Lev Vygotsky's Sociocultural Theory of Cognitive Development
Lev Vygotsky's Sociocultural Theory of Cognitive Development
Lev Vygotsky's Sociocultural Theory of Cognitive Development
Cognitive Development
By Saul Mcleod, PhD | Updated August 18, 2022,
Article Content
Unlike Piaget's notion that childrens' development must necessarily precede their
learning, Vygotsky argued, "learning is a necessary and universal aspect of the
process of developing culturally organized, specifically human psychological
function" (1978, p. 90). In other words, social learning tends to precede (i.e.,
come before) development.
Vygotsky's Ideas
Vygotsky's theory focuses on the role of culture in the development of mental
abilities e.g. speech and reasoning in children.
According to Vygotsky, adults in society foster children’s cognitive development
by engaging them in challenging and meaningful activities. Adults convey to
children the way their culture interprets and responds to the world.
They show the meaning they attach to objects, events and experiences. They
provide the child with what to think (the knowledge) and how to think (the
processes, the tools to think with).
The interactions with others significantly increases not only the quantity of
information and the number of skills a child develops, it also affects the
development of higher order mental functions such as formal reasoning.
Vygotsky
argued that higher mental abilities could only develop through the interaction
with more advanced others.
Vygotsky proposed that children are born with elementary mental abilities such
as memory and perception and that higher mental functions develop from these
through the influence of social interactions.
Vygotsky agreed with Piaget that the development of cognitive abilities takes
place in stages and he also agreed broadly with the description of the stages
however he viewed cognitive development as a social process where children
learn from experienced adults.
Vygotsky stated that language has two functions. Inner speech is used for
mental reasoning and external speech is used to converse with others. These
operations occur separately. Indeed, before the age of two, a child employs
words socially; they possess no internal language. Once thought and language
merge, however, the social language is internalized and assists the child with
their reasoning. Thus, the social environment is ingrained within the child’s
learning.
(i) Vygotsky states the importance of cultural and social context for learning.
Cognitive development stems from social interactions from guided learning within
the zone of proximal development as children and their partner's co-construct
knowledge. In contrast, Piaget maintains that cognitive development stems
largely from independent explorations in which children construct knowledge of
their own.
(ii) For Vygotsky, the environment in which children grow up will influence how
they think and what they think about.
o Attention
o Sensation
o Perception
o Memory
Each culture provides its children tools of intellectual adaptation that allow
them to use the basic mental functions more effectively/adaptively.
Vygotsky, therefore, sees cognitive functions, even those carried out alone, as
affected by the beliefs, values, and tools of intellectual adaptation of the
culture in which a person develops and therefore socio-culturally determined.
The tools of intellectual adaptation, therefore, vary from culture to culture - as
in the memory example.
Shaffer (1996) gives the example of a young girl who is given her first jigsaw.
Alone, she performs poorly in attempting to solve the puzzle. The father then
sits with her and describes or demonstrates some basic strategies, such as
finding all the corner/edge pieces and provides a couple of pieces for the child
to put together herself and offers encouragement when she does so.
As the child becomes more competent, the father allows the child to work more
independently. According to Vygotsky, this type of social interaction involving
cooperative or collaborative dialogue promotes cognitive development.
Although the implication is that the MKO is a teacher or an older adult, this is
not necessarily the case. Many times, a child's peers or an adult's children may
be the individuals with more knowledge or experience.
For example, who is more likely to know more about the newest teenage music
groups, how to win at the most recent PlayStation game, or how to correctly
perform the newest dance craze - a child or their parents?
In fact, the MKO need not be a person at all. Some companies, to support
employees in their learning process, are now using electronic performance
support systems.
Electronic tutors have also been used in educational settings to facilitate and
guide students through the learning process. The key to MKOs is that they
must have (or be programmed with) more knowledge about the topic being
learned than the learner does.
For example, the child could not solve the jigsaw puzzle (in the example above)
by itself and would have taken a long time to do so (if at all), but was able to
solve it following interaction with the father, and has developed competence at
this skill that will be applied to future jigsaws.
ZPD is the zone where instruction is the most beneficial as it is when the task is
just beyond the individual’s capabilities. To learn we must be presented with
tasks that are just out of our ability range. Challenging tasks promote the
maximum cognitive growth.
Vygotsky (1978) sees the Zone of Proximal Development as the area where the
most sensitive instruction or guidance should be given - allowing the child to
develop skills they will then use on their own - developing higher mental
functions.
Some children were allowed to play with their mother in a similar situation before
they attempted it alone (zone of proximal development) while others were allowed
to work on this by themselves (Piaget's discovery learning).
Freund found that those who had previously worked with their mother (ZPD)
showed the greatest improvement compared with their first attempt at the
task. The conclusion being that guided learning within the ZPD led to greater
understanding/performance than working alone (discovery learning).
For Vygotsky, thought and language are initially separate systems from the
beginning of life, merging at around three years of age. At this point speech
and thought become interdependent: thought becomes verbal, speech becomes
representational. When this happens, children's monologues internalized to
become inner speech. The internalization of language is important as it drives
cognitive development.
Thus private speech, in Vygotsky's view, was the earliest manifestation of inner
speech. Indeed, private speech is more similar (in its form and function) to inner
speech than social speech.
Vygotsky sees "private speech" as a means for children to plan activities and
strategies and therefore aid their development. Private speech is the use of
language for self-regulation of behavior. Language is, therefore, an accelerator
to thinking/understanding (Jerome Bruner also views language in this way).
Vygotsky believed that children who engaged in large amounts of private speech
are more socially competent than children who do not use it extensively.
Vygotsky (1987) notes that private speech does not merely accompany a child’s
activity but acts as a tool used by the developing child to facilitate cognitive
processes, such as overcoming task obstacles, enhancing imagination, thinking,
and conscious awareness.
Children use private speech most often during intermediate difficulty tasks
because they are attempting to self-regulate by verbally planning and
organizing their thoughts (Winsler et al., 2007).
The frequency and content of private speech are then correlated with behavior
or performance. For example, private speech appears to be functionally related
to cognitive performance: It appears at times of difficulty with a task.
For example, tasks related to executive function (Fernyhough & Fradley, 2005),
problem-solving tasks (Behrend et al., 1992), schoolwork in both language (Berk
& Landau, 1993), and mathematics (Ostad & Sorensen, 2007).
Berk (1986) provided empirical support for the notion of private speech. She
found that most private speech exhibited by children serves to describe or
guide the child's actions.
Berk also discovered than child engaged in private speech more often when
working alone on challenging tasks and also when their teacher was not
immediately available to help them. Furthermore, Berk also found that private
speech develops similarly in all children regardless of cultural background.
Childrens’ use of private speech diminishes as they grow older and follows a
curvilinear trend. This is due to changes in ontogenetic development whereby
children are able to internalize language (through inner speech) in order to
self-regulate their behavior (Vygotsky, 1987).
For example, research has shown that childrens’ private speech usually peaks at
3–4 years of age, decreases at 6–7 years of age, and gradually fades out to
be mostly internalized by age 10 (Diaz, 1992).
Vygotsky proposed that private speech diminishes and disappears with age not
because it becomes socialized, as Piaget suggested, but rather because it goes
underground to constitute inner speech or verbal thought” (Frauenglass & Diaz,
1985).
Vygotsky
believed everything is learned on two levels.
First, through interaction
with others, and then integrated into the individual’s mental structure.
Traditionally, schools have not promoted environments in which the students play
an active role in their own education as well as their peers'. Vygotsky's theory,
however, requires the teacher and students to play untraditional roles as they
collaborate with each other.
Instead of a teacher dictating her meaning to students for future recitation, a
teacher should collaborate with her students in order to create meaning in ways
that students can make their own (Hausfather, 1996).
ZPD
Because Vygotsky asserts that cognitive change occurs within the zone of
proximal development, instruction would be designed to reach a developmental
level that is just above the student's current developmental level.
Vygotsky's theories also feed into the current interest in collaborative learning,
suggesting that group members should have different levels of ability so more
advanced peers can help less advanced members operate within their ZPD.
Reciprocal Teaching
A contemporary educational application of Vygotsky's theory is "reciprocal
teaching," used to improve students' ability to learn from text. In this method,
teachers and students collaborate in learning and practicing four key skills:
summarizing, questioning, clarifying, and predicting. The teacher's role in the
process is reduced over time.
Reciprocal teaching allows for the creation of a dialogue between students and
teachers. This two way communication becomes an instructional strategy by
encouraging students to go beyond answering questions and engage in the
discourse (Driscoll, 1994; Hausfather, 1996).
Scaffolding
Also, Vygotsky theory of cognitive development on learners is relevant to
instructional concepts such as "scaffolding" and "apprenticeship," in which a
teacher or more advanced peer helps to structure or arrange a task so that a
novice can work on it successfully.
A teacher's
role is to identify each
individual’s current level of development and
provide them with
opportunities to cross their ZPD.
Scaffolding describes the conditions that support the child’s learning, to move
from what they already know to new knowledge and abilities.
During scaffolding the support offered by a adult (or more knowledgeable other)
gradually decreases as the child becomes more skilled in the task. As the adult
withdraws their help, the child assumes more of the strategic planning and
eventually gains competence to master similar problems without the aid of a
teacher or more knowledgeable peer.
As the child progresses through the ZPD, the level of scaffolding necessary
declines from 5 to 1.
The teacher must engage students' interest, simplify tasks so they are
manageable, and motivate students to pursue the instructional goal. In addition,
the teacher must look for discrepancies between students' efforts and the
solution, control for frustration and risk, and model an idealized version of the
act (Hausfather, 1996).
The structure of our schools do not reflect the rapid changes our society is
experiencing. The introduction and integration of computer technology in society
has tremendously increased the opportunities for social interaction.
Computer technology is a cultural tool that students can use to mediate and
internalize their learning. Recent research suggests changing the learning
contexts with technology is a powerful learning activity (Crawford, 1996). If
schools continue to resist structural change, students will be ill prepared for
the world they will live.
Critical Evaluation
Vygotsky's work has not received the same level of intense scrutiny that Piaget's
has, partly due to the time-consuming process of translating Vygotsky's work
from Russian. Also, Vygotsky's sociocultural perspective does not provide as many
specific hypotheses to test as did Piaget's theory, making refutation difficult, if
not impossible.
Perhaps the main criticism of Vygotsky's work concerns the assumption that it is
relevant to all cultures. Rogoff (1990) dismisses the idea that Vygotsky's ideas
are culturally universal and instead states the concept of scaffolding - which is
heavily dependent on verbal instruction - may not be equally useful in all
cultures for all types of learning. Indeed, in some instances, observation and
practice may be more effective ways of learning certain skills.
There is much emphasis on social interaction and culture but many other aspects
for development are neglected such as the importance of emotional factors e.g.
the joys of success and the disappointments and frustration of failure, these
acts as motivation for learning.
Fact Checking
References
Behrend, D.A., Rosengren, K.S., & Perlmutter, M. (1992). The relation between
private speech and parental interactive style. In R.M. Diaz & L.E. Berk (Eds.),
Private speech: From social interaction to self-regulation (pp. 85–100). Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Berk, L. E., & Landau, S. (1993). Private speech of learning-disabled and normally
achieving
children in classroom academic and laboratory contexts. Child
Development, 64, 556–571.
Diaz, R. M., & Berk, L. E. (1992). Private speech: From social interaction to self-
regulation.
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Piaget, J. (1959). The language and thought of the child (Vol. 5). Psychology
Press.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). Thinking and speech. In R.W. Rieber & A.S. Carton
(Eds.),
The collected works of L.S. Vygotsky, Volume 1: Problems of general
psychology
(pp. 39–285). New York: Plenum Press. (Original work published
1934.)
Winsler, A., Abar, B., Feder, M. A., Schunn, C. D., & Rubio, D. A. (2007). Private
speech and executive functioning among high-functioning children with autistic
spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1617-
1635.
Further Information
Constructivism
Jerome Bruner