Holden Origin of Speech 2004
Holden Origin of Speech 2004
Holden Origin of Speech 2004
SPECIAL SECTION
are breaking that miracle down into a se- of room for interpretation—and conflict. “If (Science, 20 November 1998, p. 1455). But
ries of smaller, more manageable “mira- you want a consensus, you won’t get it,” all we know for certain, says Pinker, is that
cles,” involving disparate capacities such says cognitive scientist Philip Lieberman of fully developed language was in place by at
as the ability to imitate facial expressions Brown University. With no fossils of speech, least 50,000 years ago, when humans in Eu-
or to string movements together. They’re the origin of language remains “a mystery rope were creating art and burying their
not fantasizing that the human brain at dead, symbolic behaviors that point un-
some point suddenly found that it could * www.ling.ed.ac.uk/evolang equivocally to fluent language.
SPECIAL SECTION
The motor route
Understanding when language emerged
will probably have to await better under-
standing of how it emerged. In recent
years many researchers have become in-
creasingly attracted to the notion that
changes in the brain’s motor areas were
crucial for language capability.
Although we tend to associate language
first with sound rather than movement,
speech may be better understood as a motor
activity, says Deacon. Like other fine motor
activities such as threading a needle or play-
ing the violin, speech demands extraordinar-
ily fine and rapid motor control. Elaborate From ape to human. Magnetic resonance images of a bonobo brain are warped onto the shape of a
movements of the larynx, mouth, face, human cortex, viewed from (left to right) the side, top, and front. Red and yellow areas in the tem-
tongue, and breath must be synchronized poral region (linked to language) and in the prefrontal and occipital regions had to be stretched the
with cognitive activity. most to reach the human configuration, whereas blue areas are similar in apes and humans.
Thus researchers are probing the links be-
tween language and areas in the brain that ease, which disrupts the basal ganglia, suffer touch below the neck due to a strange virus.
offer glimpses into what might have been the They have shown that Parkinson’s patients his own and listeners’ view. “The hands are
“intermediate behavioral manifestations” be- with basal ganglia damage have more trouble really precisely linked to speech articula-
tween animal communication and speech. with regular verbs than with irregular ones. tion,” says McNeill. “Gesture is not a behav-
Many researchers think hand and face Conjugating a regular verb such as “walk,” ioral fossil that was superseded by language
gestures offer behavior that is more analo- Pinker explains, is a combinatorial, sequen- but an indispensable part of language.”
gous to speech than are animal vocaliza- tial task that calls for adding the “ed” for past But not everyone is ready to dismiss the
tions. In all other mammals, both breathing tense. But retrieving the past tense for an ir- meaningfulness of animal calls, with differ-
and articulation are directed by brain areas regular verb such as “come” simply calls on ing views often dependent on a scientist’s
quite separate from those associated with long-term memory. Such tasks require other specialty. Primatologist Marc Hauser of Har-
human speech, notes Pinker. Lieberman ar- brain areas as well, but Lieberman argues vard, for example, believes that primate calls
gues that nonhuman primates engage in “a that the basal ganglia are a common element are better candidates for speech precursors
limited number of stereotyped calls” such as in both movement and language disorders. than any gestures are. With chimp gestures,
alarm calls and that they don’t have the in- Indeed, although many other brain areas, “nothing gives a suggestion of anything
teractive or combinatorial quality of lan- including those responsible for articulation, referential”—that is, having an explicit asso-
guage. Apes’ anatomy is such that they hearing, planning, and memory, had to de- ciation with a concept or thing—he says.
“could produce a [phonetically] reduced velop to support language, there is abundant Primate alarm calls, in his view, “kind of
form of human speech,” adds Lieberman. behavioral evidence for an intimate connec- look like words.” For example, he cites work
“But they don’t.” They’re much better at tion between language and motor abilities, by psychologist Klaus Zuberbühler of the
signing, because apes’ motor behaviors have says Pinker. For example, psychologist University of St. Andrews, U.K., who has re-
more flexibility and are more involved David McNeill of the University of Chicago ported that African Diana monkeys can
in social interaction—through gaze, mouth cites the case of a man who lost all sense of modulate their alarm calls to indicate what
and facial movements, and type of animal (leopard or eagle) is threaten-
limb gestures—than their ing. Such sounds, says Hauser, “have a far
calls, Lieberman says. greater … connection to language than any
Lieberman argues that discovery on nonvocal signals.”
the crucial changes that Many linguists, too, are unmoved by
laid the groundwork for motor arguments, which they do not be-
language ability occurred lieve can explain how the brain developed
in brain circuits connected syntax. “Motor organs are for muscular
with the basal ganglia, sub- movements,” says Derek Bickerton of the
cortical structures involved University of Hawaii, Manoa, and that
in movement. In his view puts them at the “end of the pipeline” of
the basal ganglia is the “se- language production. “Whatever organiz-
quencing engine” that es motor movements is on a par with
makes combinations— what organizes throwing movements,”
both verbal and gestural— says Bickerton. “The purpose is to put
possible. As evidence he things in a regular invariant sequence.”
points to the fact that pa- Hand and mouth. Chimps gesture with both face and hands to That, he says, is very different from mak-
tients with Parkinson’s dis- help express themselves. ing sentences, which requires “putting
things into an extremely plastic order de- tecture for imitation”—in people. He com- with an intuitive sense of how their body
termined by your conceptual structure.” bined the results of single-cell brain record- parts correspond with those of others. Thus a
ings in monkeys with functional magnetic small child knows how to raise its hand in re-
Mirror, mirror resonance imaging in humans while they sponse to a parental wave. “There’s obviously
Despite such caveats, the motor-language watched or imitated finger movements or fa- a direct representation of your body in its
connection continues to draw attention, in cial expressions. Iacoboni says that in addi- body,” says Studdert-Kennedy.
part because of a 1996 discovery that many tion to Broca’s, the circuit comprises an area The theory developed new life when
see as the first hard data in years to bolster in the superior temporal cortex (which over- Studdert-Kennedy brought it to bear on
the theory. This is the so-called mirror neu- laps with Wernicke’s and has neurons that re- questions of language evolution. Mirror
ron system found in monkeys’ brains. spond to face and body movements) and one neurons, he says, “for the first time provide
Mirror neurons’ link to language depends in the parietal cortex, the homolog to the an example of a direct physiological
on imitation, a skill largely unique to humans macaque area called PF, which combines vi- hookup between input and output”: the ob-
and considered vital to language. Although sual and bodily information. “The neural ar- servation of an action and its imitation. In-
parrots and dolphins can do vocal mimicry, chitecture for imitation … overlaps very well deed, Rizzolatti’s group recently reported
imitation is not as a rule a that the macaque has “audiovisual” mirror
mammalian attribute: Even neurons: Some of the cells in F5 fire not
nonhuman primates do it only when a macaque watches a meaning-
poorly (contrary to the im- ful grasping action, but when it hears the
plication of the term “to sound of one, such as the sound of breaking
ape”). But imitation is the peanuts (Science, 2 August 2002, p. 846).
“opened a whole new approach to the lan- have shown that they (unlike another imitator, speech, people quickly develop sign lan-
guage evolution story,” says Arbib of USC. the parrot) are guided by the “gestural” fea- guage, as has been shown by the case of a
“What would a mirror system for grasping be tures of the sound—that is, by the actions of community of deaf Nicaraguans who created
doing in the speech area of the brain?” The the mouth rather than by a sound’s acoustic their own language.
researchers concluded that these mirror cells features. A well-known trick to demonstrate Given the strong role of manual and facial
form a system for matching the observation this is known as the McGurk effect: If you gesture in speech and the relatively recent fi-
and execution of mouth and hand actions— watch someone pronounce the syllable “ga” nal mutation of the FOXP2 gene, Corballis
the first steps toward imitation. while listening to a recording of someone argues that “autonomous” speech may not
So far, mirror neurons have been found in saying “ba,” you will likely hear “da,” a have become fully developed until the cultur-
only two brain areas in macaques, and the sound anatomically between the other two. al explosion beginning 50,000 years ago. The
single-cell brain recording technique that re- This means “you perceive speech by re- mirror system, he believes, reinforces his the-
vealed the macaque neurons isn’t done on ferring the sounds you hear to your own pro- ory, because it apparently evolved first for
humans. But Iacoboni believes he has identi- duction mechanism,” says Studdert-Kennedy. manual control. It “probably picked up vocal
fied a similar circuit—“a core neural archi- Humans, unlike other animals, are equipped and facial control quite late in hominid evolu-
SPECIAL SECTION
tion,” he says, as speech became the pre- them will be through ever-finer brain imag- mals, will help yield a better “taxonomy” of
ferred modality for communication for vari- ing technology so they can, as Bickerton the language conundrum, especially if bol-
ous reasons, such as the need to free the puts it, “find out the flow chart for a sen- stered by computational modeling. But an-
hands for work or to talk in the dark. tence in the brain.” Harvard’s Hauser and swers won’t come all at once. “I see this as
But others believe equally strongly that colleagues believe that research in animals a process of gradual convergence. The
even if movement and language are insepara- may identify behavioral analogs for “recur- problem space is shrinking” at long last,
ble, language is primarily an oral, not manual, sion”: the ability to string words together in says Bickerton. “It will be solved when that
behavior. Psychologist Peter MacNeilage of infinite hierarchical combinations. Arbib space goes to zero, not when someone
the University of Texas, Austin, has devel- predicts that the discovery of other types of comes up with the killer solution.”
oped a theory that monkey oral behaviors mirror systems, in both humans and ani- –CONSTANCE HOLDEN
(not vocalizations) are precursors of human
syllables, and he argues that the mirror neu-
ron system—especially the recent discovery NEWS
of neurons that respond to lip smacking and
nut cracking—bolsters his ideas.
MacNeilage suggests that the brain’s sup-
plementary motor area (an area adjacent to
The First Language?
the primary motor cortex that is important for Genetic and linguistic data indicate—but can’t quite prove—that our ancient
motor memory and sequential movements) ancestors spoke with strange clicking noises
controls the physical constraints on vocal ex-