that the overall accuracy in
identifying complex internal states is reduced in people who have been neglected or
abused in childhood (Nazarov et al., 2014; Pears & Fisher, 2005), even though they
could be even more accurate than other people in the process of identifying negative
feelings (Wagner & Linehann, 1999). This likely happens because their interpersonal
functioning has been sensitized early to threats and dangers, thus their mind has been
forced to operate as a “smoke detector” (van der Kolk, McFarlane & Weisaeth, 1996)
for avoiding further trauma, which is a double-edged sword because while on the one
hand this condition may provide some degree of insulation against overwhelming
stressors (Kluft, 1990b), on the other hand it may also generate many errors in
Schimmenti and Caretti (2016) complemented the observations on the role of lack
resulting from child abuse and neglect. Since self-regulation abilities are limited during
DEVELOPMENTAL ROOTS OF DISSOCIATION 10
Parental unavailability, rejection, and abuse force children to use their underdeveloped
2015b). Thus, children may try to repair the relational failures and to re-establish the
attachment bond through the hyperactivation of the attachment system, which may lead
process involving a metabolic shutdown that serves to conserve energies and to foster
survival (Schimmenti & Caretti, 2016). These two mechanisms likely represent the
children to survive maltreatment, but at the same time it will also hinder the
and Caretti (2016) suggested a linkage between the affect dysregulation deriving from
and a paucity of fantasies and dreams (Nemiah, Freyberger, & Sifneos, 1976; Taylor &
Bagby, 2013; Taylor, Bagby, & Parker, 1997). Although alexithymia is a normally
distributed trait in the community (Parker, Keefer, Taylor, & Bagby, 2008), early
1976) and many empirical findings (e.g., Bermond, Vorst, & Moorman, 2006; Güleç,
Altintaş, İnanç, Bezgin, Koca, & Güleç, 2014; Schimmenti et al., 2015a) support the
view that the presence of increased levels of alexithymia is related to trauma exposure
in the individual’s life. In particular, Krystal (1988, 1997) argued on the basis of his
DEVELOPMENTAL ROOTS OF DISSOCIATION 11
development due to psychic trauma in early childhood, and that it can also emerge from
Therefore, it is possible that the likelihood of having low levels of theory of mind
and high levels of alexithymia is increased when parental failures of care have occurred
during one’s childhood, and that theory of mind and alexithymia act as mediators in the
relationship between the quality of care received during childhood and the dissociative
experiences of individuals.
The current study aimed to investigate the relationships between theory of mind,
alexithymia, dissociation, and the quality of the relationship with parents during
childhood in a large adult sample in order to integrate the developmental and clinical
alexithymia, and childhood emotional neglect were intercorrelated and that a multiple
neglect and dissociation would be mediated by theory of mind and alexithymia, would
To this purpose, the associations between these variables were examined, and a
multiple mediation model was tested. The model configuring the investigated variables
----------
DEVELOPMENTAL ROOTS OF DISSOCIATION 12
----------
The model in Figure 1 predicts that childhood emotional neglect will be positively
related to dissociation, and that theory of mind and alexithymia will mediate this
predictive association. In detail, the model predicts that the scores on childhood
emotional neglect will be negatively related to theory of mind scores and positively
related to alexithymia scores, that theory of mind scores will decrease dissociation
scores whereas alexithymia scores will increase dissociation scores, and that a
childhood emotional neglect and dissociation scores will become not significant after
the inclusion of theory of mind scores and alexithymia scores in the model. Moreover,
the relationship between theory of mind scores and alexithymia scores should further
contribute to the relationship between the scores on childhood emotional neglect and
dissociation scores, such that higher scores on theory of mind should be negatively
related to alexithymia scores and should decrease the impact of alexithymia scores on
dissociation scores.
specification of the more general trauma model of dissociation, which postulates that
dissociative symptoms and dissociative disorders are directly and indirectly related to
traumatic experiences (Dalenberg et al., 2012, 2014). In fact, it has been theoretically
stressed in the introduction of this article that child abuse and neglect can be extremely
traumatizing because on the psychobiological, affective, and cognitive levels they are
too much for a child to tolerate (Schimmenti & Caretti, 2016). So, in this model the high
DEVELOPMENTAL ROOTS OF DISSOCIATION 13
scores for dissociation are considered a result of real experiences of parental emotional
neglect.
called the fantasy model or sociocognitive model (Spanos, 1996), in which dissociation
reverse causation hypothesis for the model depicted in Figure 1 would be true, i.e.,
dissociation, theory of mind, and alexithymia would predict the perception of the
model of dissociation has little empirical support (Dalenberg et al., 2014), it has many
endorsers among clinicians, so the prediction deriving from this competing theoretical
model was checked to partially rule out the possibility of a reverse causation.
Method
Participants
The study sample includes 792 Caucasian adults (358 males, 45.2%; 434 females,
54.8%), recruited in five cities of Sicily, Italy. Participants ranged in age from 18 to 64
years old (M = 35.75, SD = 10.95). The mean number of years of education was 13.09
(SD = 3.40). There were no gender differences in relation to participant age (t(790)=.65, p
Procedures
Participants were recruited between April 2013 and June 2014 through public
different sizes and populations. People who contacted the research office were informed
about the nature of the study, and those who agreed to participate signed an informed
DEVELOPMENTAL ROOTS OF DISSOCIATION 14
participated in this study. Seven participants (0.9%) reported the lack of a mother or a
father figure during their childhood, so their responses were excluded from the analysis.
Participants did not receive any compensation for their involvement in the study. At the
end of the study, they were debriefed and thanked. The study was approved by the IRB
Measures
Parental Bonding Instrument (PBI; Parker, Tulping, & Brown, 1979). The PBI is
a self-report measure to assess parenting behaviors. Respondents are asked to recall how
their parents acted toward them during the first 16 years of their life, rating their
point Likert scale (0 to 3). Twelve of the 25 items concern the Care dimension (e.g.,
parental affection, warmth, support, and availability. Care scores range from 0 to 36 for
both mother and father, with higher scores indicating higher care by the parent. The
remaining 13 items concern the Overprotection dimension (e.g., “Tried to make me feel
control, infantilization of the child, and lack of support for the development of
autonomy in the child. Overprotection scores can range from 0 to 39 for both mother
and father, with higher scores indicating more controlling behaviors by the parent.
The psychometric properties of the PBI are good, with good internal consistency, high
test-retest reliability, and considerable support for its validity as a measure of actual
parenting in both clinical and non-clinical samples (Parker, 1989). The Italian
translation of the PBI (Scinto, Marinangeli, Kalyvoka, Daneluzzo, & Rossi, 1999) was
DEVELOPMENTAL ROOTS OF DISSOCIATION 15
used in this study, and consistent with previous research (Schimmenti, Maganuco,
neglect was calculated by subtracting the sum of PBI maternal and paternal care scores
from the sum of PBI maternal and paternal overprotection scores, that is: [(PBI
father’s care)]. Therefore, higher scores on this index indicate higher exposure to
parental emotional neglect during childhood. Cronbach’s alphas of the PBI scales in this
study were .89 for mother’s care, .91 for father’s care, .82 for mother’s control, and .82
Likert scale). Items assess the percentage of time individuals experience these
symptoms (e.g., “Some people have the experience of looking in a mirror and not
recognizing themselves. Circle the number to show what percentage of the time this
happens to you.”). The overall score of the DES-II can range from 0% to 100%, and is
the average score obtained by adding up the 28 item scores and dividing by 28. The
psychometric properties of the DES-II are good, with excellent internal consistency,
good test-retest reliability, and good convergent validity (van Ijzendoorn & Schuengel,
1996). The Italian translation of the measure (Schimmenti, 2015a) was used in the
current study to assess dissociation. Cronbach’s alpha of the DES-II in this study
was .94.
DEVELOPMENTAL ROOTS OF DISSOCIATION 16
Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test – Revised Version (Eyes Test; Baron-Cohen,
Wheelwright, Hill, Raste, & Plumb, 2001). The Eyes Test was used to assess theory of
mind. The Eyes Test is a performance task in which participants are presented with a
series of 36 photographs of the eye region alone, and they are required to select which
of four mental state descriptors (a target word and three foil words) best describe the
thoughts or feelings expressed by the eyes of the individuals in the pictures. A glossary
of the mental state words is provided for participants to consult at any point if required.
One point is assigned for each correct response. Scores on the Eyes Test can range from
0 to 36, with higher scores indicating higher levels of theory of mind. The Eyes Test has
reliability, and good convergent validity (Vellante et al., 2013). The Italian translation
of the measure (Serafin & Surian, 2004) was used in the current study to assess theory
of mind. Cronbach’s alpha of the Eyes Test in this study was .64.
20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20; Bagby, Parker, & Taylor, 1994;
Bagby, Taylor, & Parker, 1994). The TAS-20 is a 20-item self-report measure for the
assessment of alexithymia. Participants are asked how much they agree (1 = strongly
disagree; 5 = strongly agree) with each statement (e.g., “I have feelings that I can’t quite
identify”). The TAS-20 total score is the sum of responses to all 20 items, so it can
range from 20 to 100. Higher scores indicate higher levels of alexithymia. The TAS-20
has good reliability and validity, and has a 3-factor structure that is consistent across
countries (Taylor & Bagby, 2013). The Italian translation of the measure (Bressi et al.,
1997) was used in the current study to assess alexithymia. Cronbach’s alpha of TAS-20
Data analysis
DEVELOPMENTAL ROOTS OF DISSOCIATION 17
Descriptive statistics were computed for all the variables investigated in the study.
Gender differences were examined through t-test for independent samples. Associations
between the variables were examined though Pearson’s r correlations. The proposed
multiple mediation model was examined using the Process Macro for SPSS (Hayes,
2013), applying Model 6 with 5,000 bias-corrected bootstrap samples and controlling
for sociodemographic variables (i.e., gender, age, and years of education). Given the
cross-sectional nature of the study, Model 6 of the Process Macro was also applied to
test for the reverse causation model, i.e., to partially rule out the possibility that the
theory of mind, and alexithymia scores, in accordance with the sociocognitive model of
dissociation. A p value of .05 was set as the critical level for statistical significance (for
bootstrap analyses, if the 95% confidence interval includes 0, then the effect is not
significant, if 0 is not in the interval, then the effect is statistically significant; see Hayes,
2013).
Results
Descriptive statistics are presented in Table 1 for the full sample and
----------
----------
The participants’ scores were all in the normal range. There were no gender
significantly lower scores than females on theory of mind. Males were also less
DEVELOPMENTAL ROOTS OF DISSOCIATION 18
The intercorrelations between the investigated variables (all df=790) are reported
in Table 2. All of the associations between the variables were in the expected direction.
DES-II scores were negatively correlated with the Eyes Test scores, and positively
correlated with the TAS-20 scores and with the scores on childhood emotional neglect.
Furthermore, DES-II scores were negatively correlated to age and years of education.
The associations between theory of mind, alexithymia, and childhood emotional neglect
were also in the predicted direction. The scores on childhood emotional neglect were
negatively correlated with the Eyes Test scores and positively correlated with the TAS-
20 scores. There was a negative correlation between the Eyes Test scores and TAS-20
scores. .
----------
----------
Finally, the multiple mediation model presented in Figure 1 was tested. All
variables were sufficiently normal in their distributions to render the statistics involved
in the mediation analyses valid. However, the predictor and the mediators were mean-
calculated, and 5,000 bias-corrected bootstrap samples were used in order to minimize
The results of multiple mediation analyses entirely supported the model presented
in Figure 1. After controlling for gender (males coded as 1, female as 2; t=-.96, p=.34,
n.s.), age (t=-8.15, p<.01; B=-.31, SE=.04, CI[-.39, -.24]), and years of education (t=-
DEVELOPMENTAL ROOTS OF DISSOCIATION 19
2.07, p=.04.; B=-.27, SE=.13, CI[-.52, -.01]), the scores on childhood emotional neglect
were negative predictors of Eyes Test scores (t=-3.81, p<.01; B=-.03, SE =.01, CI[-.04,-
.01]) and positive predictors of TAS-20 scores (t=8.87, p<.01; B=.15, SE=.02,
CI[.11, .18]). Eyes Test scores were negative predictors of TAS-20 scores (t=-7.30,
p<.01; B=-.60, SE =.08, CI[-.77, -.44]). Both Eyes Test scores and TAS-20 scores
predicted DES-II scores in the model, but in opposite directions: Eyes Test scores were
negative predictors of DES-II scores (t=-6.05, p<.01; B=-.70, SE=.12, CI[-.93, -.47]),
whereas TAS-20 scores were positive predictors of DES-II scores (t=7.17, p<.01; B=.30,
SE=.04, CI[.22, .38]). The previously significant and positive association between the
scores on childhood emotional neglect and DES-II scores (t=4.59, p<.01; B=.10, SE=.02,
CI[.07, .14]) became not significant after the inclusion of the Eyes Test scores and TAS-
20 scores in the model (t=1.61, p=.11, n.s.; B=.03, SE=.02, CI[-.01, .07]). This final
model was significant (R2=.24; F(6,785)=38.49, p<.01; t=8.48, p<.01; B=37.52, SE=4.42,
CI[28.84, 46.21]), and bootstrap analysis showed that the scores on childhood emotional
neglect had indirect effects on DES-II scores (B=.07, SE=.01, CI[.05, .09]), through
Eyes Test scores (B=.02, SE=.01, CI[.01, .03]), TAS-20 scores (B=.04, SE=.01,
CI[.03, .06]), and even the relationship between Eyes Test scores and TAS-20 scores
(B= .005, SE=.001, CI[.002, .009]). A summary of the multiple mediation analysis
---
---
In contrast, another multiple mediation analysis did not support the reverse
variable, and the Eyes Test and TAS-20 as the potential mediators. In fact, DES-II
scores did not add to the prediction of childhood emotional neglect in the final model
(t=1.58, p=.11, n.s.; B =.10, SE =.06, CI[-.02, .22]) and had no indirect effects on the
dependent variable through the Eyes Test (B=.02, SE=.02, CI[-.02, .05]), which also
was not significant in the model (B=-.18, SE=.18, CI[-.52, .17]). Therefore, the results
of multiple mediation analyses fully supported the trauma model of dissociation (based
Discussion
Bromberg, 2011; Chefetz, 2015; Ensink et al., 2014, 2015; Fonagy et al., 2002; Howell,
2005; Jurist, Slade, & Bergner, 2008; Schimmenti & Caretti, 2016; Schore, 2009).
Consistent with research showing that dissociation is linked with parental failures of
care, and not only with child abuse (Draijer & Langeland, 1999; Dutra et al., 2009;
Ogawa et al., 1997), this theoretical model predicted that emotional neglect during
feelings, describing feelings, and using them as a guide for one’s own behaviors. This
model also predicted that reduced levels of theory of mind and higher levels of
the dissociative processes that serve to protect the self from psychically painful
experiences. The rationale of this model is based on the assumption that individuals
who experienced emotional neglect during their childhood are likely to perceive
DEVELOPMENTAL ROOTS OF DISSOCIATION 21
mental states, which is strictly related to attachment experiences (Fonagy et al., 2002;
Liotti, 1999), might be less developed, and reduced mentalizing might have further
result, this condition may have forced them to rely on dissociative processes as a
The findings of the current study on a relatively large sample of Italian adults
positively associated with childhood emotional neglect and alexithymia, while it was
showed that theory of mind scores and alexithymia scores fully mediated the positive
association between the participants’ scores on childhood emotional neglect and their
dissociation scores. These results support the view that adequate parenting fosters the
(Schimmenti & Caretti, 2016), that mentalization further promotes affect regulation
(Ensink et al., 2014a, b), and that mentalization and affect regulation protect from
which also involves lack of mentalization and difficulties with emotion regulation. The
suggest that age and education may additionally inform researchers and clinicians on the
Younger and less educated participants were more exposed to the direct and indirect
DEVELOPMENTAL ROOTS OF DISSOCIATION 22
research on dissociation and with the consideration that the capacity for integration and
the sense of self-continuity can improve, even at the neurobiological level, as a result of
age and educational growth (Armstrong, Putnam, Carlson, Libero, & Smith, 1997;
Putnam, 1997; Schimmenti, 2015a, c), among other factors. The sociocultural
environment in which the study was conducted might instead explain the higher scores
on parental overprotection among females. In fact, research has shown that female
2001). However, gender did not add to the multiple mediation model. Finally, the
that may exacerbate a negative perception of childhood experiences, did not find
As with every research, this study comes with a number of limitations. First, the
sample included only adult volunteers from the normal population, so the results of this
study are not immediately generalizable to other people. Future studies should utilize
clinical samples of individuals with current clinical disorders to extend these findings to
measures used in the present study are applied worldwide and have demonstrated
emotional neglect was developed in this research, which helped for an effective testing
DEVELOPMENTAL ROOTS OF DISSOCIATION 23
of the theoretical model presented in the article, but prevented further analyses on the
individual effects of maternal and paternal care on dissociation scores. Future research
on the potentially specific effects of maternal and paternal lack of cares on dissociative
symptoms is warranted. Fourth, although an attempt was made to rule out a reverse
definitively establish causal links. Thus, longitudinal studies with clinical and
nonclinical samples are greatly needed to further test the proposed multiple mediation
model on the developmental roots of dissociation. However, this initial study provided
some empirical evidence that reduced mentalizing abilities and increased levels of
alexithymia are implied in the relationship between emotional neglect during childhood
and heightened levels of dissociation. This may have important clinical implications: in
fact, since dissociation has been empirically linked with many psychiatric disorders, it
might be crucial in clinical practice to assess the presence and severity of dissociative
experiences and to explore whether child abuse and neglect, alexithymia, and lack of
processes and whether these factors continue to exert a strong influence in the
Conclusions
The findings of this study supported the hypothesis that reduced levels of theory
of mind and increased levels of alexithymia may result from parental failures of care
during childhood and may lead to dissociative symptoms. Therefore, it is likely that
people who display excessively activated dissociative processes will greatly benefit
Allen (2013) has recently suggested, when mentalizing is supported in the therapeutic
DEVELOPMENTAL ROOTS OF DISSOCIATION 24
dysregulated feelings and painful mental states on the cognitive and affective levels,
which may lead to clinically significant improvements. For this reason, the
enhancement of mentalized affectivity (Fonagy et al., 2002; Jurist, 2005, 2010) could be
a necessary, albeit difficult, goal to achieve in the treatment of individuals who were
emotionally neglected during their childhood and who show dissociative symptoms.
These patients may need to experientially discover the subjective meaning of their
affective experiences beyond intellectual understanding—in other words, they may need
to discover how to feel and think about their feeling at the same time—before being
ready to process the traumatic memories related to their negative childhood experiences.
This may be a necessary step in clinical work to prevent the overwhelming feelings
coming out abruptly with a flood of dysregulation (Bromberg, 1998, 2011; Schimmenti,
2012).
On another level, the findings of this study may be of some relevance in a time in
which psychoanalysis has finally rediscovered the detrimental effects of child sexual
abuse on individuals’ mental and behavioral functioning (Davies & Frawley, 1992;
Shengold, 1989). In fact, these findings may remind us of the need to address in therapy
any childhood experience that threatened and eventually damaged the development of
self and its integration, not just childhood sexual abuse. They may also remind us that
parental neglect, rejection, and lack of responsiveness might have similar negative
because such experiences negatively affect the quality of object relations and the
internal working models of attachment (Draijer & Langeland, 1999; Schimmenti &
DEVELOPMENTAL ROOTS OF DISSOCIATION 25
Bifulco, 2015). At a more basic level, they can also remind us of Ferenczi’s (1929) old
lesson that emotional neglect—in combination with abuse or not—can deprive children
of the emotional nourishment they need to grow up healthy and in accordance with their
own inherent tendencies and attitudes. The psychoanalytic tradition suggests that
emotional neglect can constitute a trauma for a child (Ferenczi, 1929; Khan, 1963; Kris,
1956), so it might be crucial for clinicians to understand whether this type of trauma
References
Armstrong, J. G., Putnam, F. W., Carlson, E. B., Libero, D. Z., & Smith, S. R. (1997).
Bagby, R. M., Parker, J. D. A., & Taylor, G. J. (1994). The twenty-item Toronto
3999(94)90005-1
Bagby, R. M., Taylor G. J., & Parker, J. D. A. (1994). The twenty-item Toronto
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The “Reading
the Mind in the Eyes” Test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults
10.1017/S0021963001006643
DEVELOPMENTAL ROOTS OF DISSOCIATION 27
332-360. doi:10.1080/13546800500368607
Bifulco, A., & Moran, P. (1998). Wednesday’s child: Research into women’s experience
Bowman, S. E., Blix, S., & Coons, P. M. (1985). Multiple personality in adolescence:
Braehler, C., Valiquette, L., Holowka, D., Malla, A. K., Joober, R., Ciampi, A., …,
doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.05.033
Bressi, C., Taylor, G.J., Parker, J.D.A., Bressi, S., Brambilla, V., Aguglia, E., Allegranti,
I., ... Invernizzi, G. (1996). Cross validation of the factor structure of the 20-item
Bromberg, P. M. (2011). The shadow of the tsunami and the growth of the relational
doi:10.1300/J229v07n04_02
10.1037/a0027748
Coons, P. M. (1986). Child abuse and multiple personality disorder: Review of the
literature and suggestions for treatment. Child Abuse & Neglect, 10, 455-462. doi:
10.1016/0145-2134(86)90049-9
Dalenberg, C. J., Brand, B. L., Gleaves, D. H., Dorahy, M. J., Loewenstein, R. J.,
Cardeña, E., … Spiegel, D. (2012). Evaluation of the evidence for the trauma and
doi:10.1037/a0027447
Dalenberg, C. J., Brand, B. L., Loewenstein, R. J., Gleaves, D. H., Dorahy, M. J.,
Cardeña, E., … Spiegel, D. (2014). Reality versus fantasy: Reply to Lynn et al.
Davies, J. M., & Frawley, M. G. (1994). Treating the adult survivor of childhood sexual
Draijer, N., & Langeland, W. (1999). Childhood trauma and perceived parental
Dutra, L., Bureau, J., Holmes, B., Lyubchik, A., & Lyons-Ruth, K. (2009). Quality of
10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181a653b7
Ensink, K., Berthelot, N., Bernazzani, O., Normandin, L., & Fonagy, P. (2014). Another
step closer to measuring the ghosts in the nursery: Preliminary validation of the
10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01471
Ensink, K., Fonagy, P., Berthelot, N., Normandin, L., & Bernazzani, O. (2015).
step closer to measuring the ghosts in the nursery: preliminary validation of the
10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00697
Ferenczi, S. (1929). The unwelcome child and his death instinct. The International
Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., Jurist, E. L., & Target, M. (2002). Affect regulation,
mentalization, and the development of the self. New York: Other Press.
doi:10.1016/S0145-2134(02)00342-3
DEVELOPMENTAL ROOTS OF DISSOCIATION 30
Güleç, M.Y., Altintaş, M., İnanç, L., Bezgin, C.H., Koca, E. K., & Güleç, H. (2014).
doi:10.1016/j.jad.2012.06.033
Howell, E.F. (2005). The dissociative mind. Hillsdale, NJ: The Analytic Press.
Infurna, M. R., Reichl, C., Parzer, P., Schimmenti, A., Bifulco, A., & Kaess, M. (2016).
10.1016/j.jad.2015.09.006
doi:10.1037/0736-9735.22.3.426
doi:10.1080/07351690903206496
Jurist, E. L., Slade, A., & Bergner, S. (2008), (eds.). Mind to mind: Infant research,
Kluft, R. P. (1987). The parental fitness of mothers with multiple personality disorder:
2134(87)90067-6
10.1080/07351692009348887
Kluft, R. P. (2013). Shelter from the storm: Processing the traumatic memories of
Krystal, H. (1988). Integration and self healing: Affect, trauma, alexithymia. Hillsdale,
Krystal, H., 1997. Desomatization and the consequences of infantile psychic trauma.
Lanius, R., Vermetten, E., Loewenstein, R. J., Brand, B., Schmahl, C., Bremner, J. D, &
10.1080/07351699909534275
old boy and his mother. Jourrnal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent
Minty, B. (2005). The nature of emotional child neglect and abuse. In J. Taylor & B.
Daniel (Eds.), Child neglect: Practice issues for health and social care (pp. 57–
doi:10.1080/15299732.2011.608781
Nazarov, A., Frewen, P., Parlar, M., Oremus, C., MacQueen, G., McKinnon, M., &
193-201. doi:10.1111/acps.12142
Nemiah, J. C., Freyberger, H., & Sifneos, P. E. (1976). Alexithymia: A view of the
Ogawa, J. R., Sroufe, L. A., Weinfield, N. S., Carlson, E. A., & Egeland, B. (1997).
O’Neil (Eds.), Dissociation and the dissociative disorders – DSM-V and beyond
Parker, G., Tupling, H., Brown, L. B. (1979) A parental bonding instrument. British
8341.1979.tb02487.x
Parker, J. D. A., Keefer, K. V., Taylor, G. J., & Bagby, R. M. (2008). Latent structure of
Pears, K. C., & Fisher, P. A. (2005). Emotion understanding and theory of mind among
doi:10.1016/S0167-8760(01)00162-3
Ross, C. A., Norton, G. R., & Wozney, K. (1989). Multiple personality disorder: An
Sar, V., Akyüz, G., Kundakçi, T,, Kiziltan, E., & Dogan, O. (2004). Childhood trauma,
“Another step closer to measuring the ghosts in the nursery: preliminary validation
10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00380
Schimmenti, A., & Bifulco, A. (2015). Linking lack of care in childhood to anxiety
Schimmenti, A., & Caretti, V. (2010). Psychic retreats or psychic pits? Unbearable
132. doi:10.1037/a0019414
Schimmenti, A., & Caretti V. (2016). Linking the overwhelming with the unbearable:
Schimmenti, A., Maganuco, N. R., Gelsomino, E., La Marca, L., Di Dio, N. & Gervasi,
Schimmenti, A., Passanisi, A., Caretti, V., La Marca, L., Granieri, A., Iacolino, C., ...
Schore, A. N. (2009). Attachment trauma and the developing right brain: Origins of
dissociative disorders: DSM-V and beyond (pp. 141–153). New York, NY:
Routledge.
Scinto, A., Marinangeli, M. G., Kalyvoka, A., Daneluzzo, E., & Rossi, A. (1999).
esplorativa e confermatoria [The use of the Italian version of the Parental Bonding
283.
Serafin, M., & Surian, L. (2004). Il test degli Occhi: uno strumento per valutare la
‘teoria della mente’ [Eyes Test: A measure to assess 'theory of mind']. Giornale
Shengold, L. (1989). Soul murder: The effects of childhood abuse and deprivation. New
Taylor, G. J., & Bagby, R. M. (2013). Psychoanalysis and empirical research: The
99–133. doi:10.1177/0003065112474066
Taylor, G. J., Bagby, R. M., & Parker, J. D. A. (1997). Disorders of affect regulation:
University Press.
van der Kolk, B. A., McFarlane, A. C., & Weisaeth L. (1996), (eds.). Traumatic stress:
The effects of overwhelming experience on mind, body, and society. New York,
van der Kolk, B.A. (2014). The body keeps the score: Brain, mind, and body in the
10.1016/0272-7358(96)00006-2
Vellante, M., Baron-Cohen, S., Melis, M., Marrone, M,, Petretto, D. R., Masala, C., &
Preti, A. (2013). The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test: Systematic review of
Wagner, A. W., & Linehan, M. (1999). Facial expression recognition ability among
Zanarini, M.C., Frankenburg, F.R., Jager-Hyman, S., Reich, D.B., & Fitzmaurice, G.
(2008). The course of dissociation for patients with borderline personality disorder
Childhood Emotional Neglect - 20.56 (22.91) -72 - 54 - 24.13 (21.42) - 17.61 (23.69) 4.02**
2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
1. DES-II -.32** .34** .16** -.16** .13** -.11** .10** -.24** -.16**
2. Eyes Test - -.30** -.15** .12** -.12** .04 -.17** -.03 .21**
9. Age - -.03
Note: The values in bracket squares indicate the predictive association between childhood emotional neglect and dissociation without the