2002, Vol.86, Issues 3, Dementia
2002, Vol.86, Issues 3, Dementia
2002, Vol.86, Issues 3, Dementia
Preface
Dementia
James D. Bowen, MD
Guest Editor
Dementia is one of the most important yet difficult conditions that phy-
sicians are asked to manage. It takes an enormous toll on patients, even-
tually robbing them of the accumulated wealth of their life experiences,
destroying the very essence of their beings. Families likewise suffer from
the loss of patriarchs and matriarchs while communities lose the guidance
and wisdom of seasoned leaders. With 6% to 10% of those over 65 years of
age affected, dementia takes a staggering economic toll that will only worsen
as the population ages. Though dementia is common, and despite the sever-
ity of its impact, physicians and families often overlook the diagnosis.
Recognizing dementia is not the only challenge because identifying the cor-
rect diagnosis may be difficult. Finally, the management of these patients
can be daunting even for skilled geriatricians.
This issue of Medical Clinics of North America is devoted to dementia. It
is divided into three sections discussing clinical aspects, recent scientific
insights, and treatments. The first article provides an overview of the diag-
nosis of patients with dementia. The clinical history, examination, labora-
tory evaluation, and differential diagnosis are reviewed. The next five
articles review specific dementing diseases that are of particular importance
today. Vascular dementia is reviewed in the second article. This represents
the second most common cause of dementia, after Alzheimer’s disease.
The third and fourth articles review frontotemporal dementia and dementia
with Lewy bodies. These two conditions have recently been distinguished
from other types of dementia. New criteria have been proposed, allowing
0025-7125/02/$ - see front matter Ó 2002, Elsevier Science (USA). All rights reserved.
PII: S 0 0 2 5 - 7 1 2 5 ( 0 2 ) 0 0 0 1 9 - 6
xii Preface / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) xi–xiii
that have added so much excitement to this field, and that provide such hope
for the future.
James D. Bowen, MD
Guest Editor
Department of Neurology
University of Washington
Box 356465, Room RR650
1959 N.E. Pacific Street
Seattle, WA 98195-6465, USA
Dementia is one of the most costly and disabling diseases associated with
aging. The emotional impact of the disease on patients and families is devas-
tating, and the cost to society is staggering. Annual costs for caring for a
single patient with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) are reported to be between
$35,000 and $47,000, totaling more than $140 billion dollars per year in the
United States assuming there are 4 million people with AD [1–3]. This figure
is projected to rise as the proportion of the population older than the age
of 65 years increases. As treatments for AD and other dementias that can
extend the period individuals have reasonably good cognitive and physical
functioning become available, the accurate and early diagnosis of these dis-
orders becomes more crucial.
This article provides an overview of the bedside and clinic evaluation of
patients with complaints of forgetfulness or other cognitive or behavioral
disturbances and reviews distinguishing features of dementia and other con-
ditions that may be confused with dementia. The laboratory and imaging
evaluation of the patient with dementia and the various causes of dementia
are described. Current clinical practice guidelines and practice parameters
are reviewed as relevant for the primary care practitioner. For the most part,
these guidelines are similar; however, there are important inconsistencies
that are discussed.
* Corresponding author.
E-mail address: [email protected] (G.W. Ross).
This work was supported by National Institute on Aging contract NO1-AG-4-2149, US
Department of the Army grant DAMD17-98-1-8621, and Department of Veterans Affairs
medical research funds. The information contained in this article does not necessarily reflect the
position or the policy of the US government, and no official endorsement should be inferred.
0025-7125/02/$ - see front matter Ó 2002, Elsevier Science (USA). All rights reserved.
PII: S 0 0 2 5 - 7 1 2 5 ( 0 2 ) 0 0 0 0 9 - 3
456 G.W. Ross, J.D. Bowen / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 455–476
Table 1
Mini-mental state examination
Maximum score
What is the (year)(season)(date)(day)(month)? 5
Where are we (state)(country)(town)(hospital)(floor)? 5
Registration
Name three objects. Give 1 second to say each. 3
Ask the patient all three after you have said them.
Give 1 point for each correct answer.
Repeat them until the patient learns all three count trials and record.
Attention and calculation
Serial 7’s. Give 1 point for each correct. Stop after five answers. 5
Alternatively, spell ‘‘world’’ backwards.
Recall
Ask for the three objects repeated previously. Give 1 point for each 3
correct answer.
Language
Names a pencil and watch. 2
Repeat the following: ‘‘No ifs, ands, or buts.’’ 1
Follow a three-stage command: ‘‘Take a paper in your right hand, 3
fold it in half, and put it on the floor.’’
Read and obey the following: CLOSE YOUR EYES. 1
Write a sentence. 1
Copy design [two overlapping pentagrams]. 1
460 G.W. Ross, J.D. Bowen / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 455–476
Laboratory tests
Laboratory tests should be performed to identify infectious, metabolic,
toxic, and inflammatory disorders that can cause neuropsychologic impair-
ment. Required tests include a complete blood cell count; tests of electrolyte,
serum glucose, vitamin B12, and folate levels; tests of liver, renal, and thy-
roid function; and a serologic test for syphilis [19,20]. In a recent study, the
use of these tests affected patient management in 13% of consecutive patients
being evaluated for dementia [32].
Further diagnostic testing should be based on clinical suspicion. This
includes serum or urine tests for toxins, drugs, or heavy metals when expo-
sure is suspected; sedimentation rate for suspected infectious or inflam-
matory disorders; and human immunodeficiency virus antibody testing.
Genetic testing may be useful in those with three or more first-degree rela-
tives with a dementing illness. Assuming that mass lesions are absent, a lum-
bar puncture may help to diagnose metastatic cancer, infection, vasculitis,
encephalitis, meningitis, syphilis, or hydrocephalus. Lumbar puncture may
be particularly useful in dementia patients less than 55 years of age or in
those with rapid progression, unusual dementia, or immunosuppression
[30]. Specific tests on cerebrospinal fluid (CSF), although not standard, may
be helpful for diagnosis. For example, elevation of the normal brain protein
14-3-3 in the CSF of patients with progressive dementia without CSF pleo-
cytosis has been reported to be 96% sensitive and 99% specific for Creutz-
feldt-Jakob disease [20,34]. An electroencephalogram may identify the
periodic sharp wave complexes associated with Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease and
may be helpful in distinguishing depression or delirium from dementia [30].
Table 2
Problems presenting as memory loss
Dementia
Worried well
Normal aging
Depression
Delirium
Stroke syndromes
Bradykinesia
Abulia
Seizure
Excessive daytime somnolence
Amnestic syndrome
suggest that the stigma attached to this label could actually work to the
patient’s detriment [40].
Depression
Memory impairment is commonly associated with major depression in the
elderly and may be the presenting symptom of this common and treatable dis-
order [41]. In fact, memory complaints in the elderly may be more related to
depression than to objective memory impairment [42]. The essential features
of major depression are sadness that is persistent or anhedonia (loss of inter-
est in usual activities) [5]. In addition to memory impairment and poor con-
centration, depressed patients experience sleep and appetite disturbances, loss
of energy, psychomotor retardation, feelings of worthlessness or guilt, or
recurrent thoughts of death. Clinical screening tools for depression are avail-
able, such as the Hamilton Scale or the Geriatric Depression Scale [43,44].
Patients with depression generally have impaired recall with relative spar-
ing of recognition memory. The cognitive and mood symptoms can resolve
completely with treatment, although response to treatment may be difficult
to assess when dementia and depression coexist. Additionally, because de-
pression with cognitive impairment may presage the development of demen-
tia, it is important to follow patients so as to assess treatment effectiveness
and track progression of cognitive deficits [45].
Delirium
Delirium or acute confusional state is another condition that is common
in the elderly. According to the DSM-IV [5], delirium requires the following:
1. Disturbance of consciousness (ie, reduced clarity of awareness of the
environment) with reduced ability to focus, sustain, or shift attention.
2. A change in cognition (eg, memory deficit, disorientation, language dis-
turbance) or the development of a perceptual disturbance that is not
better accounted for by a preexisting, established, or evolving dementia.
3. The disturbance develops over a short period (usually hours to days)
and tends to fluctuate during the course of the day.
Illusions and hallucinations may be seen, speech may be incoherent at
times, sleep-wake cycles are often disturbed, and psychomotor activity is
increased or decreased. Delirium is associated with a variety of systemic
illnesses, infections, and toxic and metabolic disturbances. Hospitalized
patients in general and surgical patients in particular are prone to delirium.
Features that help to differentiate delirium from dementia include rapid
onset, short duration, and disturbance of consciousness that often waxes
and wanes between agitation and lethargy. It is important to recognize that
patients with dementia are at increased risk for delirium and that delirium
and dementia may coexist [46].
464 G.W. Ross, J.D. Bowen / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 455–476
Amnestic syndromes
Amnesia is defined as an inability to learn new information and is an early
sign of AD. Unlike AD, however, the amnestic syndromes are characterized
by isolated amnesia with preservation of other areas of neuropsychologic
function, such as language and visuospatial ability. Amnestic syndromes are
generally associated with conditions that affect the mesial temporal lobes
and their connections with the fornix, mamillary bodies, and thalamus.
Causes of amnesia include Korsakov’s syndrome associated with alcohol
abuse and thiamine deficiency, herpes encephalitis, and head trauma [47].
It is important to recognize that patients with isolated memory loss with-
out apparent cause are at high risk for developing dementia and should be
closely monitored [48].
Aphasia
Language disturbance or aphasia complicates the evaluation of the cog-
nitively impaired patient. An aphasic patient may be unable to participate in
memory testing because of an inability to comprehend instructions, repeat
words or phrases, or read. Aphasia is associated with dysfunction in the
dominant hemisphere, usually the left hemisphere. Anterior lesions cause
nonfluent aphasia with sparse verbal output, whereas posterior lesions in
Wernicke’s area are associated with fluent verbal output with word substitu-
tions or paraphasias and impaired comprehension [49]. Dominant hemi-
sphere strokes are the most common cause of aphasia; however, anomia
may be the earliest feature of AD or frontal lobe degenerative dementia [21].
Etiology of dementia
Once it has been determined that a patient’s complaints of memory loss
are the result of dementia, the physician must determine the nature of the
dementing disorder. Table 3 provides a partial list of the many causes of
dementia. The following discussion highlights some of the more common
and reversible causes. Detailed information on some of these conditions may
be found in other articles in this issue. Comprehensive reviews of the causes
of dementia are also available [4,18].
Table 3
Causes of dementia
Cortical degenerative dementias Dementias associated with parkinsonism
Alzheimer’s disease Parkinson’s disease
Frontotemporal dementia Dementia with Lewy bodies
Vascular dementias Progressive supranuclear palsy
Multiple large vessel infarcts Multiple systems atrophy
Single strategic infarct Cortical-basal ganglionic degeneration
Lacunar state Idiopathic basal ganglia calcifications
Binswanger’s disease Parkinsonism-dementia complex of Guam
CADASILa Other extrapyramidal disorders
Toxic/metabolic conditions Wilson’s disease
Medication-induced dementia Huntington’s disease
Alcohol-related dementia Hallervorden-Spatz disease
Dementia related to heavy metal Dementias related to infections
exposure Prion diseases
Vitamin B12 deficiency Creutzfeldt-Jakob Disease
Folate deficiency Gerstmann-Straussler-Scheinker Disease
Hypo- or hyperthyroidism Kuru
Hypo- or hyperparathyroidism New-variant Creutzfeldt-Jakob Disease
Hypo or hypermagnesemia Neurosyphilis
Hypo or hypercalcemia AIDS dementia
Cushing’s disease Chronic meningitis
Addison’s disease Fungal
Renal failure Tuberculosis
Liver failure Lyme disease
Porphyria Viral encephalitis
Domoic acid poisoning Whipple’s disease
Paraneoplastic syndromes Trauma-related dementias
Limbic encephalitis Dementia related to closed-head injury
Autoimmune/inflammatory disorders Chronic subdural hematoma
Multiple sclerosis Dementia pugilistica
Behcet’s disease Miscellaneous disorders
Lupus erythematosus Normal pressure hydrocephalus
Sarcoidosis Hippocampal sclerosis
Temporal arteritis and other central Central nervous system tumors
nervous system vasculitides Mitochondrial encephalopathies
a
Cerebral autosomal dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalo-
pathy.
Alzheimer’s disease
AD is the most common cause of dementia, comprising over two thirds
of all cases in most studies [50]. There is no confirmed biologic marker for
AD; however, standardized clinical criteria have improved the accuracy of
diagnosis to greater than 85% [8,51]. Onset is insidious, and progression
466 G.W. Ross, J.D. Bowen / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 455–476
Vascular dementia
The concept of (VsD) is evolving as it becomes increasingly recognized
that in addition to causing cognitive decline alone, vascular lesions modify
the expression of Alzheimer pathologic findings [54]. VsD is clinically recog-
nized in patients with a prior history of strokes, focal neurologic findings, or
strokes on neuroimaging. The classic sudden onset with stepwise deteriora-
tion of VsD probably occurs in less than half of cases. Because of these clini-
cally silent cases, it is important to obtain neuroimaging in all cases of
dementia. The cognitive deficits associated with VsD depend on the lesion
location. Large vessel strokes cause cortical deficits, such as aphasia and
focal neurologic deficits, such as hemianopia and hemiparesis. Multiple
small vessel strokes cause a subcortical clinical presentation with forgetful-
ness and prominent executive function deficits. Pseudobulbar palsy, gait dis-
turbances, and urinary incontinence are also common. Although several sets
of criteria exist for VsD [5,9,55], the clinician should be concerned with
stroke prevention in any dementia patient with cerebrovascular risk factors
or vascular lesions on neuroimaging.
Frontotemporal dementia
Frontotemporal dementia consists of a clinically and pathologically
heterogeneous group of disorders, including Pick’s disease. These have in
common degeneration of the frontal and temporal lobes. Diagnostic criteria
for frontotemporal dementia are listed in Table 4 [56]. Behavioral changes,
including disinhibition, impulsiveness, social inappropriateness, apathy, and
withdrawal, are early and prominent features. These behavioral changes pro-
vide the most important clue allowing the differentiation of this condition
G.W. Ross, J.D. Bowen / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 455–476 467
Table 4
Criteria for the diagnosis of frontotemporal dementia
A. Dominant deficits in behavior and conduct appearing early in the course
Loss of personal awareness (neglect of hygiene and grooming)
Loss of social graces and awareness
Disinhibition (sexually provocative or demanding, inappropriate jocularity), overactivity,
or restlessness, often with a stereotyped repertoire
Impulsivity, distractibility
Hyperorality (dietary changes, excessive eating, smoking, or alcohol consumption,
preference for sweet foods or food fads, oral exploration of objects)
Withdrawal from social contact, apathy or inertia
Stereotyped or perseverative behaviors (wandering, repetitive clapping, humming or
singing, ritualistic toileting, dressing)
B. Speech output changes
Progressive reduction of speech (late mutism, economy of speech)
Stereotypy of speech (few repeated phrases or themes), perseveration
Echolalia
C. Physical signs
Early or prominent primitive or ‘‘frontal’’ reflexes
Early incontinence
Late akinesia, rigidity, tremor
D. Deficits in social comportment, behavior, judgment, or language are out of proportion to
memory deficit. Memory loss is variable, often seems to result from lack of concern or effort;
frontal lobe impairments most notable: abstraction, planning, self-regulation of behavior
from AD. Language disturbances may also appear early, whereas visuospatial
function remains intact until later in the illness [57]. Neuroimaging may allow
the visualization of focal atrophy, but the disease can often be recognized
clinically before changes on routine imaging are apparent [58]. Single photon
emission CT imaging demonstrates hypoperfusion in the frontal and temporal
lobes before atrophy in these regions is evident on structural imaging [58].
however, the practitioner should be familiar with the basic clinical distinc-
tions, because the correct diagnosis affects patient management.
Dementia occurs at some time during the course of the illness in approxi-
mately 40% of patients with Parkinson’s disease [59]. Cognitive decline begins
at least 1 year after the onset of the movement disorder and is associated with
impaired recall that is aided with recognition cues, prominent executive func-
tion deficits, and intact language [60]. Dementia with Lewy bodies, in contrast
to Parkinson’s disease, is identifiable by fluctuating cognitive performance,
well-formed visual hallucinations unrelated to dopaminergic therapy, and
parkinsonism that emerges simultaneously with the cognitive impairment.
These features allow it to be differentiated from AD and Parkinson’s disease.
It is particularly important to recognize dementia with Lewy bodies because of
the severe adverse reactions that these patients may have to neuroleptic med-
ications used to treat behavioral problems. Because of this, neuroleptics
should be avoided in the treatment of this disease [61]. Diagnostic criteria for
identifying dementia with Lewy bodies are listed in Table 5 [62]. Other degen-
erative parkinsonian syndromes are much less common. Progressive supra-
nuclear palsy begins at the age of 40 years or older and is characterized by a
rigid-akinetic form of parkinsonism, dementia, supranuclear gaze palsy,
severe dysarthria, neck rigidity (usually in extension), minimal tremor, and fre-
quent falls [63]. Multisystem atrophy refers to a group of adult-onset progres-
sive neurodegenerative disorders (ie, striatonigral degeneration, Shy-Drager
syndrome, olivopontocerebellar atrophy) characterized by parkinsonism that
Table 5
Criteria for diagnosing Lewy body dementia
A. The central requirement is progressive cognitive decline of sufficient magnitude to interfere
with normal social or occupational function. Prominent or persistent memory impairment
may not necessarily occur in the early stages but is usually evident with progression. Deficits
on tests of attention and frontal-subcortical skills and visuospatial ability may be especially
prominent.
B. Two of the following are required for a probable diagnosis, and one for a possible diagnosis
of dementia with Lewy bodies:
Fluctuating cognition with pronounced variations in attention and alertness
Recurrent visual hallucinations that are typically well formed and detailed
Spontaneous motor features of parkinsonism
D. Features supportive of the diagnosis are as follows:
Repeated falls
Syncope
Transient loss of consciousness
Neuroleptic sensitivity (deterioration in cognitive function, parkinsonism, drowsiness,
and some features of so-called neuroleptic malignant syndrome)
Systematized delusions
Hallucinations in other modalities
E. A diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies is less likely in the presence of the following:
Stroke disease, evident as focal neurologic signs or on brain imaging
Evidence on physical examination and investigation of any physical illness or other brain
disorder sufficient to account for the clinical picture
G.W. Ross, J.D. Bowen / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 455–476 469
Toxin-related dementias
Wernicke’s encephalopathy, characterized by delirium, ophthalmoplegia,
and ataxia, is related to thiamine deficiency and is associated with pro-
longed heavy use of alcohol. Prompt thiamine replacement may reverse the
delirium and other signs, although some patients still evolve to Korsakoff’s
syndrome, an amnestic disorder that may be permanent [72]. Although the
DSM-IV recognizes alcohol-induced dementia that persists after heavy
drinking, the lack of pathologic findings in the brain related to alcohol abuse
has called into question the direct toxic effect of alcohol on the brain [5,73].
The increased risk of head injury and association of heavy prolonged alco-
hol use with blood disorders that can lead to stroke may also cause persis-
tent cognitive impairment.
Medications are a common cause of delirium and cognitive decline in the
elderly and may be responsible for 1.5% to 10% of all clinically diagnosed
dementias [74]. Patients with dementia may be particularly susceptible to
further cognitive impairment with medication use [75–77]. Sedatives and
hypnotics (eg, benzodiazepines), anticholinergic drugs (eg, trihexyphenidyl
and meclizine), antidepressants (eg, amitriptyline), analgesics, and antihy-
pertensives (eg, beta-blockers) are all commonly prescribed medications that
can cause cognitive impairment reversible on withdrawal of the medication
[18]. Medications used to treat the behavioral complications of dementia,
such as the antipsychotics haloperidol and thioridazine, may worsen the
memory impairment. The lowest possible dose should be used to control the
target symptoms, and the need for continued use of the medication should
be assessed at regular intervals.
feet and hands, loss of vibratory and position sensation, ataxia, extremity
weakness, and neuropsychiatric disturbances. Confusion, memory loss, para-
noia, hallucinations, depression, and irritability are all features of the neuro-
psychiatric syndrome that has been described as megaloblastic madness
[78,79]. Importantly, among those with cognitive and behavioral symptoms
and vitamin B12 deficiency, as many as one fourth do not have the megalo-
blastic anemia that is classically associated with pernicious anemia [80].
Focal areas of cerebral demyelination characterize the brain pathologic find-
ings much like the loss of myelin in the posterior and lateral columns of the
spinal cord associated with this syndrome. Treatment consists of intramus-
cular administration of vitamin B12. This usually results in improvement in
the motor and sensory deficits and has been reported to improve language
and frontal function in individuals with vitamin B12–related cognitive
impairment [81]. Folate deficiency may cause a similar syndrome that can
be treated with oral folic acid supplements.
Dementia associated with hypothyroidism is characterized by inatten-
tion, memory impairment, and impaired abstraction. Psychosis may occur
[4]. Owing partly to the common assessment of thyroid function, the myx-
edema syndrome of edema, weight gain, thick skin, cold intolerance, consti-
pation, and psychomotor slowing is rarely seen. More often, clinicians
encounter patients with elevated thyrotropin levels, normal thyroid hor-
mone levels, and absent or mild symptoms [82]. Evidence from community-
based studies demonstrates an association between this condition known as
subclinical hypothyroidism and cognitive impairment [83]. Furthermore,
treatment with thyroxine has been associated with significant improvement
in cognitive function [82].
Summary
The initial approach to the patient with memory complaints should consist
of a focused history, mental status examination, and functional assessment.
Patients with MCI should be monitored every 6 to 12 months for conversion
to dementia. Delirium, depression, amnestic disorders, and aphasias should
be considered in the differential diagnosis of memory impairment. Once a
diagnosis of dementia is made, patients should have a brain CT or MRI scan
and laboratory tests to assist with determining the cause.
It is crucial that dementia be recognized and evaluated at the earliest
stage so as to begin appropriate therapy and allow the patient to have a role
in management decisions. In the future, therapies for MCI may prevent con-
version to dementia. The need for early recognition makes the development
of diagnostic tools, such as quantitative or functional neuroimaging, and
genetic or clinical biologic markers essential.
References
[1] Ernst RL, Hay JW, Fenn C, Tinklenberg J, Yesavage JA. Cognitive function and the costs
of Alzheimer disease. An exploratory study. Arch Neurol 1997;54:687–93.
[2] Evans DA, Funkenstein HH, Albert MS, Scherr PA, Cook NR, Chown MJ. Prevalence of
Alzheimer’s disease in a community population of older persons: higher than previously
reported. JAMA 1989;262:2551–6.
[3] Rice DP, Fox PJ, Max W, Webber PA, Lindeman DA, Hauck WW. The economic burden
of Alzheimer’s disease care. Health Aff (Millwood) 1993;12:164–76.
[4] Cummings JL, Benson DF. Dementia: a clinical approach. Boston, MA: Butterworths; 1992.
p. 1–17.
[5] American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders.
4th edition. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994. p. 143–7.
[6] Ross GW, Abbott RD, Petrovitch H, Masaki K, Abbott RD, Teng EL, et al. Frequency
and characteristics of silent dementia among elderly Japanese-American men. The
Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. JAMA 1997;277:800–5.
G.W. Ross, J.D. Bowen / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 455–476 473
[7] Callahan CM, Hendrie HC, Tierney WM. Documentation and evaluation of cognitive
impairment in elderly primary care patients. Ann Intern Med 1995;122:422–9.
[8] McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, Katzman R, Price D, Stadlan EM. Clinical
Diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the
auspices of the Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s
Disease. Neurology 1984;34:939–44.
[9] Roman GC, Tatemichi TK, Erkinjuntti T, Cummings JL, Masdeav JC, Garcia JH, et al.
Vascular dementia: diagnostic criteria for research studies. Report of the NINDS-AIREN
International Workshop [see comments]. Neurology 1993;43:250–60.
[10] Fuh JL, Teng EL, Lin KN, et al. The Informant Questionnaire on Cognitive Decline in the
Elderly (IQCODE) as a screening tool for dementia for a predominantly illiterate Chinese
population. Neurology 1995;45:92–6.
[11] Jorm AF, Jacomb PA. The Informant Questionnaire on Cognitive Decline in the Elderly
(IQCODE): socio-demographic correlates, reliability, validity and some norms. Psychol
Med 1989;19:1015–22.
[12] Cummings JL. The Neuropsychiatric Inventory: assessing psychopathology in dementia
patients. Neurology 1997;48:S10–6.
[13] Reisberg B, Borenstein J, Salob SP, Ferris SH, Franssen E, Georgotas A. Behavioral
symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease: phenomenology and treatment. J Clin Psychiatry
1987;48:9–15.
[14] Blessed G, Tomlinson BE, Roth M. The association between quantitative measures of
dementia and of senile change in the cerebral grey matter of elderly subjects. Br J
Psychiatry 1968;114:797–811.
[15] Pfeffer RI, Kurosaki TT, Harrah CH Jr, Chance JM, Filos S. Measurement of functional
activities in older adults in the community. J Gerontol 1982;37:323–9.
[16] Lawton MP, Brody EM. Assessment of older people: self-maintaining and instrumental
activities of daily living. Gerontologist 1969;9:179–86.
[17] Corey-Bloom J, Thal LJ, Galasko D, Folstein M, Drachman D, Raskind M, et al.
Diagnosis and evaluation of dementia. Neurology 1995;45:211–8.
[18] Costa PT, Williams TF, Albert MS, Butters NM, Folstein MF, Gilman S, et al. Recog-
nition and initial assessment of AlzheimerÕs disease and related disorders: clinical practice
guideline 19. AHCPR publication 97–0702. Rockville, Maryland: US Department of
Health and Human Services, Public Health Service, Agency for Health Care Policy and
Research; 1996.
[19] Cummings JL, Booss J, Dickinson BD, Hazlewood MG, Jarvik LF, Matuszewski K,
et al. Dementia identification and assessment: guidelines for primary care practitioners.
Washington, DC: US Department of Veterans Affairs, and Oakbrook, IL: University
Health System Consortium; 1997.
[20] Knopman DS, DeKosky ST, Cummings JL, Chui H, Corey-Bloom J, Relkin N, et al.
Practice parameter: diagnosis of dementia (an evidence-based review). Report of the
Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. Neurology
2001;56:1143–53.
[21] Ross GW, Cummings JL, Benson DF. Speech and language alterations in dementia
syndromes: characteristics and treatment. Aphasiology 1990;4:339–52.
[22] Strub RL, Black FW. The mental status examination in neurology. Philadelphia: FA Davis
Company; 1985.
[23] Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. Mini-Mental State: a practical method for grading
the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975;12:189–98.
[24] Teng EL, Hasegawa K, Homma A, Imai Y, Larson E, Graves A, et al. The Cognitive
Abilities Screening Instrument (CASI): a practical test for cross-cultural epidemiological
studies of dementia. Int Psychogeriatr 1994;6:45–58.
[25] Rosen WG, Mohs RC, Davis KL. A new rating scale for Alzheimer’s disease. Am J
Psychiatry 1984;141:1356–64.
474 G.W. Ross, J.D. Bowen / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 455–476
[26] Morris JC, Heyman A, Mohs RC, Hughes JP, van Belle G, Fillenbaum G, et al. The
Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD), part 1: clinical and
neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 1989;39:1159–65.
[27] Crum RM, Anthony JC, Bassett SS, Folstein MF. Population-based norms for the Mini-
Mental State Examination by age and educational level. JAMA 1993;269:2386–91.
[28] Yano K, Grove JS, Masaki KH, White LR, Petroritch H, Chen R, et al. The effects of
childhood residence in Japan and testing language on cognitive performance in late life
among Japanese American men in Hawaii. J Am Geriatr Soc 2000;48:199–204.
[29] Small GW, Rabins PV, Barry PP, Buckholtz NS, DeKosky ST, Ferris SH, et al. Diagnosis
and treatment of Alzheimer disease and related disorders. Consensus statement of the
American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry, the Alzheimer’s Association, and the
American Geriatrics Society. JAMA 1997;278:1363–71.
[30] Practice parameter for diagnosis and evaluation of dementia (summary statement). Report
of the Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. Neuro-
logy 1994;44:2203–6.
[31] Gifford DR, Holloway RG, Vickrey BG. Systematic review of clinical prediction rules for
neuroimaging in the evaluation of dementia. Arch Intern Med 2000;160:2855–62.
[32] Chui H, Zhang Q. Evaluation of dementia: a systematic study of the usefulness of the
American Academy of Neurology’s practice parameters. Neurology 1997;49:925–35.
[33] Jagust WJ. Neuroimaging in dementia. Neurol Clin 2000;18:885–902.
[34] Johnson RT, Gibbs CJ Jr. Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease and related transmissible spongiform
encephalopathies. N Engl J Med 1998;339:1994–2004.
[35] Petersen RC, Smith G, Kokmen E, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG. Memory function in normal
aging. Neurology 1992;42:396–401.
[36] Howieson DB, Holm LA, Kaye JA, Oken BS. Neurologic function in the optimally healthy
oldest old. Neuropsychological evaluation. Neurology 1993;43:1882–6.
[37] Small SA, Stern Y, Tang M, Mayeux R. Selective decline in memory function among
healthy elderly. Neurology 1999;52:1392–6.
[38] Rubin EH, Storandt M, Miller JP, Kinscherf DA, Grant EA, Morris JC, et al. A
prospective study of cognitive function and onset of dementia in cognitively healthy elders.
Arch Neurol 1998;55:395–401.
[39] Petersen RC, Stevens JC, Ganguli M, Tangalos EG, Cummings JL, DeKosky ST. Practice
parameter: early detection of dementia: mild cognitive impairment (an evidence-based
review). report of the Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of
Neurology. Neurology 2001;56:1133–42.
[40] Hogan DB, McKeith IG. Of MCI and dementia: improving diagnosis and treatment.
Neurology 2001;56:1131–2.
[41] Burt DB, Zembar MJ, Niederehe G. Depression and memory impairment: a meta-analysis
of the association, its pattern, and specificity. Psychol Bull 1995;117:285–305.
[42] Bolla KI, Lindgren KN, Bonaccorsy C, Bleecker ML. Memory complaints in older adults.
Fact or fiction? Arch Neurol 1991;48:61–4.
[43] Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1960;23:56–61.
[44] Sheikh JI, Yesavage JA. Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS): recent evidence and deve-
lopment of a shorter version. In: Brink TL, editor. Clinical gerontology: a guide to assess-
ment and intervention. New York: The Haworth Press; 1986 p. 165–73.
[45] Visser PJ, Verhey FR, Ponds RW, Kester A, Jolles J, et al. Distinction between preclinical
Alzheimer’s disease and depression. J Am Geriatr Soc 2000;48:479–84.
[46] Tune L, Ross C. Delirium. In: Coffey CE, Cummings JL,editor. Textbook of geriatric
neuropsychiatry. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press; 1994. p. 351–68.
[47] Cummings JL. Clinical neuropsychiatry. Orlando, FL: Grune & Stratton; 1985. p. 36–47.
[48] Bowen J, Teri L, Kukull W, McCormick W, McCurry SM, Larson EB, et al. Progression to
dementia in patients with isolated memory loss. Lancet 1997;349:763–5.
[49] Benson DF. Aphasia, alexia, and agraphia. New York: Churchill Livingstone; 1979.
G.W. Ross, J.D. Bowen / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 455–476 475
[50] Graves AB, Larson EB, Edland SD, Bowen JD, McCormick WC, McCurry SM, et al.
Prevalence of dementia and its subtypes in the Japanese American population of King
County, Washington State. The Kame Project. Am J Epidemiol 1996;144:760–71.
[51] Gearing M, Mirra SS, Hedreen JC, Sumi SM, Hansen LA, Heyman A. The Consortium
to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD). Part X. Neuropathology
confirmation of the clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 1995;45:461–6.
[52] Cummings JL, Miller B, Hill MA, Nashkes R. Neuropsychiatric aspects of multi-infarct
dementia and dementia of the Alzheimer type. Arch Neurol 1987;44:389–93.
[53] Chui HC, Teng EL, Henderson VW, Moy AC. Clinical subtypes of dementia of the
Alzheimer type. Neurology 1985;35:1544–50.
[54] Snowdon DA, Greiner LH, Mortimer JA, Riley KP, Greiner PA, Markesberry WR. Brain
infarction and the clinical expression of Alzheimer disease. The Nun Study. JAMA 1997;
277:813–7.
[55] Chui HC, Victoroff JI, Margolin D, Jagust W, Shankle R, Katzman R. Criteria for the
diagnosis of ischemic vascular dementia proposed by the State of California Alzheimer’s
Disease Diagnostic and Treatment Centers. Neurology 1992;42:473–80.
[56] The Lund and Manchester Groups. Clinical and neuropathological criteria for fronto-
temporal dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1994;57:416–8.
[57] Hodges JR. Frontotemporal dementia (Pick’s disease): clinical features and assessment.
Neurology 2001;56(Suppl):S6–10.
[58] Miller BL, Cummings JL, Villanueva-Meyer J, Boone K, Mehringer CM, Lesser IM, et al.
Frontal lobe degeneration: clinical, neuropsychological, and SPECT characteristics.
Neurology 1991;41:1374–82.
[59] Cummings JL. Intellectual impairment in Parkinson’s disease: clinical, pathologic, and
biochemical correlates. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 1988;1:24–36.
[60] Ross GW, Mahler ME, Cummings JL. The dementia syndrome of Parkinson’s disease:
cortical and subcortical features. In: Huber SJ, Cummings JL, editors. Neurobehavioral
aspects of Parkinson’s disease. New York: Oxford University Press; 1992. p. 132–48.
[61] McKeith IG, Galasko D, Kosaka K, Perry EK, Dickson DW, Hansen LA, et al. Consensus
guidelines for the clinical and pathologic diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB):
report of the Consortium on DLB International Workshop. Neurology 1996;47:1113–24.
[62] McKeith IG, Perry EK, Perry RH. Report of the Second Dementia with Lewy body
International Workshop: diagnosis and treatment. Consortium on Dementia with Lewy
Bodies. Neurology 1999;53:902–5.
[63] Golbe LI. Progressive supranuclear palsy. In: Watts RL, Koller WC, editors. Move-
ment disorders: neurologic principles and practice. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1997.
p. 279–95.
[64] Quinn N. Multi-system atrophy—the nature of the beast. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
1989;52(Suppl):78–89.
[65] Hurtig HI. Vascular parkinsonism. In: Stern MB, Koller WC, editors. Parkinsonian
syndromes. New York: Marcel Dekker; 1993. p. 81–93.
[66] Kumar R, Bergeron C, Pollanen MS, Lang AE. Cortical-basal ganglionic degeneration. In:
Jankovic J, Tolosa E, editors. Parkinson’s disease and movement disorders. Baltimore:
Williams & Wilkins; 1993. p. 297–316.
[67] Chen JC, Levy ML. Causes, epidemiology, and risk factors of chronic subdural hematoma.
Neurosurg Clin North Am 2000;11:399–406.
[68] Iantosca MR, Simon RH. Chronic subdural hematoma in adult and elderly patients.
Neurosurg Clin North Am 2000;11:447–54.
[69] Machulda MM, Haut MW. Clinical features of chronic subdural hematoma: neuropsy-
chiatric and neuropsychologic changes in patients with chronic subdural hematoma.
Neurosurg Clin North Am 2000;11:473–7.
[70] Voelker JL. Nonoperative treatment of chronic subdural hematoma. Neurosurg Clin
North Am 2000;11:507–13.
476 G.W. Ross, J.D. Bowen / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 455–476
[71] Kotwica Z. Treatment of chronic subdural hematoma by burr holes and closed-system
drainage. Neurosurg Clin North Am 2000;11:503–5.
[72] Victor M, Adams RD, Collins GC. The Wernicke-Korsakoff syndrome. Philadelphia:
FA Davis Company; 1989.
[73] Victor M. Alcoholic dementia. Can J Neurol Sci 1994;21:88–99.
[74] Bowen JD, Larson EB. Drug-induced cognitive impairment. Defining the problem and
finding solutions. Drugs Aging 1993;3:349–57.
[75] Clarfield AM. The reversible dementias: do they reverse? Ann Intern Med 1988;109:476–86.
[76] Larson EB, Kukull WA, Buchner D, Reifler BV. Adverse drug reactions associated with
global cognitive impairment in elderly persons. Ann Intern Med 1987;107:169–73.
[77] Larson EB, Reifler BV, Featherstone HJ, English DR. Dementia in elderly outpatients:
a prospective study. Ann Intern Med 1984;100:417–23.
[78] Holmes JM. Cerebral manifestations of vitamin-B12 deficiency. BMJ 1956;2:1394–6.
[79] Smith ADM. Megaloblastic madness. BMJ 1960;2:1840–5.
[80] Lindenbaum J, Healton EB, Savage DG, Brust JC, Garrett TJ, Podell R, et al. Neuropsy-
chiatric disorders caused by cobalamin deficiency in the absence of anemia or macrocytosis.
N Engl J Med 1988;318:1720–8.
[81] Eastley R, Wilcock GK, Bucks RS. Vitamin B12 deficiency in dementia and cognitive
impairment: the effects of treatment on neuropsychological function. Int J Geriatr
Psychiatry 2000;15:226–33.
[82] Cooper DS. Clinical practice. Subclinical hypothyroidism. N Engl J Med 2001;345:260–5.
[83] Ganguli M, Dodge HH, Chen P, Belle S, DeKosky ST. Ten-year incidence of dementia in a
rural elderly US community population: the MoVIES Project. Neurology 2000;54:1109–16.
[84] Corkill RG, Cadoux-Hudson TA. Normal pressure hydrocephalus: developments in
determining surgical prognosis. Curr Opin Neurol 1999;12:671–7.
[85] Thomsen AM, Borgesen SE, Bruhn P, Gjerris F. Prognosis of dementia in normal-pressure
hydrocephalus after a shunt operation. Ann Neurol 1986;20:304–10.
[86] Vanneste JA. Diagnosis and management of normal-pressure hydrocephalus. J Neurol
2000;247:5–14.
Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 477–499
History
Thomas Willis first described postapoplectic dementia in the seventeenth
century [4]. In 1894, Otto Binswanger and Alois Alzheimer separated VaD
from dementia paralytica (neurosyphilis), a common cause of dementia at
that time, and identified at least four different clinicopathologic forms of
VaD [5]. Based on their work, Kraepelin concluded in 1910 that arterio-
sclerotic insanity, also known as cerebral arteriosclerosis, was the most fre-
quent form of senile dementia [4]. This unwarranted simplification of the
complexity of the field implied that progressive narrowing and blockage
of large cerebral arteries led to decreased cerebral blood flow, neuronal
death, brain atrophy, and dementia. This concept persisted until the late
1970s, when the importance of AD as the main cause of senile dementia was
0025-7125/02/$ - see front matter 2002, Elsevier Science (USA). All rights reserved.
PII: S 0 0 2 5 - 7 1 2 5 ( 0 2 ) 0 0 0 0 8 - 1
478 G.C. Román / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 477–499
Pathogenesis
VaD may be caused by a number of vascular lesions and pathogenetic
mechanisms as listed in Table 1 [8]. Of particular importance in the elderly
is small vessel disease, which causes lacunes and white matter ischemic
lesions. These ischemic lesions may interrupt the recently described frontal
Table 1
Pathological lesions capable of producing vascular dementia
1. Multi-infarct dementia
Multiple large complete infarcts, cortico-subcortical in location, usually with perifocal
incomplete infarction involving the white matter
2. Strategic infarct dementia
A single brain infarct, often lacunar in size, damages functionally critical areas of the brain
(angular gyrus, thalamus, basal forebrain, posterior cerebral artery and anterior cerebral
artery territories)
3. Small-vessel disease with dementia
Subcortical
Binswanger’s disease
CADASIL (cerebral autosomal dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and
leukoencephalopathy)
Lacunar dementia or lacunar state (état lacunaire)
Multiple lacunes with extensive perifocal incomplete infarctions
Cortical and subcortical
Hypertensive and arteriolosclerotic angiopathy
Amyloid angiopathies (including British dementia)
Collagen-vascular disease with dementia
4. Ischemic-hypoxic dementia (hypoperfusive)
Diffuse anoxic-ischemic encephalopathy
Restricted injury due to selective vulnerability
Incomplete white-matter infarction
Border-zone infarction
5. Hemorrhagic dementia
Traumatic subdural hematoma
Subarachnoid hemorrhage
Cerebral hematoma
Venous thrombosis
6. Other mechanisms
Modified from Brun A. Pathology and pathophysiology of cerebrovascular dementia: pure
subgroups of obstructive and hypoperfusive etiology. Dementia 1994;5:145–7; with permission.
G.C. Román / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 477–499 479
Table 2
Behavioral manifestations of subcortical vascular dementia caused by lesions interrupting
prefrontal-subcortical circuits
1. Dorso-lateral prefrontal circuit lesions
Lesions
May include cortical strokes involving dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, infarctions at head
of the caudate nucleus, lacunar lesions in putamen or thalamus, or ischemic white
matter lesions involving loop segments, such as capsular genu lacunes
Manifestations
Executive dysfunction, poor word list generation and decreased verbal fluency, impaired
motor programming, perseveration, impersistence, and difficulty with set shifting (loss
of Luria’s kinetic melody)
2. Orbito-frontal-subcortical circuit lesions
Lesions
May include anterior cerebral artery strokes involving lateral orbitofrontal cortical
territories, or subcortical circuit lesions interrupting connections at the level of caudate,
basal ganglia, thalamus or white matter loop links
Manifestations
Disinhibited behaviors, mania, and obsessive-compulsive disorder
3. Medial-frontal (cingulate) cortex-subcortical circuits
Lesions
May include anterior cerebral artery strokes involving medial frontal cortical territories,
or subcortical circuit lesions interrupting connections at the level of caudate, basal
ganglia, thalamus or white matter loop links
Manifestations
Apathy, slowing of information processing, mood changes, or vascular depression
Modified from Cummings JL. Frontal-subcortical circuits and human behavior. Arch
Neurol 1993;50:873–80; and Cummings JL. Anatomic and behavioral aspects of frontal-
subcortical circuits. Ann NY Acad Sci 1995;769:1–13; with permission.
480 G.C. Román / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 477–499
Epidemiology
AD and VaD are the most common causes of senile dementia, with VaD
ranking second after AD [3,38–40]. Cerebrovascular pathologic findings and
heart disease are common in the elderly, and their prevalences increase with
age; as a result, histologic changes of AD in the elderly often coexist with
stroke and vascular pathologic changes [41,42]. Somewhat more unexpected
was the observation of the adjuvant role of cerebral infarction in AD [43],
whereby elderly subjects with the presence of one or two lacunar strokes
seem to require a lesser amount of senile plaques and neurofibrillary tangles
to manifest signs of dementia.
Recently, Hénon and her colleagues [44,45] demonstrated that careful
questioning can separate patients with preexisting memory loss and prob-
able AD from pure cases of poststroke VaD. About one third of poststroke
VaD patients harbor this dual pathologic profile. These cases should be
classified as ‘‘AD plus CVD’’ instead of the commonly used but incorrect
G.C. Román / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 477–499 481
Prevalence
Jorm et al [40] performed a meta-analysis of 47 international studies on
VaD prevalence and found that prevalence increases exponentially with age
up to the age of 95 years. Prevalence rates double every 5.3 years compared
with every 4.5 years for AD. Prevalence rates range from approximately 1.5
per 100 persons from the age of 70 to 75 years to 14 to 16 per 100 persons at
the age 80 years and above. In Europe, VaD was more prevalent than AD in
85-year-olds in Italy and Gothenburg (Sweden) [46]. VaD seems to be more
common in men in contrast to AD, which predominates in women. Also,
VaD has a peculiar geographic (racial) variation, being more prevalent in
Asian populations than in white populations. This is probably a result of the
preponderance of small vessel disease in Oriental races. The prevalence of
AD versus VaD tends to increase among ethnic Japanese who migrated to
Hawaii, however, indicating environmental interaction on genetic suscep-
tibility [47].
Incidence
Incidence data for VaD from longitudinal cohort studies are rather lim-
ited. Jorm and Jolley [48] performed a meta-analysis of 11 studies of VaD
with age-specific incidence data. More recently, Dubois and Herbert [49] used
age-based standardized incidence ratios to analyze data from 10 incidence
studies of VaD in comparison to the Canadian Study of Health and Aging.
Age-based standardized incidence ratios ranged from 0.42 to 2.68, confirming
the geographic variation of VaD. These variations may be a result of genetic,
environmental, or methodologic differences between studies.
Poststroke dementia
The most common form of VaD is probably poststroke dementia (also
known as MID when dementia develops after multiple strokes). The inci-
dence of poststroke dementia is relatively easy to ascertain when cognitive
tests are performed after stroke (typically, at 3 months after ictus). There are
methodologic variations that depend on the definition of dementia and test
cutoffs used. In Helsinki [50], the figures ranged from 6% to 25.5%, whereas
in New York City [51,52], poststroke dementia ranged between 27% and
41%. This means that in the United States alone, approximately 125,000
new cases of VaD after ischemic stroke occur every year (approximately one
third of the estimated 360,000 incident cases of AD) [39,53]. Based on these
figures, the prevalence of poststroke VaD would be above 1 million elderly
people currently suffering from poststroke VaD [53]. It should be noted,
however, that most cases of poststroke VaD remain undiagnosed.
482 G.C. Román / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 477–499
Table 3
Principal risk factors for poststroke vascular dementia
1. Age: older age
2. Education: lower educational level
3. Personal factors: lower-income, current smokers
4. Genetic factors: family history of dementia
5. Stroke type: recurrent strokes
6. Stroke location: left-sided lesions, ‘‘strategic strokes’’ (ie, posterior association areas such as
gyrus angularis; posterior cerebral artery territories including paramedian thalamic artery
territory, inferomedial temporal lobes, and hippocampus; watershed or border-zone infarcts
mainly involving superior frontal and parietal regions; bilateral anterior cerebral artery
territories, anterior choroidal artery strokes, and basal forebrain lesions; and frontal white
matter lesions); inferior capsular genu stroke producing diaschisis of frontal lobes and
cerebellum
7. Stroke volume: lesions larger than 50–100 ml of tissue destruction, larger peri-lesional
incomplete ischemic areas involving white matter, larger periventricular white matter
ischemic lesions
8. Stroke complications: hypoxic and ischemic complications of acute stroke (ie, seizures,
cardiac arrhythmias, aspiration pneumonia, hypotension)
9. Stroke manifestations: dysphagia, gait limitations, urinary impairment
the inferior genu of the internal capsule, causing ipsilateral blood flow reduc-
tion to the inferomedial-frontal cortex by a mechanism of diaschisis [71]. This
is considered a thalamocortical disconnection syndrome [69]. An unusual
case of subcortical dementia resulting from a giant lacune with a unilateral
left-sided mammillothalamic tract lesion has been recently reported [72].
Table 4
NINDS-AIREN diagnostic criteria for vascular dementia
I. The criteria for the diagnosis of probable VaD include all of the following:
1. Dementia: Impairment of memory and two or more cognitive domains (including
executive function), interfering with ADLs and not due to physical effects of stroke
alone. Exclusion criteria: Alterations of consciousness, delirium, psychoses, severe
aphasia or deficits precluding testing, systemic disorders, Alzheimer’s disease, or other
forms of dementia.
2. Cerebrovascular disease: Focal signs on neurological examination (hemiparesis, lower
facial weakness, Babinski sign, sensory deficit, hemianopia, dysarthria) consistent with
stroke (with or without history of stroke, and evidence of relevant CVD by brain CT or
MRI including multiple large-vessel infarcts or a single strategically placed infarct
(angular gyrus, thalamus, basal forebrain, or PCA or ACA territories), as well as
multiple basal ganglia and while matter lacunes or extensive periventricular white matter
lesions, or combinations thereof. Exclusion criteria: Absence of cerebrovascular lesions
on CT or MRI.
3. A relationship between the above two disorders is manifested or inferred by the presence
of one or more of the following:
A. Onset of dementia within 3 months following a recognized stroke.
B. Abrupt deterioration in cognitive functions or fluctuating, stepwise progression of
cognitive deficits.
II. Clinical features consistent with the diagnosis of probable VaD include the following:
1. Early presence of gait disturbances (small step gait or marche à petits pas, or magnetic,
apraxic-ataxic or parkinsonian gait).
2. History of unsteadiness and frequent, unprovoked falls.
3. Early urinary frequency, urgency, and other urinary symptoms not explained by
urologic disease.
4. Pseudobulbar palsy.
5. Personality and mood changes, abulia, depression, emotional incontinence, or other
deficits including psychomotor retardation and abnormal executive function.
III. Features that make the diagnosis of VaD uncertain or unlikely include:
1. Early onset of memory deficit and progressive worsening of memory and other cognitive
functions such as language (transcortical sensory aphasia), motor skills (apraxia), and
perception (agnosia), in the absence of corresponding focal lesions on brain imaging.
2. Absence of focal neurological signs, other than cognitive disturbances.
3. Absence of CVD on CT or MRI.
Abbreviations: ACA, anterior cerebral artery; ADLs, activities of daily living; CT, com-
puterized tomography; CVD, cerebrovascular disease; MRI, magnetic resonance imaging;
PCA, posterior cerebral artery; VaD; vascular dementia.
From Román GC, Tatemichi TK, Erkinjuntti T, et al. Vascular dementia: diagnostic criteria
for research studies. Report of the NINDS-AIREN International Workshop. Neurology 1993;43:
250–60; with permission.
Table 5
Items of the ischemic score
Item Score (total: 18)
Abrupt onset 2
Stepwise deterioration 1
Fluctuating course 2
Nocturnal confusion 1
Preservation of personality 1
Depression 1
Somatic complaints 1
Emotional incontinence 1
History of hypertension 1
History of stroke 2
Associated atherosclerosis 1
Focal neurological symptoms 2
Focal neurological signs 2
Modified from Hachinski VC, Zilhka E, DuBoulay GH, McAllister VL, Marshall J.
Cerebral blood flow in dementia. Arch Neurol 1975;32:632–7; with permission.
G.C. Román / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 477–499 489
control subjects. The frequency and severity of CVD in older patients with
AD point to the possibility that vascular risk factors may predispose not
only to VaD but to the development of AD [103]. Population-based epide-
miologic data [104,105] have shown that vascular risk factors, such as hyper-
tension, carotid artery wall thickness, cholesterol, and peripheral vascular
disease, often occur in patients who develop AD. The vascular role of the
apolipoprotein E e4 allele [105,106], a risk factor for AD, may partially
explain this interaction.
Primary prevention
Of significant public health interest are the observations that the treat-
ment of hypertension [112–115] and the use of statins [116–119] have been
associated with decreased incidence of dementia in the elderly.
Memantine
This agent is a potent noncompetitive antagonist of the N-methyl-D-
aspartate receptor with nootropic properties [135]. Memantine has been
shown to be well tolerated and useful in severe dementia [136]. A recently
completed pivotal 28-week trial of memantine in patients with mild to mod-
erate VaD enrolled 321 patients who received 2 · 10 mg/d or placebo. The
study demonstrated good tolerance and improvement in cognitive tests,
including the Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale–Cognitive Subscale
(ADAS-Cog) and MMSE [137].
G.C. Román / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 477–499 491
Pentoxifylline (Trental)
This is a xanthine derivative with hemorheologic and immunomodula-
tory properties [138]. The multicenter European Pentoxifylline Multi-infarct
Dementia Study demonstrated significant cognitive improvement in the
MID form of VaD in comparison with placebo [139]; these findings con-
firmed previous results [140]. Recently, Solerte et al [141] described hemor-
heologic alterations in patients with AD but not in age-matched controls or
in patients with VaD. Abnormalities included hyperviscosity, increased sedi-
mentation rate, hyperfibrinogenemia, and increased acute-phase reactants;
these changes were correlated with increased levels of tumor necrosis fac-
tor-a and interferon-c. Pentoxifylline treatment lowered fibrinogen and
tumor necrosis factor-a levels and corrected these abnormalities.
Antiplatelet agents
Aspirin [142], triflusal [143], and Ginkgo biloba [144] have been used in
patients with MID or with AD plus CVD with modest positive results. It
should be noted that the MMSE was the primary endpoint for cognitive
evaluation in most of these patients.
Cholinesterase inhibitors
The encouraging results obtained with the use of cholinergic agents in
VaD may give some clinicians the mistaken impression that VaD is really
AD or, at best, a mixed dementia [145]. It seems clear that (at least in the old-
est patients) both vascular and degenerative mechanisms contribute to AD
dementia [146]. This overlap occurs, however, only in one third of patients
diagnosed with VaD [147]. Sometimes, such as in the case of an anterior chor-
oidal artery occlusion, the patients fulfill the criteria for AD [148].
Cholinergic drugs are effective in VaD because these patients have choli-
nergic deficits related to ischemic involvement of basal forebrain neurons
(nucleus basalis of Meynert) or their projections. Wallin et al [149] found
pronounced disturbances of the serotoninergic and cholinergic systems in
subcortical and cortical gray matter at postmortem brain examination of
patients with VaD. These widespread neurotransmitter deficits probably
are not caused by localized brain infarcts per se. Likewise, Tohgi et al [150]
found acetylcholine concentrations in the cerebrospinal fluid of patients with
BD and MID to be significantly lower than in controls, whereas choline
concentrations were higher than in controls or AD patients. Recently, using
postmortem brain tissue materials, Martin-Ruiz et al [151] demonstrated the
relative integrity of nicotinic receptors in definite cases of VaD. Nicotinic
receptors control cerebral vasodilatation.
Of the available cholinergic agents approved for the treatment of AD,
donepezil hydrochloride (Aricept), rivastigmine tartrate (Exelon), and gal-
antamine hydrobromide (Reminyl) have been used in patients with VaD.
A large, 24-week, multicenter, randomized, placebo-controlled interna-
tional trial of donepezil in VaD has been completed recently [152]. The trial
492 G.C. Román / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 477–499
Other agents
Atypical antipsychotic drugs, such as risperidone and olanzapine, have
been useful in the treatment of agitation and disruptive behaviors. The use
of cholinergic medications often controls these problems.
For some patients with depression and anxiety, the use of antidepres-
sants, such as the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors citalopram or ser-
traline, may be required. The use of tricyclic antidepressants in the elderly
patient with VaD is discouraged because of anticholinergic effects and
orthostatic hypotension.
Summary
VaD is the second most common cause of dementia in the elderly after
AD. VaD is defined as the loss of cognitive function resulting from ischemic,
ischemic-hypoxic, or hemorrhagic brain lesions as a result of CVD and car-
diovascular pathologic changes. Diagnosis requires (1) cognitive loss (often
predominantly subcortical), (2) vascular brain lesions demonstrated by imag-
ing, and (3) exclusion of other causes of dementia, such as AD. VaD is
excluded by brain imaging showing no evidence of vascular lesions. VaD
may be caused by multiple strokes (MID or poststroke dementia) but also
by single strategic strokes, multiple lacunes, and hypoperfusive lesions such
as border zone infarcts and ischemic periventricular leukoencephalopathy
(Binswanger’s disease). Primary and secondary prevention of stroke and
cardiovascular disease decreases the burden of VaD. Genetic advice is
needed in patients with familial forms, such as CADASIL. Treatment
G.C. Román / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 477–499 493
References
[1] Román GC. Vascular dementia. New Issues in Neurosciences 1992;4:1–62.
[2] Román GC, Tatemichi TK, Erkinjuntti T, et al. Vascular dementia: diagnostic criteria for
research studies. Report of the NINDS-AIREN International Work Group. Neurology
1993;43:250–60.
[3] Prencipe M, Ferretti C, Casini AR, et al. Stroke, disability, and dementia. Results of a
population survey. Stroke 1997;28:531–6.
[4] Román GC. A historical review of the concept of vascular dementia: lessons from the past
for the future. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 1999;13(Suppl 3):S4–8.
[5] Román GC. On the history of lacunes, état criblé, and the white matter lesions of vascular
dementia. Cerebrovasc Dis 2002;13(Suppl 2):1–6.
[6] Tomlinson BE, Blessed G, Roth M. Observations on the brains of demented old people.
J Neurol Sci 1970;11:205–42.
[7] Hachinski V, Lassen N, Marshall J. Multi-infarct dementia: a cause of mental deterio-
ration in the elderly. Lancet 1974;14:207–10.
[8] Brun A. Pathology and pathophysiology of cerebrovascular dementia: pure subgroups of
obstructive and hypoperfusive etiology. Dementia 1994;5:145–7.
[9] Wolfe N, Linn R, Babikian VL, et al. Frontal systems impairment following multiple
lacunar infarcts. Arch Neurol 1990;47:129–32.
[10] Ishii N, Nishihara Y, Imamura T. Why do frontal lobe symptoms predominate in vascular
dementia with lacunes? Neurology 1986;36:340–5.
[11] Alexander GE, DeLong MR, Strick PL. Parallel organization of functionally segregated
circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Ann Rev Neurosci 1986;9:137–81.
[12] Cummings JL. Frontal-subcortical circuits and human behavior. Arch Neurol 1993;50:873–80.
[13] Mega MS, Cummings JL. Frontal-subcortical circuits and neuropsychiatric disorders.
J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1994;6:358–70.
[14] Cummings JL. Anatomic and behavioral aspects of frontal-subcortical circuits. Ann NY
Acad Sci 1995;769:1–13.
[15] Masterman DL, Cummings JL. Frontal-subcortical circuits: the anatomic basis of exec-
utive, social and motivated behaviors. J Psychopharmacol 1997;11:107–14.
[16] Fuster JM. Executive frontal functions. Exp Brain Res 2000;133:66–70.
[17] Fuster JM. Prefrontal neurons in networks of executive memory. Brain Res Bull 2000;52:
331–6.
[18] Masterman DL, Cummings JL. Frontal-subcortical circuits: the anatomic basis of exec-
utive, social and motivated behaviors. J Psychopharmacol 1997;11:107–14.
[19] Cummings JL. Vascular subcortical dementias: clinical aspects. Dementia 1994;5:177–80.
[20] Cummings JL. Subcortical dementia as a manifestation of cerebrovascular disease. In:
Román GC, editor. Vascular dementia. Proceedings of the NINDS-AIREN International
Workshop on Vascular Dementia. NIH, Bethesda, MD, April 19–21, 1991. New Issues in
Neurosciences 1992;4:151–8.
[21] Román GC. Senile dementia of the Binswanger type: a vascular form of dementia in the
elderly. JAMA 1987;258:1782–8.
[22] Román GC, Royall DR. Executive control function: a rational basis for the diagnosis of
vascular dementia. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 1999;13(Suppl 3):S69–80.
494 G.C. Román / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 477–499
[23] Moody DM, Bell MA, Challa VE. Features of cerebral vascular pattern that predict
vulnerability to perfusion or oxygenation deficits: an anatomical study. AJNR Am J
Neuroradiol 1990;11:431–9.
[24] Ravens JR. Vascular changes in the human senile brain. Adv Neurol 1978;20:487–501.
[25] Duvernoy HM, Delon S, Vannson JL. Cortical blood vessels of the human brain. Brain
Res Bull 1981;7:519–79.
[26] Furuta A, Ishii N, Nishihara Y, Horie A. Medullary arteries in aging and dementia.
Stroke 1991;22:442–6.
[27] Awad IA, Johnson PC, Spetzler RF, Modak JA. Incidental subcortical lesions identified
on magnetic resonance imaging in the elderly: II. Postmortem pathological correlations.
Stroke 1986;17:1090–2.
[28] van Swieten JC, van den Hout JHW, van Ketel BA, et al. Periventricular lesions in the
white matter on magnetic resonance imaging in the elderly: a morphometric correlation
with arteriolosclerosis. Brain 1991;114:761–74.
[29] Rogers RL, Meyers JS, Mortel KF, et al. Decreased cerebral blood flow precedes multiinfarct
dementia, but follows senile dementia of the Alzheimer type. Neurology 1986;36:1–6.
[30] Yao H, Sadoshima S, Kuwabara Y, et al. Cerebral blood flow and oxygen metabolism in
patients with vascular dementia of the Binswanger type. Stroke 1990;21:1694–9.
[31] Guo Z, Viitanen M, Fratiglioni L, Winblad B. Low blood pressure and dementia in
elderly people: the Kungsholmen Project. BMJ 1996;312:805–8.
[32] Zuccalà G, Onder G, Pedone C, et al. Hypotension and cognitive impairment: selective
association in patients with heart failure. Neurology 2001;57:1986–92.
[33] Pullicino PM, Hart J. Cognitive impairment in congestive heart failure? Embolism vs
hypoperfusion. Neurology 2001;57:1945–6.
[34] O’Sullivan M, Jones DK, Summers P, et al. Evidence for cortical ‘‘disconnection’’ as a
mechanism of age-related cognitive decline. Neurology 2001;57:632–8.
[35] Baron JC, Levasseur M, Mazoyer B, et al. Thalamocortical diaschisis: positron emission
tomography in humans. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1992;55:935–42.
[36] Moser DJ, Cohen RA, Paul RH, et al. Executive function and magnetic resonance
imaging subcortical hyperintensities in vascular dementia. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol
Behav Neurol 2001;14:89–92.
[37] Schmahmann JD, Sherman JC. The cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome. Brain 1998;121:
561–79.
[38] Román GC. Vascular dementia. In: Fisher M, editor. Clinical atlas of cerebrovascular
disorders. London: Wolfe; 2000. p. 13.1–23.
[39] Chui H. Vascular dementia, a new beginning. Shifting focus from clinical phenotype to
ischemic brain injury. Neurol Clin 2000;18:951–77.
[40] Jorm AF, Korten AE, Henderson AS. The prevalence of dementia: a quantitative inte-
gration of the literature. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1987;76:465–79.
[41] Galasko D, Hansen LA, Katzman R, et al. Clinico-neuropathological correlations in
Alzheimer’s disease and related dementia. Arch Neurol 1994;51:888–95.
[42] Lim A, Tsuang D, Kukull W, et al. Clinico-neuropathological correlation of Alzheimer’s
disease in a community-based case series. J Am Geriatr Soc 1999;47:564–9.
[43] Snowdon DA, Greiner LH, Mortimer JA, et al. Brain infarction and the clinical
expression of Alzheimer’s disease. The Nun Study. JAMA 1997;277:813–7.
[44] Hénon H, Pasquier F, Durieu I, et al. Pre-existing dementia in stroke patients: baseline
frequency, associated factors and outcome. Stroke 1997;28:2429–36.
[45] Hénon H, Durieu I, Guerouaou D, et al. Post-stroke dementia: incidence and relationship
to pre-stroke cognitive decline. Neurology 2001;57:1216–22.
[46] Hofman A, Rocca WA, Brayne C, et al. The prevalence of dementia in Europe: a
collaborative study of 1980–1990 findings. Int J Epidemiol 1991;20:736–48.
[47] Petrovitch H, White LR, Ross GW, et al. Accuracy of clinical criteria for AD in the
Honolulu-Asia Aging Study, a population-based study. Neurology 2001;57:226–34.
G.C. Román / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 477–499 495
[48] Jorm AF, Jolley D. The incidence of dementia. A meta-analysis. Neurology 1998;51:
728–33.
[49] Dubois MF, Herbert R. The incidence of vascular dementia in Canada: a comparison with
Europe and East Asia. Neuroepidemiology 2001;20:179–87.
[50] Pohjasvaara T, Erkinjuntti T, Vataja R, Kaste M. Dementia three months after stroke.
Baseline frequency and effect of different definitions of dementia in the Helsinki Stroke
Aging Memory Study (SAM) cohort. Stroke 1997;28:785–92.
[51] Tatemichi TK, Desmond DW, Stern Y, et al. After stroke: Frequency, patterns, and
relationship to functional abilities. Neurosurg Psychiatry 1994;57:202–7.
[52] Desmond DW, Moroney JT, Bagiella E, et al. Dementia as a predictor of adverse
outcomes following stroke. An evaluation of diagnostic methods. Stroke 1998;29:69–74.
[53] Tatemichi TK, Desmont DW, Paik M, et al. Clinical determinants of dementia related to
stroke. Ann Neurol 1993;33:568–75.
[54] Leys D, Pasquier F. How can cerebral infarcts and hemorrhages lead to dementia?
J Neural Transm 2000;59(Suppl):S31–6.
[55] Leys D, Erkinjuntti T, Desmond DW, et al. Vascular dementia: the role of cerebral
infarcts. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 1999;13(Suppl 3):S38–48.
[56] Liu CK, Miller BL, Cummings JL, et al. A quantitative MRI study of vascular dementia.
Neurology 1991;42:138–43.
[57] Gorelick PB, Chatterjee A, Patel D, et al. Cranial computed tomographic observations in
multi-infarct dementia: a controlled study. Stroke 1992;23:804–11.
[58] Moroney JT, Bagiella E, Desmond DW, et al. Risk factors for incident dementia after
stroke. Role of hypoxic ischemic disorders. Stroke 1996;27:1283–9.
[59] Moody DM, Brown WR, Challa VR, et al. Cerebral microvascular alterations in aging,
leukoaraiosis, and Alzheimer’s disease. Ann NY Acad Sci 1997;826:103–16.
[60] Román GC. From UBOs to Binswanger’s disease. Impact of MRI on VaD research.
Stroke 1996;27:1269–73.
[61] Longstreth WT Jr, Manolio TA, Arnold A, et al. Clinical correlates of white matter
findings on cranial magnetic resonance imaging of 3301 elderly people. The Cardiovas-
cular Health Study. Stroke 1996;27:1274–82.
[62] Román GC. Vascular dementia today. Rev Neurol (Paris) 1999;155(Suppl 4):S64–72.
[63] Ott B, Saver J. Unilateral amnesic stroke: six new cases and a review of the literature.
Brain 1993;24:1033–42.
[64] Alexander M, Freeman M. Amnesia after anterior communicating artery aneurysm
rupture. Neurology 1984;34:752–7.
[65] Benson D, Cummings J, Tsai S. Angular gyrus syndrome simulating Alzheimer’s disease.
Arch Neurol 1982;39:616–20.
[66] Castaigne P, Buge A, Cambier J, et al. Démence thalamique d’origine vasculaire par
ramollissement bilatéral, limité au territoire du péduncule retromamillaire. A propos de
deux observations anatomo-cliniques. Rev Neurol (Paris) 1966;114:89–107.
[67] Bogousslavsky J. Thalamic infarcts. In: Donnan G, Norrving B, Bamfortd J, Bogous-
slavsky J, editors. Lacunar and other subcortical infarctions. Oxford: Oxford University
Press; 2000. p. 149–70.
[68] Caplan LR, Schmahmann JD, Kase CS, et al. Caudate infarcts. Arch Neurol 1990;47:
133–43.
[69] Tatemichi TK, Desmont DW, Prohovnik I. Confusion and memory loss from cap-
sular genu infarction: a thalamocortical disconnection syndrome? Neurology 1992;42:
1966–79.
[70] Tatemichi TK, Desmont DW, Prohovnik I. Strategic infarcts in vascular dementia. A
clinical and imaging experience. Arzneimittelforschung 1995;45:371–85.
[71] Chukwudelunzu FE, Meschia JF, Graff-Radford NR, Lucas JA. Extensive metabolic and
neuropsychological abnormalities associated with discrete infarction of the genu of the
internal capsule. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2001;71:658–62.
496 G.C. Román / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 477–499
[72] Uggetti C, Egitto MG, Pichiecchio A, et al. Subcortical dementia associated with strik-
ing enlargement of the Virchow-Robin spaces and transneural degeneration of the left
mammillo-thalamic tract. Cerebrovasc Dis 2001;12:287–90.
[73] Erkinjuntti T, Inzitari D, Pantoni L, et al. Research criteria for subcortical vascular
dementia in clinical trials. J Neural Transm 2000;59(Suppl):S23–30.
[74] Marie P. Des foyers lacunaires de désintégration et de différents autres états cavitaires du
cerveau. Revue de Médecine (Paris) 1901;21:281–98.
[75] Hachinski V, Potter P, Merskey H. Leukoaraiosis: an ancient term for a new problem.
Arch Neurol 1987;44:21–3.
[76] Binswanger O. Die Abgrenzung der allgemeinen progressiven Paralyse (Referat, erstattet
auf der Jahres versammlung des Vereins Deutscher Irrenärtzte zu Dresden am 20 Sept.
1894). Berl Klin Wochenschr 1894;31:1103–5, 1137–9, 1180–6.
[77] Román GC. The identity of lacunar dementia and Binswanger disease. Med Hypotheses
1985;16:389–91.
[78] Fredriksson K, Brun A, Gustafson L. Pure subcortical arteriosclerotic encephalopathy
(Binswanger’s disease): a clinicopathological study. Part I: clinical features. Cerebrovasc
Dis 1992;2:82–6.
[79] Brun A, Fredriksson K, Gustafson L. Pure subcortical arteriosclerotic encephalopathy
(Binswanger’s disease): a clinicopathological study. Part II: pathologic features. Cere-
brovasc Dis 1992;2:626–33.
[80] Caplan LR. Binswanger’s disease—revisited. Neurology 1995;45:626–33.
[81] Román GC. Lacunar dementia. In: Hutton JT, Kenny AD, editors. Senile dementia of
the Alzheimer type. New York: Alan R. Liss; 2000. p. 131–51.
[82] Tournier-Lasserve E, Iba-Zizen MT, Romero N, Bousser M-G. Autosomal dominant
syndrome with stroke-like episodes and leukoencephalopathy. Stroke 1991;22:1297–302.
[83] Tournier-Lasserve E, Joutel A, Melki J, et al. Cerebral autosomal dominant arteriopathy
with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy maps to chromosome 19q12. Nat
Genet 1993;3:256–9.
[84] Bousser M-G, Tournier-Lasserve E. Summary of the Proceedings of the First Interna-
tional Workshop on CADASIL. Paris, May 19–21, 1993. Stroke 1994;25:704–7.
[85] Van Bogaert L. Encéphalopathie sous-corticale progressive (Binswanger) àévolution
rapide chez deux soeurs. Med Hellen 1955;24:961–72.
[86] Desmond DW, Moroney JT, Lynch T, et al. CADASIL in a North American family.
Clinical, pathologic, and radiologic findings. Neurology 1998;51:844–9.
[87] Dichgans M, Mayer M, Uttner I, et al. The phenotypic spectrum of CADASIL: clinical
findings in 102 cases. Ann Neurol 1998;44:731–9.
[88] Chabriat H, Bousser M-G, Pappata S. Cerebral autosomal dominant arteriopathy with
subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy: a positron emission tomography study in
two affected family members. Stroke 1995;26:1729–30.
[89] Ruchoux MM, Guerouaou D, Vandenhaute B, et al. Systemic vascular smooth muscle
impairment in cerebral autosomal dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and
leukoencephalopathy. Acta Neuropathol (Berl) 1995;89:500–12.
[90] Joutel A, Favrole P, Labauge P, et al. Skin biopsy immunostaining with a Notch3
monoclonal antibody for CADASIL diagnosis. Lancet 2001;358:2049–51.
[91] Joutel A, Vahedi K, Corpechor C, et al. Strong clustering and stereotyped nature of
mutations in CADASIL patients. Lancet 1997;350:1511–5.
[92] Mead S, James-Galton M, Revesz T, et al. Familial British dementia with amyloid
angiopathy: early clinical, neuropsychological and imaging findings. Brain 2000;123:975–91.
[93] Ghiso JA, Holton J, Miravalle L, et al. Systemic amyloid deposits in familial British
dementia. J Biol Chem 2001;276:43909–14.
[94] Román GC, Tatemichi TK, Erkinjuntti T, et al. Vascular dementia: diagnostic criteria for
research studies. Report of the NINDS-AIREN International Workshop. Neurology
1993;43:250–60.
G.C. Román / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 477–499 497
[95] Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PH. ‘‘Mini-mental state.’’ A practical method for
grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975;12:189–98.
[96] Royall DR, Mulroy AR, Chiodo LK, Polk MJ. Clock drawing is sensitive to executive
control: a comparison of six methods. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 1999;54:328–33.
[97] Reitan RM, Davidson LA. Clinical neuropsychology: current status and applications.
Washington, DC: Hemisphere; 1974.
[98] Grigsby J, Kaye K, Robbins LJ. Reliabilities, norms and factor structure of the
Behavioral Dyscontrol Scale. Percept Mot Skills 1992;74:883–92.
[99] Mungas D, Jagust WJ, Reed BR, et al. MRI predictors of cognition in subcortical
ischemic vascular disease and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 2001;57:2229–35.
[100] Vinters HV, Ellis WG, Zarow C, et al. Neuropathologic substrates of ischemic vascular
dementia. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 2000;59:931–45.
[101] Moroney JT, Bagiella E, Desmond DW, et al. Meta-analysis of the Hachinski ischemic
score in pathologically verified dementias. Neurology 1997;49:1096–105.
[102] Petersen RC, Doody R, Kurz A, et al. Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment.
Arch Neuro 2001;58:1985–92.
[103] Jagust W. Untangling vascular dementia. Lancet 2001;358:2097–8.
[104] Skoog I. Status of risk factors for vascular dementia. Neuroepidemiology 1998;17:2–9.
[105] Hofman A, Ott A, Breteler MM, et al. Atherosclerosis, apolipoprotein E, and prevalence
of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease in the Rotterdam study. Lancet 1997;349:151–4.
[106] Haan M, Shemanski L, Jagust W, et al. Predictors of cognitive change in the Cardio-
vascular Health Study: the role of cardiovascular and genetic risk factors. JAMA 1999;282:
40–6.
[107] Gueyffier F, Boissel J-P, Boutitie F, et al. Effect of antihypertensive treatment in patients
having already suffered from stroke: gathering the evidence. The INDANA (INdividual
Data ANalysis of Antihypertensive intervention trials) Project Collaborators. Stroke
1997;28:2557–62.
[108] Blauw GJ, Lagaay AM, Smelt AHM, et al. Stroke, statins and cholesterol: a meta-analysis
of randomized, placebo-controlled, double-blind trials with HMG-CoA reductase inhi-
bitors. Stroke 1997;28:946–50.
[109] Majid A, Delanty N, Kantor J. Antiplatelet agents for secondary prevention of ischemic
stroke. Ann Pharmacother 2001;35:1241–7.
[110] Hart RG, Sherman DG, Easton D, Cairns JA. Prevention of stroke in patients with
nonvalvular atrial fibrillation. Neurology 1998;51:674–81.
[111] Barnett HJM, Taylor DW, Eliasziw M, et al. Benefit of carotid endarterectomy in patients
with symptomatic moderate or severe stenosis. N Engl J Med 1998;339:1415–25.
[112] Skoog I, Lernfelt B, Landahl S, et al. 15-year longitudinal study of blood pressure and
dementia. Lancet 1996;347:1141–5.
[113] Forette F, Seux ML, Staessen JA, et al. Prevention of dementia in randomised double-
blind placebo-controlled Systolic Hypertension in Europe (Syst-Eur) trial. Lancet 1998;352:
1347–51.
[114] Rigaud AS, Hanon O, Seux ML, Forette F. Hypertension and dementia. Curr Hypertens
Rep 2001;3:454–7.
[115] Birkenhager WH, Forette F, Seux ML, et al. Blood pressure, cognitive functions, and
prevention of dementias in older patients with hypertension. Arch Intern Med 2001;161:
152–6.
[116] Jick H, Zornberg GL, Jick SS, Seshadri S, et al. Statins and the risk of dementia. Lancet
2000;356:1627–31.
[117] Fassbender K, Simons M, Bergmann C, et al. Simvastatin strongly reduces levels of
Alzheimer’s disease beta-amyloid peptides Abeta 42 and Abeta 40 in vitro and in vivo.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2001;98:5856–61.
[118] Delanty N, Vaughan CJ, Sheehy N. Statins and neuroprotection. Expert Opinion on
Investigational Drugs 2001;10:1847–53.
498 G.C. Román / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 477–499
[119] Simons M, Keller P, Dichgans J, Schulz JB. Cholesterol and Alzheimer’s disease: is there
a link? Neurology 2001;57:1089–93.
[120] Cacciatore F, Abete P, Ferrara N, et al. Congestive heart failure and cognitive impairment
in an older population. J Am Geriatr Soc 1998;46:1343–8.
[121] Román GC. New insight into Binswanger disease. Arch Neurol 1999;56:1061–2.
[122] Flemming KD, Nguyen TT, Abu-Lebdeh HS, et al. Hyperhomocysteinemia in patients
with cerebral autosomal dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoence-
phalopathy (CADASIL). Mayo Clin Proc 2001;76:1213–8.
[123] Román GC. Therapeutic strategies for vascular dementia. In: O’Brien J, Ames D,
Burns A, editors. Dementia. 2nd edition. London: Arnold/Oxford University Press; 2000.
p. 667–81.
[124] Cochrane AL. Concluding remarks. In: Tognoni G, Garattini S, editors. Treatment and
prevention of cerebrovascular disorders. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1979. p. 453–5.
[125] Román GC. Perspectives in the treatment of vascular dementia. Drugs Today 2000;36:
641–53.
[126] Pantoni L, Rossi R, Inzitari D, et al. Efficacy and safety of nimodipine in subcortical
vascular dementia: a subgroup analysis of the Scandinavian Multi-infarct Dementia Trial.
J Neurol Sci 2000;175:124–34.
[127] Spanish Nicardipine Study in Vascular Dementia Group. An experimental, randomized,
double-blind, placebo-controlled clinical trial to investigate the effect of nicardipine on
cognitive function in patients with vascular dementia. Rev Neurol (Barcelona) 1999;28:
835–45.
[128] Flicker L, Grimley Evans G. Piracetam for dementia or cognitive impairment (Cochrane
Review). Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2001;2:CD001011.
[129] Bottini G, Vallar G, Cappa S, et al. Oxiracetam in dementia: a double-blind, placebo-
controlled study. Acta Neurol Scand 1992;86:237–41.
[130] Herrmann WM, Stephan K, Gaede K, et al. A multicenter randomized double-blind study
on the efficacy and safety of nicergoline in patients with multi-infarct dementia. Dement
Geriatr Cogn Disord 1997;8:9–17.
[131] Bes A, Orgogozo JM, Poncet M, et al. A 24-month, double-blind, placebo-controlled
multicentre pilot study of the efficacy and safety of nicergoline 60 mg per day in elderly
hypertensive patients with leukoaraiosis. Eur J Neurol 1999;6:313–22.
[132] Cacabelos R, Alvarez XA, Franco-Maside A, et al. Effect of CDP-choline on cognition
and immune function in Alzheimer’s disease and multi-infarct dementia. Ann NY Acad
Sci 1993;695:321–3.
[133] Fioravanti M, Yanagi M. Cytidinediphosphocholine (CDP choline) for cognitive and
behavioural disturbances associated with chronic cerebral disorders in the elderly. Coch-
rane Database Syst Rev 2000;4:CD000269.
[134] Secades JJ, Frontera G. CDP-choline: pharmacological and clinical review. Methods Find
Exp Clin Pharmacol 1995;17(Suppl B):S1–54.
[135] Mobius HJ. Pharmacologic rationale for memantine in chronic cerebral hypoperfusion,
especially vascular dementia. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 1999;13(Suppl 3):S172–8.
[136] Winblad B, Poritis N. Memantine in severe dementia: results of the 9M-Best Study
(benefit and efficacy in severely demented patients during treatment with memantine). Int
J Geriatr Psychiatry 1999;14:135–46.
[137] Mobius HJ, Stoffler A. New approaches to clinical trials in vascular dementia: memantine
in small vessel disease. Cerebrovasc Dis 2002;13(Suppl 2):61–6.
[138] Samlaska CP, Winfield EA. Clinical review: pentoxifylline. J Am Acad Dermatol 1994;30:
603–21.
[139] Europen Pentoxifylline Multi-Infarct Dementia Study Group. Pentoxifylline study. Study
EPM-ID. Eur Neurol 1996;36:315–21.
[140] Black RS, Barclay LL, Nolan KA, et al. Pentoxifylline in cerebrovascular dementia. J Am
Geriatr Soc 1992;40:237–44.
G.C. Román / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 477–499 499
[141] Solerte SB, Ceresini G, Ferrari E, Fioravanti M. Hemorheological changes and over-
production of cytokines from immune cells in mild to moderate dementia of the
Alzheimer’s type: adverse effects on cerebromicrovascular system. Neurobiol Aging 2000;
21:271–81.
[142] Williams PS, Spector A, Orrell M, Rands G. Aspirin for vascular dementia. Cochrane
Database Syst Rev 2000;2:CD001296.
[143] Lopez-Pousa S, Mercadal-Dalmau J, Marti-Cuadros AM, et al. Triflusal in the prevention
of vascular dementia [in French]. Rev Neurol (Barcelona) 1997;25146:1525–8.
[144] Oken BS, Storzbach DM, Kaye JA. The efficacy of Ginkgo biloba on cognitive function in
Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 1998;55:1409–15.
[145] Gorelick PB, Nyenhuis DL, Garron DC, Cochran E. Is vascular dementia really
Alzheimer’s disease or mixed dementia? Neuroepidemiology 1996;15:286–90.
[146] Aguero-Torres H, Winblad B. Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia. Some points of
confluence. Ann NY Acad Sci 2000;903:547–52.
[147] Kalaria RN, Ballard C. Overlap between pathology of Alzheimer disease and vascular
dementia. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 1999;13(Suppl 3):S115–23.
[148] Sarangi S, San Pedro EC, Mountz JM. Anterior choroidal artery infarction presenting as
a progressive cognitive deficit. Clin Nucl Med 2000;25:187–90.
[149] Wallin A, Alafuzoff I, Carlsson A, et al. Neurotransmitter deficits in a non-multi-infarct
category of vascular dementia. Acta Neurol Scand 1989;79:397–406.
[150] Tohgi H, Abe T, Kimura M, et al. Cerebrospinal fluid acetylcholine and choline in
vascular dementia of Binswanger and multiple small infarct types as compared with
Alzheimer-type dementia. J Neural Transm 1996;103:1211–20.
[151] Martin-Ruiz C, Court J, Lee M, et al. Nicotinic receptors in dementia of Alzheimer, Lewy
body and vascular types. Acta Neurol Scand 2000;176(Suppl):S34–41.
[152] Pratt RD, Perdomo CA. Results of clinical studies with donepezil in vascular dementia.
Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2002;10(Suppl 1):88–9.
[153] Kumar V, Anand R, Messina J, et al. An efficacy and safety analysis of Exelon in
Alzheimer’s disease patients with concurrent vascular risk factors. Eur J Neurol 2000;7:
159–69.
[154] Moretti R, Torre P, Antonello RM, Cazzato G. Rivastigmine in subcortical vascular
dementia: a comparison trial on efficacy and tolerability for 12 months follow-up. Eur J
Neurol 2001;8:361–2.
[155] Erkinjuntti T, Kurz A, Gauthier S, Bullock R, Lilienfeld S, Damarajn CR. Efficacy of
galantamine in probable vascular dementia and Alzheimer’s disease combined with
cerebrovascular disease: a randomized trial. Lancet 2002;359:1283–90.
Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 501–518
Frontotemporal dementia
Andrew Kertesz, MD, FRCPCa,*,
David G. Munoz, MD, FRCPCb,c
a
Department of Clinical Neurological Sciences, St. Joseph’s Hospital,
University of Western Ontario, 268 Grosvenor Street, London,
Ontario N6A 4V2, Canada
b
Department of Pathology and Clinical Neurological Sciences,
University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada
c
Banco de Tejidos para Investigacion Neurológica, Universidad Autonoma, Madrid, Spain
* Corresponding author.
E-mail address: [email protected] (A. Kertesz).
0025-7125/02/$ - see front matter Ó 2002, Elsevier Science (USA). All rights reserved.
PII: S 0 0 2 5 - 7 1 2 5 ( 0 2 ) 0 0 0 1 1 - 1
502 A. Kertesz, D.G. Munoz / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 501–518
Pick’s disease
Arnold Pick [14] described behavioral and aphasic symptoms associated
with frontotemporal atrophy more than 100 years ago. Several of his subse-
quent articles dealt with progressive aphasia and progressive apraxia on the
basis of focal atrophy. Pick’s initial cases only had gross examination with-
out any microscopic data, but the clinical pattern and its relation to focal
atrophy are the basis of the syndrome. Gans [15] suggested the eponymic
term and considered a predilection for the phylogenetically younger frontal
and temporal lobes in the etiology. Subsequently, PiD was defined on the
basis of histology as initially described by Alzheimer [16]. Onari and Spatz
[17] re-examined the cases of Pick and cases from others emphasizing Pick
bodies and Pick cells (large ballooned neurons). Later, it became apparent
that cases of clinical PiD with frontal lobe and temporal lobe symptomatol-
ogy often do not show this typical histologic picture on autopsy [18–20].
Most series of PiD were based on postmortem examination, and the clinical
features were often incompletely described because of the retrospective na-
ture of these studies. A dichotomy of nosology arose, because some people
use the term Pick’s disease on the basis of histologic criteria, whereas others
describe the clinical picture of focal atrophies as Pick did originally. This
gave rise to the notion that PiD is difficult to diagnose in vivo.
Many of the clinical descriptions had dramatic frontal lobe deficits.
Schneider [21,22] described several stages of PiD: first, a ‘‘disturbance of
judgement and asocial behaviour,’’ followed by aphasia and, later, by more
generalized dementia. The temporal lobe variety of PiD presenting with pro-
gressive aphasia was described quite early [14,18,23–26], and these descrip-
tions are similar to those of PPA appearing later. Caron [27], in his review of
PiD, stated that the most common form is characterized by early develop-
ment of aphasia, and others also emphasized early speech disturbance or
aphasia in PiD [28–30].
A. Kertesz, D.G. Munoz / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 501–518 503
There have been several case descriptions of PiD in which the patients
had prominent extrapyramidal features [19,24,31,32]. Ferraro and Jervis
[33] stated that extrapyramidal symptoms were common in PiD. Sometimes,
unilateral rigidity and parkinsonism were the first symptoms to attract
attention. It was recognized that subcortical changes occur in PiD even
without extrapyramidal symptomatology [19]. Constantinidis et al [20]
described extrapyramidal involvement particularly in ‘‘group B’’ patients,
and Mann et al [34] described extrapyramidal involvement in 8 of 12 of FLD
patients. Changes in the basal ganglia, especially in the striatum and sub-
stantia nigra in addition to cortical pathologic changes, occurred in most
of 30 cases in one review [35]. Many of the subcortical varieties of PiD are
similar to corticonigral and CBD clinically and pathologically.
Munoz-Garcia and Ludwin [36] differentiated the ‘‘generalized’’ variety
of PiD from the cortical variety because of the subcortical extent of the
pathologic findings. The clinical description of these patients is quite similar
to the description of the cortical variety of PiD. None of these patients had
prominent extrapyramidal symptoms, but some had features of the Klüver-
Bucy syndrome [37]. The relatively early age of patients has been observed
by authors describing the subcortical or generalized varieties of PiD
[19,36,38]. PiD generally has a presenile onset below the age of 65 years
in contrast to the age at onset of most AD patients. In addition, familial
cases tend to have an even earlier onset.
It was recognized that the disease sometimes occurred in families [39].
More than half of autopsy-diagnosed cases of PiD were familial [40]. A large
family with PiD, where 25 of 51 examined members were affected [41] with a
mostly behavioral presentation, was subsequently found to have a genetic
linkage to chromosome 17 [42]. Several other families were described with
various tau mutations with classic PiD pathologic findings [43–46].
There were larger series of PiD described in the literature in the early 1950s
from Europe [47–50]. Frontotemporal atrophy was the most common form
of the disease (54%). Frontal atrophy only occurred in 25% of cases, and tem-
poral atrophy only occurred in 17% of cases. The brunt of the atrophy was
most often in the medial orbital, the inferior frontal gyri, and the anterior
third of the superior temporal gyrus. More than half of the cases showed
atrophy on the left side more than on the right side, and in 20% of the cases,
right frontal atrophy was more prominent than left frontal atrophy. Con-
stantinidis et al [20] classified PiD as (1) with Pick bodies, (2) only with
swollen neurons, and (3) only gliosis. They believed that ‘‘in spite of the dis-
similarities between these forms, considering the absence of sufficient knowl-
edge about pathogenesis, it seems prudent at present to maintain the
uniqueness of Pick’s entity’’ [20]. They thought the clinical differences
between these forms were not related to the nature of histologic alterations
but rather to the temporal lobe or frontal predominance of the abnormality.
With the development of neuroimaging, frontotemporal lobe atrophy
was demonstrated with increasing frequency in vivo, first, with air studies;
504 A. Kertesz, D.G. Munoz / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 501–518
then, with CT scans in the 1970s; and more recently, with MRI and single
photon emission CT scans. The in vivo diagnosis of PiD continued to be
made sporadically on the basis of frontal and temporal symptomatology
supported by the focal atrophy on imaging and a normal electroencephalo-
gram [51]. Instead of shifting the diagnosis of PiD back to the clinic, how-
ever, the in vivo studies applied new labels, such as FLD, FTD, and PPA, to
clinical PiD, while reserving the diagnosis of PiD to increasingly restricted
histologic criteria.
The pathologic definition of PiD, as defined by pathologists, is character-
ized as round argyrophilic inclusions in the neuronal cytoplasm called Pick
bodies. They are best demonstrated with traditional silver stains, such as
Bodian or Bielchowsky stain, but do not stain with the Gallyas method
[11]. There is variable labeling with tau antibodies, ubiquitin, and chromo-
granin A. Electron microscopy has revealed bodies made up of 15-nm
straight fibrils [36,52] and long-period twisted fibrils [53]. Neocortical Pick
bodies are preferentially located in small neurons, and they are pathognomo-
nic in the dentate gyrus [53], whereas traditional silver stains and tau anti-
bodies identify scattered Pick bodies in the neocortex in CBD as well [54].
Large ballooned neurons (Pick cells), superficial cortical spongiosis neuro-
nal loss, and gliosis occur in the atrophied areas, and these features are com-
mon in CBD, DLDH, and other forms of the Pick complex. Complement
proteins and complement inhibitors are detected in the neuronal cytoplasm,
suggesting that complement activation is interrupted before reaching comple-
tion and causing neuronal lysis [55]. Additionally, Pick body–bearing neurons
(but not Pick cells) are surrounded by activated microglial cells and T lympho-
cytes [56].
Progressive subcortical gliosis [38,57] is clinically similar to PiD but, so
far, only remains a pathologic diagnosis. More than 30 cases have been
described in the literature, with the largest series being those of Verity and
Wechsler [58] and Bergeron et al [59]. Because white matter gliosis is com-
mon in other varieties of Pick complex pathologic entities, the separation
of this variation is hardly justified [11].
Frontotemporal dementia
In the second half of the 1980s, two European groups described FLD as a
distinct entity and contrasted the clinical features with those of AD [1–3,5].
They estimated its relative incidence to be 15% to 20% of degenerative
dementias. Both groups recognized that even though some of the cases had
Pick bodies and most did not, the clinical syndrome was the same. They
called the pathologic entity without Pick bodies ‘‘frontal lobe dementia
type,’’ which consisted of neuronal loss and gliosis in the frontal cortex with
or without spongiform changes or ballooned neurons [5]. At the same time,
Knopman et al [6] described a similar clinicopathologic picture as DLDH.
A. Kertesz, D.G. Munoz / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 501–518 505
The groups that described dementia of the frontal lobe type changed the
terminology to FTD [4]. The term frontotemporal degeneration or frontotem-
poral dementia [60] does not include the frequent subcortical involvement,
parietal pathologic findings, or extrapyramidal symptomatology. Further-
more, it does not distinguish between the clear-cut behavioral presentation
of FTD and aphasic presentation of PPA, which is one of the most valuable
contributions of the recent descriptions of the clinical picture in these con-
ditions. It does reflect, however, the frequent combination of frontal and
anterior temporal atrophy originally described by Pick.
The striking alterations in personality and behavior have become synon-
ymous with FTD. The hallmark of this distinctive form of dementia is the
combination of disinhibition with apathy, although at different stages of
the disease, one or the other symptom may be predominant. Disinhibition
often refers to social inappropriateness ranging from childish rude behavior
to exposure and kleptomania. It overlaps with impulsivity, poor judgment,
irresponsibility, and irritability. Other manifestations, such as restlessness,
pacing, wandering, and aggression, are also seen.
One of the characteristic groups of behaviors is characterized by persevera-
tion of words, gestures, and actions as well as obsessive stereotypic routines.
These patients are inflexible, insisting on eating, buying, or doing the same
thing at the same time; hoarding objects, clothes, or even garbage; perseverat-
ing with stories; and telephoning people inappropriately. Patients may be pre-
occupied with germs, pills, money, sex, types of clothing, or clock watching
while they carry out their stereotypic routines. If attempts are made to restrain
these behaviors, anger and violent resistance often occur. In more advanced
stages, echolalia, incessant clapping, singing, and laughing are observed.
Other disinhibition phenomena, such as hypersexuality, hyperorality, and
utilization behavior, are sometimes grouped under the term Klüver-Bucy
syndrome, which originated from the observation of monkeys with bilateral
temporal lobectomy [37]. The hypersexuality may be only verbal and gestural
in middle-aged or older individuals. Hyperorality often manifests in gluttony
and overeating, and many patients develop food fads, particularly for
sweets. Patients may consume large quantities of candy or cookies in one sit-
ting. We had patients who would eat in the same chicken restaurant day in
and out. Others insisted on a diet of milk and bananas or had plum sauce
with everything. Some patients grab food off the serving plate before any-
body else starts or even take food off the plates of others. The compulsive eat-
ing and drinking sometimes extends to inedible objects as in coprophagia.
Utilization behavior is the need to touch, feel, examine, or pick up objects
within reach and sight, similar to what others called ‘‘environmental depen-
dence’’ or ‘‘hypermetamorphosis’’ [61]. The final stages of disinhibition mani-
fest in urinary and stool incontinence, although this may occur early in FTD
patients who are still oriented.
There is a group of behaviors that can be characterized as ‘‘negative,’’
consisting of apathy, aspontaneity, indifference, and emotional flatness.
506 A. Kertesz, D.G. Munoz / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 501–518
Patients are unable to recognize the extent of their own behavioral distur-
bance and most often insist there is nothing wrong. A striking disinterest
in family matters or in the plight of others is sometimes the presenting fea-
ture. More apparent extremes of indifference and apathy culminate in perso-
nal neglect, for example, not washing or changing underwear. Another
negative behavior is the lack of ability to plan complex activity or pay atten-
tion in a sustained manner so as to complete a task, which is often called the
‘‘dysexecutive syndrome.’’ Decreasing language and communication is also
part of this negative cluster of symptoms, which may eventually resemble
progressive aphasia resulting in mutism.
Lately, several anatomic correlations with the behavioral variants of
FTD have been attempted. Snowden et al [62] distinguished the ‘‘disinhib-
ited overactive’’ variety of FTD. These patients are impulsive and distracti-
ble. Their social and personal disinhibitions are embarrassing to families.
The pathologic changes tend to be in the orbital surface of the frontal lobes
and the temporal neocortex. The ‘‘apathetic inert subtype’’ may be seen at
presentation or subsequent to the disinhibited behavior. These patients have
pathologic changes in the dorsal lateral convexity as well as in the temporal
neocortex. The ‘‘stereotypic rigid subtype’’ of FTD is associated with perse-
verative and ritualistic behavior and tends to be associated with akinesia and
rigidity early in the disease. Pathologic changes are considered to involve the
basal ganglia and temporal neocortex, with relative sparing of the frontal
lobes. Others emphasize the ‘‘loss of self,’’ a serial criminal behavior, and
obsessive copying or coloring behavior with right temporal atrophy [63].
Progressive aphasia was also renamed the left temporal variety of FTD.
In our experience, these variants tend to overlap a great deal, and their
separation may not be clinically or pathologically feasible.
Patients may perform surprisingly well on neuropsychologic tests,
although impulsiveness, distractibility, and lack of cooperation tend to inter-
fere with testing rather early in the disease. Sometimes, only the results of
complex executive tests of shifting sets, such as the Wisconsin Card Sorting
Test, interference (the Stroop Test), or planning (the Tower of Hanoi), are
abnormal. There are patients, however, whose behavior is grossly abnormal,
yet they can perform well on the traditional frontal lobe tests. A behavioral
inventory, such as that designed in our clinic, seems to be more helpful in the
clinical diagnosis and even in the quantitation of severity of the illness [64].
Similar behavioral inventories for FTD have been used at other centers
[65,66]. The more general Neuropsychiatric Inventory has also been used
to differentiate AD from FTD but has less specific items for FTD [67].
The pathologic profile of FTD was originally described as a separate
entity, consisting of focal atrophy with superficial cortical spongiosis, glio-
sis, and neuronal loss, but this is generic to all the varieties of Pick complex
pathologic findings [11]. Several families with FTD have been described with
linkage to chromosome 17 [68]. The combination of phenotypes in these
families suggests that the various clinical manifestations in the much more
A. Kertesz, D.G. Munoz / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 501–518 507
common sporadic illness are also related. Some of these families have tau-
positive pathologic findings and tau mutation, but tau-negative families with
motor neuron type inclusions are also described [69].
An association of FTD with motor neuron disease (MND) has been
increasingly recognized [70,71]. Ubiquitin-positive and tau-negative inclu-
sion bodies in the dentate gyrus and nonmotor cortex were described as a
marker of this MND type of FTD [72,73]. Strong et al [74] reviewed the neu-
ropsychologic deficit in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, which tends to be the
frontal type. When MND supervenes, however, the rapid course precludes
development of full-blown FTD.
Corticobasal degeneration
When Rebeiz et al [88] described corticodentatonigral degeneration, they
considered it to be a distinct entity characterized by an akinetic extrapyra-
midal syndrome, apraxia, ‘‘alien hand,’’ and vertical gaze palsy, but they
recognized the similarity of the pathologic findings to those of PiD (alien
hand refers to one hand interfering with the other among other phenomena).
The syndrome was ignored for 20 years and then resurrected using the term
corticobasal degeneration or corticobasal ganglionic degeneration [89,90].
Most of the literature concerning this condition acknowledges the clinical
and pathologic overlap between CBD and PiD [91–93].
In addition to the ballooned neurons (Pick cells) and superficial layer
(cortical spongiosis), CBD has certain distinctive features. The cortical neu-
ronal inclusions are positive by Gallyas stain, and the hippocampus is usual-
ly spared. Most of the pathologic changes are subcortical, and the inclusions
often have a ring or kidney shape and are less homogenous than in Pick
bodies; however, without the Gallyas stain, they may be difficult to distin-
guish. The abundance of tau-positive oligodendroglial inclusions and astro-
cytic plaques is characteristic.
CBD patients suffer from a dichotomy similar to that of PiD patients in
that the pathologic and clinical descriptions do not fully overlap. There are
A. Kertesz, D.G. Munoz / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 501–518 509
some case reports describing patients who presented clinically with CBD as
defined by unilateral rigidity, apraxia, and alien hand syndrome but had the
pathologic findings of PiD with Pick bodies [31,94,95]. Other cases, patholo-
gically typical of CBD, have a frontal type of dementia without the present-
ing extrapyramidal features [91,96,97]. Typical CBD pathologic findings can
be seen with a clinical picture of PPA [9,78,98–100]. In a recent study, we
suggested that the clinical syndrome of prominent apraxia unilateral extra-
pyramidal syndrome with the alien hand phenomenon should be designated
as CBD syndrome, regardless of the pathologic findings. Further evidence of
the clinical and neuropathologic overlap between CBD, FTD, and PPA has
been accumulated. The evidence is overwhelming that CBD is also part of
the Pick complex [101].
There is also a significant overlap between CBD and progressive supra-
nuclear palsy (PSP) clinically and pathologically, suggesting that PSP may
be considered in the same biologic spectrum as CBD [102–105]. Typically,
unilateral presentation of extrapyramidal symptoms in combination with
apraxia leads to the diagnosis of CBD; axial dystonia, falls, and bilateral
rigidity are considered more typical of PSP [106]. Nevertheless, these dis-
tinctions are not mutually exclusive. The increasingly frequent recognition
of gaze palsy in CBD also contributes to the clinical similarities of these
entities.
complex [109], with a variable amount of discussion and reference to the clin-
ical and pathologic overlap. Table 1 summarizes the terms used to describe
similar conditions.
If one considers all the cases of FTD, PPA with and without MND, and
CBD as part of the Pick complex, the entity becomes much less of a rarity.
According to some estimates, including all pathologic variants, the incidence
of FTD may be as high as 20% of degenerative dementias [2,40,110], and
PPA reports may represent another 10%, even considering the substantial
overlap with FTD. The percentage is higher if only presenile cases are con-
sidered [108]. This number would be further increased by the inclusion of
CBD cases. We have now examined over 150 patients with Pick complex
in our clinic compared with 600 patients with probable AD, and we fre-
quently continue to see suspected cases. Approximately half of these
patients have FTD (behavioral), half have PPA (language) presentation, and
a smaller number (7%) start with the movement disorder of CBD. The num-
bers match or surpass those of patients with vascular dementia. The ratio of
Pick complex to AD may turn out to be 1:4 or approximately 25% of degen-
erative dementias rather than the estimates of PiD based on autopsy mate-
rial using restrictive histologic criteria. Admittedly, epidemiologic studies
are lacking, and a selection bias is playing a role in centers with an interest
in the disease. Nevertheless, a revision of the mistaken belief that the disease
is rare and difficult to diagnose should result in increased recognition; hope-
fully, successful treatment will follow.
Table 1
Glossary of Pick complex
1. Circumscribed cerebral atrophy
2. Pick’s disease (PiD)
3. Lobar atrophy
4. Progressive subcortical gliosis (PSG)
5. Corticodentatonigral degeneration
6. Generalized Pick’s disease
7. Frontal lobe dementia (FLD)
8. Primary progressive aphasia (PPA)
9. Corticobasal degeneration (CBD)
10. Dementia lacking distinctive histology (DLDH)
11. Semantic dementia
12. Frontal lobe dementia with motor neuron disease
13. Frontotemporal dementia (FTD)
14. Primary progressive apraxia
15. Nonspecific familial dementia
16. Atypical presenile dementia
17. Spongiform encephalopathy of long duration
18. Hereditary Dysphasic Dementia
19. Pallido-Ponto-Nigral Degeneration
20. Disinhibition-Dementia-Parkinsonism-Amyotrophy
A. Kertesz, D.G. Munoz / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 501–518 511
Neuropathologic findings
The underlying commonality of Pick complex neuropathologic findings is
the initially asymmetric focal atrophy of the frontotemporal regions. There
is also underlying neuronal loss, gliosis, and superficial linear spongiosis in
affected cortical areas. Ballooned neurons or Pick cells occur with variable
frequency in all varieties. They appear swollen pink on hematoxylin-eosin
staining, lack Nissl substance (neuronal achromasia) of the cytoplasm, and
express phosphorylated neurofilament episodes. There is tau reactivity in the
oligodendroglial cells and astrocytic processes. The superficial layer spon-
giosis is seen in layers II and III of the cortex in contrast to the spongiform
changes of Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease, which tend to be visible throughout.
Various distinctive features, such as Pick bodies, astrocytic plaques in CBD,
tufted astrocytes in PSP, and ubiquitin-positive and tau-negative inclusions
in MND type dementia, have been described, but they, in turn, can occur
with each of the other clinical varieties. Cases lacking any of these distinctive
features are often labeled ‘‘dementia lacking distinctive histology,’’ but we
have found ubiquitin-positive and tau-negative inclusions of the amyo-
trophic lateral sclerosis type in many of these cases previously considered
to represent DLDH [69]. There is substantial overlap between all varieties
of the Pick complex, although their distinctiveness is also argued [111]. As
mentioned previously, the clinical varieties do not predict the histologic vari-
ety, but they strongly predict the complex.
Biochemistry
Pathologic tau proteins (PTPs) are biochemical markers of various forms
of degenerative dementia, including AD, PiD, CBD, PSP, the parkinsonism-
dementia of Guam (Lytico-Bodig), and dementia pugilistica, which are col-
lectively called tauopathies. Tau mutations have been discovered only in
FTDP-17, however. Neurofibrillary tangles of AD contain all six human tau
isoforms. Abnormally phosphorylated PTPs form three bands on Western
blot studies with a molecular weight of 55, 64, and 69 kd using certain
immunologic probes in AD. Other monoclonal antibodies label other bands
indicating variously phosphorylated amino acids [112]. PiD has 64-kd and
55-kd doublets, and CBD and PSP have 69-kd and 64-kd doublets. The
amount of abnormal tau can be low in FTD, and PTP is sometimes absent.
At times, FTD has a tau triplet as in AD [112]. Sometimes, different band
compositions are obtained from different parts of the brain [113]. In some
studies, two bands were seen in brain stem neurons, and triplets were seen
in the hippocampal neurons of PSP.
Tau-negative ‘‘hereditary dysphasic disinhibition dementia’’ and some
sporadic cases of DLDH with prominent language and behavioral deficits
have been more recently attributed to the loss of tau in the brain, with the
same effect as the tauopathies with tau-positive pathologic findings [114].
512 A. Kertesz, D.G. Munoz / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 501–518
Genetics
Wilhelmsen et al [12] discovered a linkage to chromosome 17 q21–22 in a
large family with variable symptomatology of FTD, aphasia, parkinsonism,
and amyotrophy [109]. A consensus conference summarized the clinical fea-
tures of 12 families and the pathologic characteristics resembling those of
the sporadic cases. Although each family was described under different ter-
minology (see Table 1), the term frontotemporal dementia with parkinsonism
linked to chromosome 17 was accepted [68]. The microtubular-associated
protein tau was suspected as the candidate gene for mutation. A few years
later, several tau mutations were discovered [13,115,116]. Normal tau pro-
teins contribute to axonal transport by binding to microtubular protein. Six
tau isoforms are created by the differential splicing of exon 10, making three
or four repeats of the microtubular binding domain of tau.
To date, more than 20 tau mutations in more than 50 families have
been identified [117]. The exon 10 splice mutations alter the ratio of four-
repeat to three-repeat tau isoforms, most often resulting in pathologic find-
ings resembling those of CBD or PSP. The phenotypes range from FTD, to
PiD, to CBD, with the same mutation often resulting in a different clinical
presentation [43]. The missense mutations disrupt the interaction between
tau and microtubules, and unbound tau becomes abnormally phosphory-
lated and polymerized into filaments and inclusions. Mutations in exons
9, 12, and 13 lead to accumulation of all six isoforms of tau, resulting in
Alzheimer-type tangles, or to a predominance of three-repeat tau and Pick
body dementia. Although different tau mutations differentially alter bio-
chemical properties of tau isoforms [118], these mutations do not predict the
clinical presentations, but they do predict the overall clinical morphologic
picture resembling sporadic FTD or Pick complex.
Tau polymorphisms from the two main haplotypes of tau were also asso-
ciated with various phenotypes. The AO allele is overrepresented in both
PSP and CBD [119]. The allele is more common in AD with an interaction
with apolipoprotein E4; a similar interaction was found in FTD, but the
association of apolipoprotein E4 with FTD is controversial [120].
Treatment
There is evidence that cholinergic receptor binding is decreased in PiD in
affected cortical regions [121–123]. Serotonin and Imipramine binding were
decreased in the hypothalamus and frontal and temporal lobes associated
with PID [124]. The decreased serotonin binding could correlate with the
overeating and weight gain observed in some patients with PiD/FTD/Pick
complex. Other behavioral impairments, such as depression, irritability, and
apathy, with relative preservation of memory are also compatible with sero-
toninergic dysfunction [125]. Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors and
A. Kertesz, D.G. Munoz / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 501–518 513
Acknowledgment
The authors thank Bonita Stevenson for secretarial assistance.
References
[1] Gustafson L. Frontal lobe degeneration of non-Alzheimer type. II. Clinical picture and
differential diagnosis. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 1987;6:209–23.
[2] Neary D, Snowden JS, Northen B, et al. Dementia of frontal lobe type. J Neurol
Neurosurg Psychiatry 1988;51:353–61.
[3] Neary D, Snowden JS, Bowen JS, et al. Neuropsychological syndromes in presenile
dementia due to cerebral atrophy. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1986;49:163–74.
[4] The Lund and Manchester Groups. Clinical and neuropathological criteria for fronto-
temporal dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1994;57:416–8.
[5] Brun A. Frontal lobe degeneration of non-Alzheimer type. I. Neuropathology. Arch
Gerontol Geriatr 1987;6:193–208.
[6] Knopman DS, Mastri AR, Frey WH, et al. Dementia lacking distinctive histologic
features: a common non-Alzheimer degenerative dementia. Neurology 1990;40:251–6.
[7] Miller BL, Cummings JL, Villanueva-Meyer J, et al. Frontal lobe degeneration: clinical,
neuropsychological, and SPECT characteristics. Neurology 1991;41:1374–82.
[8] Mesulam M-M. Slowly progressive aphasia without generalized dementia. Ann Neurol
1982;11:592–8.
[9] Kertesz A, Hudson L, Mackenzie IRA, et al. The pathology and nosology of primary
progressive aphasia. Neurology 1994;44:2065–72.
[10] Cooper PN, Jackson M, Lennox G, et al. s, ubiquitin, and aB-crystallin—immunohis-
tochemistry defines the principal causes of degenerative frontotemporal dementia. Arch
Neurol 1995;52:1011–5.
[11] Munoz DG. The pathology of Pick complex. In: Kertesz A, Munoz DG, editors. Pick’s
disease and Pick complex. New York: Wiley & Sons; 1998. p. 211–41.
[12] Wilhelmsen KC, Lynch T, Pavlou E, et al. Localization of disinhibition dementia
parkinsonism amyotrophy complex to 17q21–22. Am J Hum Genet 1994;55:1159–65.
[13] Hutton M, Lendon CL, Rizzu P, et al. Association of missense and 50 -splice-site muta-
tions in tau with the inherited dementia FTDP-17. Nature 1998;393:702–5.
[14] Pick A. Über die Beziehungen der senilen Hirnatrophie zur Aphasie. Prager Medizinische
Wochenschrift 1892;17:165–7.
514 A. Kertesz, D.G. Munoz / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 501–518
[15] Gans A. Betrachtungen über Art und Ausbreitung des krankhaften Prozesses in einem
Fall von Pickscher Atrophie des Stirnhirns. Zeitschrift fur die Gesamte Neurologie und
Psychiatrie 1922;80:10–28.
[16] Alzheimer A. Über eigenartige Krankheitsfälle des späte en Alters. Zeitschrift fur die
Gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie 1911;4:356–85.
[17] Onari K, Spatz H. Anatomische Beitrage zur Lehre von der Pickschen umschriebenen
Grosshirnrinden-Atrophie (‘‘Picksche Krankheit’’). Zeitschrift fur die Gesamte Neurologie
und Psychiatrie 1926;101:470–511.
[18] Malamud N. Pick’s disease with atrophy of the temporal lobes. Arch Neurol Psychiatry
1940;43:210–22.
[19] Winkelman NW, Book MH. Asymptomatic extrapyramidal involvement in Pick’s disease.
Arch Neurol Psychiatry 1944;8:30–42.
[20] Constantinidis J, Richard J, Tissot R. Pick’s disease—histological and clinical corre-
lations. Eur Neurol 1974;11:208–17.
[21] Schneider C. Uber Picksche Krankheit. Monatsschrift fur Psychiatrie und Neurologie
1927;65:230–75.
[22] Schneider C. Weitere Beitrage zur Lehre von der Pickschen Krankheit. Zeitschrift fur die
Gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie 1929;120:340–84.
[23] Rosenfeld M. Die partielle Grosshirnatrophie. J Psychol Neurol 1909;14:115–30.
[24] Akelaitis AJ. Atrophy of basal ganglia in Pick’s disease. A clinicopathologic study. Arch
Neurol Psychiatry 1944;51:27–34.
[25] Wechsler AF, Verity A, Rosenstein LD, et al. Pick’s disease: a clinical, computed tomo-
graphic, and histologic study with Golgi impregnation observations. Arch Neurol 1982;39:
287–90.
[26] Holland AL, McBurney DH, Moossy J, et al. The dissolution of language in Pick’s
disease with neurofibrillary tangles: a case study. Brain Lang 1985;24:36–58.
[27] Caron M. Étude clinique de la maladie Pick. Paris: Vigot; 1934.
[28] Lüers T. Üeber den Verfall der Sprache bei der Pickschen Krankheiten (umschriebene
Atrophie der Grosshirnrinde). Zeitschrift fur die Gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie
1947;179:94–131.
[29] Kosaka K. On aphasia of Pick’s disease: a review of our own 3 cases and 49 autopsy cases
in Japan [in Japanese]. Seishin Igaku 1976;18:1181–9.
[30] Ohashi H. An aphasiologic approach to Pick’s disease. In: Hirano A, Miyoshi K, editors.
Neuropsychiatric disorders in the elderly. Tokyo: Igaku-Shoin; 1983. p. 132–5.
[31] von Branmühl A. Ueber Stammganglien veränderungen bei Pickscher Krankheit.
Zeitschrift fur die Gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie 1930;124:214.
[32] Lowenberg K. Pick’s disease—a clinicopathologic contribution. Arch Neurol Psychiatry
1935;36:768–89.
[33] Ferraro A, Jervis GA. Pick’s disease—clinicopathologic study with report of two cases.
Arch Neurol Psychiatry 1936;36:739–67.
[34] Mann DMA, South PW, Snowden JS, et al. Dementia of frontal lobe type: neuropathology
and immunohistochemistry. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1993;56:605–14.
[35] von Bagh K. Anatomic findings in 30 cases of systematic atrophy of cortex (Pick’s disease)
with special consideration of basal ganglia and long descending nerve tracts, preliminary
report. Arch Psychiatrie 1934;114:68.
[36] Munoz-Garcia D, Ludwin SK. Classic and generalized variants of Pick’s disease: clinico-
pathological, ultrastructural, and immunocytochemical comparative study. Ann Neurol
1984;16:467–80.
[37] Cummings JL, Duchen LW. Klüver-Bucy syndrome in Pick disease: clinical and patho-
logic correlations. Neurology 1981;31:1415–22.
[38] Neumann MA. Pick’s disease. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 1949;8:255–82.
[39] Malamud N, Waggoner RW. Genealogic and clinicopathologic study of Pick’s disease.
Arch Neurol Psychiatry 1943;40:288–303.
A. Kertesz, D.G. Munoz / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 501–518 515
[40] Heston LL. The clinical genetics of Pick’s disease. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1978;57:202–6.
[41] Schenk VWD. Re-examination of a family with Pick’s disease. Ann Hum Genet 1959;23:
325–33.
[42] Heutink P, Stevens M, Rizzu P, et al. Hereditary frontotemporal dementia is linked to
chromosome 17q21-q22: a genetic and clinicopathological study of three Dutch families.
Ann Neurol 1997;41:150–9.
[43] Bird TD, Nochlin D, Poorkaj P, et al. A clinical pathological comparison of three families
with frontotemporal dementia and identical mutations in the tau gene (P301L). Brain
1999;122:741–56.
[44] Murrell JR, Spillantini MG, Zolo P, et al. Tau gene mutation G389R causes a tauopathy
with abundant Pick body-like inclusions and axonal deposits. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol
1999;58:1207–26.
[45] Pickering-Brown S, Baker M, Yen SH, et al. Pick’s disease is associated with mutations in
the tau gene. Ann Neurol 2000;48:859–67.
[46] Rizzini C, Goedert M, Hodges JR, et al. Tau gene mutation K257T causes a tauopathy
similar to Pick’s disease. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 2000;59:990–1001.
[47] Sjogren T, Sjogren H, Lindgren AGH. Morbus Alzheimer and morbus Pick. Acta
Psychiatr Neurol Scand Suppl 1952;82:1–152.
[48] van Mansvelt J. Pick’s disease. A syndrome of lobar cerebral atrophy, its clinicoanato-
mical and histopathological types [thesis]. Utrecht, The Netherlands: University of
Utrecht; 1954.
[49] Lüers T, Spatz H, Picksche Krankheit. (Progressive umschriebene Grosshirnatrophie).
In: Lubarsch O, Henke F, Roessle R, editors. Handbuch der Speziellen Pathologischen
Anatomie und Histologie. Bd 13, Nerevnsystem, Teil 1. Berlin: Springer-Verlag; 1957.
p. 614–715.
[50] Delay J, Brion S. Les Démences Tardives. Paris: Masson; 1952.
[51] Mendez MF, Selwood A, Mastri AR, et al. Pick’s disease versus Alzheimer’s disease:
a comparison of clinical characteristics. Neurology 1993;43:289–92.
[52] Brion S, Mikol J. Ultrastructural study of Pick’s disease. Apropos of 3 cases. Rev Neurol
(Paris) 1971;125:273–86.
[53] Yoshimura N. Topography of Pick body distribution in Pick’s disease: a contribution to
understanding the relationship between Pick’s and Alzheimer’s disease. Clin Neuropathol
1989;8:1–6.
[54] Feany MB, Mattiace LA, Dickson DW. Neuropathologic overlap of progressive supra-
nuclear palsy, Pick’s disease and corticobasal degeneration. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol
1996;55:53–67.
[55] Singhrao SK, Biol MI, Neal JW, et al. Role of complement in the aetiology of Pick’s
disease?. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 1996;55:578–93.
[56] Galvin JE. Glial-microglial interactions and the host inflammatory response in Pick’s
disease [abstract]. Neurology 1998;50:A61.
[57] Neumann MA, Cohn R. Progressive subcortical gliosis: a rare form of presenile dementia.
Brain 1967;90:405–18.
[58] Verity MA, Wechsler AF. Progressive subcortical gliosis of Neumann: a clinicopatho-
logical study of two cases with review. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 1987;6:245–61.
[59] Bergeron C, Pollanen MS, Weyer L, et al. Unusual clinical presentations of cortical-basal
ganglionic degeneration. Ann Neurol 1996;40:893–900.
[60] Kumar A, Gottlieb G. Frontotemporal dementias. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 1993;1:95–108.
[61] Lhermitte F. ‘‘Utilization behaviour’’ and its relation to lesions of the frontal lobes. Brain
1983;106:237–55.
[62] Snowden JS, Neary D, Mann DMA. Fronto-temporal lobar degeneration: fronto-temporal
dementia, progressive aphasia, semantic dementia. London: Churchill Livingstone; 1996.
[63] Miller BL, Seeley WW, Mychack P, et al. Neuroanatomy of the self—evidence from
patients with frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 2001;57:817–21.
516 A. Kertesz, D.G. Munoz / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 501–518
[64] Kertesz A, Davidson W, Fox H. Frontal Behavioral Inventory: diagnostic criteria for
frontal lobe dementia. Can J Neurol Sci 1997;24:29–36.
[65] Barber R, Snowden JS, Craufurd D. Frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease:
retrospective differentiation using information from informants. J Neurol Neurosurg Psy-
chiatry 1995;59:61–70.
[66] Lebert F. Assessment of behavioural changes, pharmacotherapy and management of
frontotemporal dementia. In: Pasquier F, Lebert F, Scheltens Ph, editors. Frontotemporal
dementia. Dordrecht: ICG Publications; 1996. p. 71–82.
[67] Levy M, Miller BL, Cummings JL, et al. Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal
dementia: Behavioral distinctions. Arch Neurol 1996;53:687–90.
[68] Foster NL, Wilhelmsen K, Sima AFA, et al. Frontotemporal dementia and parkinsonism
linked to chromosome 17: a consensus conference. Ann Neurol 1997;41:706–15.
[69] Kertesz A, Kawarai T, Rogaeva E, et al. Familial frontotemporal dementia with ubiquitin-
positive, tau-negative inclusions. Neurology 2000;54:818–27.
[70] Neary D, Snowden JS, Mann DMA, et al. Frontal lobe dementia and motor neuron
disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1990;53:23–32.
[71] Mitsuyama Y, Takamiya S. Presenile dementia with motor neuron disease in Japan.
A new entity. Arch Neurol 1979;36:592–3.
[72] Okamoto K, Hirai S, Yamazaki T, et al. New ubiquitin-positive intraneuronal inclusions
in the extra-motor cortices in patients with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Neurosci Lett
1991;129:233–6.
[73] Wightman EM, Anderson VER, Martin J, et al. Hippocampal and neocortical ubiquitin-
immunoreactive inclusions in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis with dementia. Neurosci Lett
1992;139:269–74.
[74] Strong MJ, Grace GM. Dementia and amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. In: Kertesz A,
Munoz DG, editors. Pick’s disease and Pick complex. New York: Wiley-Liss; 1998.
p. 159–68.
[75] Weintraub S, Rubin NP, Mesulam M-M. Primary progressive aphasia: longitudinal
course, neuropsychological profile, and language features. Arch Neurol 1990;47:1329–35.
[76] Graff-Radford NR, Damasio AR, Hyman BT, et al. Progressive aphasia in a patient with
Pick’s disease. Neurology 1990;40:620–6.
[77] Kirshner HS, Tanridag O, Thurman L, et al. Progressive aphasia without dementia: two
cases with focal spongiform degeneration. Ann Neurol 1987;22:527–32.
[78] Lippa CF, Smith TW, Fontneau N. Corticonigral degeneration with neuronal
achromasia. A clinicopathological study of two cases. J Neurol Sci 1990;98:301–10.
[79] Mesulam MM, Weintraub S. Primary progressive aphasia: sharpening the focus on a clin-
ical syndrome. In Boller F, Forette F, Khachaturian Z, Poncet M, Christen Y, editors.
Heterogeneity of Alzheimer’s disease. Berlin: Springer-Verlag; 1992. p. 43–66.
[80] Galton CJ, Patterson K, Xuereb JH, et al. Atypical and typical presentations of
Alzheimer’s disease: a clinical, neuropsychological, neuroimaging and pathological study
of 13 cases. Brain 2000;123:484–98.
[81] Mesulam MM. Primary progressive aphasia. Ann Neurol 2001;49:425–32.
[82] Snowden JS, Goulding PJ, Neary D. Semantic dementia: a form of circumscribed cerebral
atrophy. Behav Neurol 1989;2:167–82.
[83] Hodges JR, Patterson K, Oxbury S, et al. Semantic dementia: progressive fluent aphasia
with temporal lobe atrophy. Brain 1992;115:1783–806.
[84] Warrington EK. The selective impairment of semantic memory. Q J Exp Psychol 1975;27:
635–57.
[85] Morris JC, Cole M, Banker BQ, et al. Hereditary dysphasic dementia and the Pick-
Alzheimer spectrum. Ann Neurol 1984;16:455–66.
[86] Goulding PJ, Northen B, Snowden JS, et al. Progressive aphasia with right-sided extra-
pyramidal signs: another manifestation of localised cerebral atrophy. Neurol Neurosurg
Psychiatry 1989;52:128–30.
A. Kertesz, D.G. Munoz / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 501–518 517
[87] Caselli RJ, Windebank AJ, Petersen RC, et al. Rapidly progressive aphasic dementia and
motor neuron disease. Ann Neurol 1993;33:200–7.
[88] Rebeiz JJ, Kolodny EH, Richardson Jr EP. Corticodentatonigral degeneration with
neuronal achromasia. Arch Neurol 1968;18:20–33.
[89] Gibb WRG, Luthert PJ, Marsden CD. Corticobasal degeneration. Brain 1989;112:1171–92.
[90] Riley DE, Lang AE, Lewis MB, et al. Cortical-basal ganglionic degeneration. Neurology
1990;40:1203–12.
[91] Clark AW, Manz HJ, White CL III, et al. Cortical degeneration with swollen chro-
matolytic neurons: its relationship to Pick’s disease. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 1986;45:
268–84.
[92] Luthert PJ, Wightman G, Leigh PN, et al. Corticobasal degeneration: immunohisto-
chemical study. Neuropathol Appl Neurobiol 1992;18:293.
[93] Jendroska K, Rossor MN, Mathias CJ, et al. Morphological overlap between corticobasal
degeneration and Pick’s disease: a clinicopathological report. Mov Disord 1995;10:111–4.
[94] Hassin GB, Levin D. Pick’s disease, clinicopathologic study and report of a case. Arch
Neurol Psychiatrie 1941;45:814.
[95] Lang AE, Bergeron C, Pollanen MS, et al. Parietal Pick’s disease mimicking cortical-basal
degeneration. Neurology 1992;44:1436–40.
[96] Paulus W, Selim M. Corticonigral degeneration with neuronal achromasia and basal
neurofibrillary tangles. Acta Neuropathol (Berl) 1990;81:89–94.
[97] Frisoni GB, Pizzolato G, Zanetti O, et al. Corticobasal degeneration: neuropsychological
assessment and dopamine D2 receptor SPECT analysis. Eur Neurol 1995;35:50–4.
[98] Dobato JL, Mateo D, de Andres C, et al. Degeneracion ganglionica corticobasal present-
andose como un sindrome de afasia progresiva primaria. Neurologia 1993;8:141.
[99] Marti-Masso JF, Lopez de Muniain A, Poza JJ, et al. Degeneracion corticobasal gang-
lionica: a proposito de siete observaciones diagnosticada clinicamente. Neurologia 1994;9:
115–20.
[100] Sakurai Y, Hashida H, Uesugi H, et al. A clinical profile of corticobasal degeneration
presenting as primary progressive aphasia. Eur Neurol 1996;36:134–7.
[101] Kertesz A, Martinez-Lage P, Davidson W, et al. The corticobasal degeneration syn-
drome overlaps progressive aphasia and frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 2000;55:
1368–75.
[102] Litvan I, Agid Y, Goetz C, et al. Accuracy of the clinical diagnosis of corticobasal
degeneration: a clinicopathologic study. Neurology 1997;48:119–25.
[103] Schneider JA, Watts RL, Gearing M, et al. Corticobasal degeneration: neuropathologic
and clinical heterogeneity. Neurology 1997;48:959–69.
[104] Pillon B, Blin J, Vidailhet M, et al. The neuropsychological pattern of corticobasal dege-
neration: comparison with progressive supranuclear palsy and Alzheimer’s disease.
Neurology 1995;45:1477–83.
[105] Dickson DW. Neuropathologic differentiation of progressive supranuclear palsy and
corticobasal degeneration. J Neurol 1999;246(Suppl):S6–15.
[106] Litvan I, Grimes DA, Lang AE, et al. Clinical features differentiating patients with
postmortem confirmed progressive supranuclear palsy and corticobasal degeneration.
J Neurol 1999;246(Suppl):S1–5.
[107] Neary D, Snowden JS, Gustafson L, et al. Frontotemporal lobar degeneration—a
consensus on clinical diagnostic criteria. Neurology 1998;51:1546–54.
[108] Rossor MN. Pick’s disease: a clinical overview. Neurology 2001;56(Suppl 4):S3–5.
[109] Lynch T, Sano M, Marder KS, et al. Clinical characteristics of a family with chromosome
17-linked disinhibition-dementia-parkinsonism-amyotrophy complex. Neurology 1994;44:
1878–84.
[110] Gustafson L, Brun A, Risberg J. Frontal lobe dementia of non-Alzheimer type. In:
Wurtman RJ, Corkin S, Growdon J, Ritter-Walker E, editors. Alzheimer’s disease. New
York: Raven Press; 1990. p. 65–71.
518 A. Kertesz, D.G. Munoz / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 501–518
[111] Feany MB, Dickson DW. Neurodegenerative disorders with extensive tau pathology: a
comparative study and review. Ann Neurol 1996;40:139–48.
[112] Delacourte A, Sergeant N, Wattez A, et al. The biochemistry of the cytoskeleton in Pick
complex. Kertesz A, Munoz DG, editors. Pick’s disease and Pick complex. New York:
Wiley-Liss; 1998. p. 243–58.
[113] Schmidt ML, Huang R, Martin JA, et al. Neurofibrillary tangles in progressive supra-
nuclear palsy contain the same tau epitopes identified in Alzheimer’s disease PHFtau.
J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 1996;55:534–9.
[114] Zhukareva V, Vogelsberg-Ragaglia V, Van Deerlin V, et al. Loss of brain tau defines novel
sporadic and familial tauopathies with frontotemporal dementia. Ann Neurol 2001;49:165–75.
[115] Poorkaj P, Bird TD, Wijsman E, et al. Tau is a candidate gene for chromosome 17 fronto-
temporal dementia. Ann Neurol 1998;43:815–25.
[116] Spillantini MG, Crowther RA, Kamphorst W, et al. Tau pathology in two Dutch
families with mutations in the microtubule-binding region of tau. Am J Pathol 1998;153:
1359–63.
[117] Hutton M. Missense and splice site mutations in tau associated with FTDP-17: multiple
pathogenic mechanisms. Neurology 2001;56(Suppl 4):S21–5.
[118] Hong M, Zhukareva V, Vogelsberg-Ragalia V, et al. Mutation-specific functional impair-
ments in distinct tau isoforms of hereditary FTDP-17. Science 1998;282:1914–7.
[119] Houlden H, Baker M, Morris HR, et al. Corticobasal degeneration and progressive
supranuclear palsy share a common tau haplotype. Neurology 2001;56:1702–6.
[120] Jesús Bullido M, Aldudo J, Frank A, et al. A polymorphism in the tau gene associated
with risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Neurosci Lett 2000;278:49–52.
[121] Yates CM, Simpson J, Maloney AFJ, et al. Neurochemical observations in a case of
Pick’s disease. Neurol Sci 1980;48:257–63.
[122] Hansen LA, DeTeresa R, Tobias H, et al. Neocortical morphometry and cholinergic
neurochemistry in Pick’s disease. Am J Pathol 1988;131:507–18.
[123] White P, Goddhardt MJ, Keet JP, et al. Neocortical cholinergic neurons in elderly people.
Lancet 1977;1:668–70.
[124] Sparks DL, Markesbery WR. Altered serotonergic and cholinergic synaptic markers in
Pick’s disease. Arch Neurol 1991;48:796–9.
[125] Mann JJ, McBride PA, Stanley M. Postmortem monoamine receptors and enzyme studies
in suicide. Ann NY Acad Sci 1986;487:114–21.
[126] Swartz JR, Miller BL, Lesser IM, et al. Frontotemporal dementia: treatment response to
serotonin selective reuptake inhibitors. J Clin Psychiatry 1997;58:212–6.
Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 519–535
Historical perspective
Parkinson’s disease
James Parkinson’s classic monograph of 1817 is generally considered the
first full description of the clinical disorder now bearing his name, Parkin-
son’s disease (PD) [1]. Physicians prior to Parkinson had described various
patients with components of the syndrome, such as tremor and gait distur-
bance, but his description was significant in bringing together the major
motor manifestations of this disease. Interestingly, his initial description
of PD specifically notes ‘‘…the senses and intellects being uninjured.’’ This
failure to recognize any intellectual impairment may have been, in part,
caused by the fact that three of his six cases were ‘‘…noticed casually in the
street’’ or were ‘‘…only seen at a distance.’’
The French physicians Trousseau and Charcot appear to have been the
first to recognize the cognitive impact of PD. Trousseau noted, in his Lec-
tures on Clinical Medicine, that ‘‘The intellect is at first unaffected, but gets
0025-7125/02/$ - see front matter 2002, Elsevier Science (USA). All rights reserved.
PII: S 0 0 2 5 - 7 1 2 5 ( 0 2 ) 0 0 0 1 2 - 3
520 J.B. Leverenz, I.G. McKeith / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 519–535
weakened at last; the patient loses his memory …precocious caducity set in’’
[2]. Charcot also recognized that in the later stages of disease ‘‘…the mind
becomes clouded and memory is lost’’ [3,4]. The English physician Sir
Edward Gowers later noted that there could be ‘‘mental weakness’’ and ‘‘loss
of memory’’ [5]. In addition, he reported a tendency to ‘‘delusions,’’ perhaps
the first description of the now well-recognized propensity for psychotic
symptoms in patients with PD and dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB).
Lewy bodies (LB) are the characteristic pathology associated with idio-
pathic PD. Friederich H. Lewy first described these intracytoplasmic inclu-
sions in the basal forebrain (substantia innominata) [6]. He did not describe,
however, any LB pathology in other regions such as the substantia nigra or
cortex. Tretiakoff appears to have been the first to describe LB in the substan-
tia nigra [7]. Subsequent reports by Foix, Hassler, Klaue, and Greenfield and
Bosanquet fully characterized the substantia nigra lesions in the brains of PD
patients, including neuronal loss, gliosis, and LB inclusions [8–11].
LB in the substantia nigra and other brainstem nuclei, such as the locus
coeruleus, have a characteristic spherical appearance with an eosinophilic
core and clear halo on standard hematoxylin and eosin staining (Fig. 1a).
In extra-brainstem regions, such as the cortex, LB are often elliptical or ir-
regularly shaped and are without a halo. This appearance of extra-brainstem
LB makes them difficult to identify using standard pathological stains, such
Fig. 1. Lewy body inclusion (arrow) in a pigmented neuron of the substantia nigra (a,
hematoxylin and eosin). Immunohistochemical staining of multiple Lewy body inclusions
(arrows) in substantia nigra neurons using antibodies to ubiquitin (b) and alpha-synuclein (c).
Lewy neurites detected by antibody to alpha-synuclein (c, small arrows). Lewy body inclusions
detected using alpha-synuclein antibodies in CA-3 neurons of the hippocampus (d, arrows).
J.B. Leverenz, I.G. McKeith / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 519–535 521
Cognitive
The primary clinical feature of DLB is progressive loss of cognitive func-
tion and associated decline of social or occupational function [13]. This pro-
gression generally occurs over years, although it may proceed more rapidly
than typically observed in AD. The cognitive profile of patients with DLB,
however, can be very similar to AD and in fact accounts for the frequent
misdiagnosis of DLB patients as having AD [29]. Similar to AD, memory
impairment can involve loss of ability to encode new memories (short-term
or recent memory), although this type of memory impairment is generally
less severe than that observed in AD patients [30,31]. Loss of the ability
to retrieve already encoded information (long-term memory) may be more
severe in DLB than AD [30]. DLB patients will also generally have more
severe visuospatial dysfunction than observed in AD patients [30,31]. Clini-
cally, this can manifest as the loss of the ability to navigate in well-known
locations (eg, getting lost in one’s own neighborhood) or on examination
by the inability to perform tasks such as clock drawing or copying figures.
J.B. Leverenz, I.G. McKeith / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 519–535 523
Similar to patients with Parkinson’s disease, patients with DLB can also
have significant problems with frontal lobe-associated cognitive skills such
as executive function. This latter problem might present clinically with loss
of ability to problem-solve or plan and successfully execute a task. On neu-
ropsychological testing, this may appear as a decline in the ability to per-
form tests such as the Wisconsin Card Sorting and Trail Making Tests
[30,31]. Although there is some overlap with the cognitive impairments in
AD, this pattern of neuropsychological dysfunction should increase the clin-
ician’s suspicion of DLB. Thus, although the bedside neuropsychological
testing (eg, Mini-Mental State Exam, Short Blessed) is important, formal
neuropsychological testing can provide a more detailed profile of cognitive
deficits that may be helpful in differentiating DLB from other disorders such
as AD. Finally, it is important to recognize that these cognitive differences
may be most evident in the early stages of disease. Neuropsychological dis-
tinctions between DLB and other neurodegenerative dementias are less clear
as a patient moves into a more severe stage of disease and cognitive deficits
become much broader.
Besides specific neuropsychological deficits, patients with DLB can exhi-
bit marked fluctuation in attention and cognition with cognitive impairment
alternating from near-normal performance to severe confusion within peri-
ods ranging from minutes to days and weeks [13,32]. This is distinct from
normal variations in function observed in all patients with neurodegenera-
tive disorders in which the severity of the fluctuations are less severe and
more predicable. In addition to fluctuations in cognition, alterations in
levels of attention and vigilance are also seen. Patients with DLB can have
episodes of severely reduced levels of arousal and also may have histories of
increased somnolence. Recent work with computerized attention and vigi-
lance tasks in DLB patients have confirmed this increased frequency of fluc-
tuation in performance on some tasks on a second-by-second basis [33]. For
clinicians, it is worth noting that these fluctuations can be helpful diagnos-
tically and can also account for visit to visit variability in patient cognitive
and functional performance.
Behavioral
Behavioral and psychiatric disturbances in dementia are a significant
source of morbidity and increase the risk for institutionalization [34]. Psy-
chotic symptoms are more frequent, persistent, and tend to occur earlier
in DLB patients, when compared with patients with AD [35–37]. Recurrent
visual hallucinations (VH) are one of the hallmarks of DLB and are one of
three core symptoms in the consensus criteria for DLB (Table 1). VH are
characteristically well formed in DLB, with patients able to describe fine
details. Emotional responses can vary, ranging from indifference to signifi-
cant distress and agitation. Level of insight may be an important factor in
the emotional response. As will be discussed in the treatment section, the
524 J.B. Leverenz, I.G. McKeith / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 519–535
Table 1
Consensus criteria for the clinical diagnosis of probable and possible DLB
1. The central feature required for a diagnosis of DLB is progressive cognitive decline of
sufficient magnitude to interfere with normal social or occupational function. Prominent or
persistent memory impairment may not necessarily occur in the early stages but is usually
evident with progression. Deficits on tests of attention and of frontal-subcortical skills and
visuospatial ability may be especially prominent.
2. Two of the following core features are essential for a diagnosis of probable DLB, and one is
essential for possible DLB:
a. Fluctuating cognition with pronounced variations in attention and alertness
b. Recurrent visual hallucinations that are typically well formed and detailed
c. Spontaneous motor features of parkinsonism
3. Features supportive of the diagnosis are:
a. Repeated falls
b. Syncope
c. Transient loss of consciousness
d. Neuroleptic sensitivity
e. Systematized delusions
f. Hallucinations in other modalities
4. A diagnosis of DLB is less likely in the presence of:
a. Stroke disease, evident as focal neurologic signs or on brain imaging
b. Evidence on physical examination and investigation of any physical illness or other brain
disorder sufficient to account for the clinical picture
From McKeith IG, et al: Consensus guidelines for the clinical and pathologic diagnosis of
dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB): report of the consortium on DLB international workshop.
Neurology 1996; 47:1114.
Motor
The four major motor symptoms of PD (resting tremor, rigidity, brady-
kinesia, and postural instability) can be observed in DLB, but with lesser
frequency and with a distribution of symptoms that differs from typical
PD [39]. In particular, patients with DLB can be bradykinetic, rigid, and
J.B. Leverenz, I.G. McKeith / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 519–535 525
are, in general, very good at correctly identifying patients who will have LB
pathology at autopsy (high positive predictive value). The lower sensitivity
values would suggest that DLB is under-diagnosed clinically. This lower sen-
sitivity is likely, in part, because of the inconsistent presence of clinical parkin-
sonism and is exacerbated by the criteria’s exclusion of patients who present
with parkinsonism more than 12 months before dementia (these patients are
currently classified as PD with dementia). Exclusion of these latter patients
lowers the ability of the presence of this core feature to identify patients with
dementia and LB pathology [13,50]. Additionally, reliable identification of
fluctuating confusion and cognition may also be a problem, as these symp-
toms are difficult to operationalize. More systematic approaches to identify-
ing fluctuating cognition may improve the clinical diagnosis of DLB.
At this point, additional prospective investigations into the clinical char-
acteristics of DLB should help improve our ability to accurately diagnosis
DLB. Ancillary studies including special imaging studies (eg, PET and
SPECT imaging) may be particularly helpful in the future. Several MRI stud-
ies have emphasized the relative preservation of medial temporal lobe
volume in DLB compared to AD [51,52], although there is no significant dif-
ference in the overall rate of brain atrophy in DLB compared with other
dementia syndromes [53]. Postmortem pathological assessment of temporal
lobe volume provides a clear correlate of this with in vivo imaging data [54].
PET and SPECT studies show consistent evidence of occipital hypometabo-
lism (reduced O2 uptake and reduced glucose utilization) compared with AD
[55,56]. The degeneration of the nigrostriatal dopaminergic projection in
DLB is demonstrable by FP-CIT SPET imaging of the pre-synaptic dopa-
mine uptake site [57,58]. This investigation has been proposed as a diagnos-
tic procedure to distinguish DLB from AD [59]. Whether other ancillary
studies such as spinal fluid testing will be helpful is less clear [60]. From a
clinician’s point of view, it is important to look prospectively for the core
and supportive signs and symptoms of DLB and to know that once a patient
fulfills these criteria, the patient is likely to have DLB pathologically.
Dementia
A focus of treatment of dementia in AD has been the reversal of the well-
characterized loss of cholinergic activity in the brain. In the United States,
J.B. Leverenz, I.G. McKeith / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 519–535 527
there are currently four medications (three of these in Great Britain) avail-
able to clinicians for treatment of the cholinergic deficit in mild to moderate
AD (see also Bonner and Peskind, in this issue) [97]. All are cholinesterase
inhibitors that increase availability of central nervous system acetylcholine
by blocking its metabolism by the cholinesterases. Tacrine was the first avail-
able cholinesterase inhibitor approved for use in AD in the United States in
1993. Since that time, three additional cholinesterase inhibitors (donepezil,
rivastigmine and galantamine) have become available with improved ease
of administration and more favorable side effects profiles than tacrine [61].
All of these ‘‘second generation’’ cholinesterase inhibitors have been shown,
in double-blind placebo-controlled studies, to have significant positive cog-
nitive and behavioral effects in AD (see Bonner and Peskind, Chapter 12).
Examination of the cholinergic system in DLB has also demonstrated a
significant reduction in activity that appears to be more severe than that
observed in AD [62]. These findings in DLB suggest that this group of
patients may also respond well to cholinesterase inhibitors [63]. Several
open-label trials have suggested that patients with DLB can tolerate choli-
nesterase inhibitors, with few reports of worsened parkinsonism, and can
have a positive clinical response [64–72]. In the first and, thus far, only
double-blind study of cholinesterase inhibitor treatment in DLB, McKeith
et al found a significant positive treatment effect of rivastigmine [73]. DLB
patients treated with rivastigmine had significantly better performance on
a computerized cognitive assessment system examining attention, working
memory, and episodic memory. Neuropsychological tests for executive func-
tion and planning also showed significant improvement with treatment. The
Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI), a systematic behavioral assessment scale,
was administered as a primary outcome measure and demonstrated signifi-
cant improvement with rivastigmine treatment on ratings of apathy, indiffer-
ence, anxiety, delusions, hallucinations, and aberrant motor behavior. Given
the significant morbidity associated with behavioral disturbance in dementia
and the high frequency of these symptoms in DLB, this latter finding has
particular importance in the treatment of patients with DLB. Further
evaluation of the cholinesterase inhibitors for patients with DLB and PD
with dementia will hopefully be available in the future.
Psychiatric features
Psychotic symptoms present a common and challenging clinical problem
in the treatment of DLB. As with other behavioral disturbances, psychotic
symptoms are associated with significant morbidity and early nursing home
placement. Double-blind placebo-controlled treatment trials of psychotic
symptoms in DLB are nonexistent (however, note rivastigmine trial in pre-
vious section [73]). Only case reports or small case series are available. There
does exist, however, a more extensive literature investigating treatment of
psychosis in PD, which suggests that the newer atypical antipsychotic agents
528 J.B. Leverenz, I.G. McKeith / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 519–535
Motor symptoms
As with psychosis, treatment of parkinsonian motor features in DLB has
been incompletely studied. Motor symptoms in DLB patients are somewhat
unique, in that there tends to be less resting tremor than observed in PD,
while there are similar levels of rigidity, bradykinesia, and postural instabil-
ity [39]. In addition, the clinical response of motor symptoms to levodopa is
modest in DLB, in comparison with the almost universal response in PD
[39]. A randomized and blinded prospective study of levodopa, or other
antiparkinsonian agents, in DLB has not been performed and is clearly
needed. Unfortunately, given the unique motor profile of DLB patients, stud-
ies of treatments in PD are not necessarily applicable.
Research in DLB
Clinical
The focus of much of the clinical research in DLB has been on diagnostic
and clinical characterization. As elaborated in previous sections, the consensus
J.B. Leverenz, I.G. McKeith / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 519–535 529
criteria for DLB have been successful in accurately identifying patients with
DLB pathology, but not as successful in identifying clinically subtle cases.
This latter problem has led to continued efforts to improve characterization
of the clinical symptoms of DLB patients and to identify biomarkers that
may help in improving diagnostic sensitivity of DLB clinical criteria.
Pathological
Pathological characterization of DLB using new histological techniques
has been a major focus of recent clinical-pathological studies [79–81]. The
clinical and pathophysiologic significance of the high frequency of alpha-
synuclein lesions in the amygdala of AD patients remains unclear. Further
evaluation of alpha-synuclein pathology and examination of other patholo-
gical markers are needed (See also Neurobiology below).
Neurobiological
Neurochemistry
Neurobiological investigations have focused on understanding the patho-
physiological basis of the unique clinical symptomatology of DLB. As
already mentioned, it has been established that DLB patients have substan-
tial loss of cholinergic function [62]. Recent evidence suggests that specific
alterations in cholinergic receptors also occur in DLB and that regionally
specific alterations in receptor density are associated with delusions and hal-
lucinations [82,83]. The well-established cholinergic disturbance in DLB and
the beneficial response to cholinesterase inhibitors are important findings
and clues to future directions of DLB research.
As would be expected, alterations in the substantia nigra-based dopami-
nergic system have been demonstrated in DLB, with reduction in dopamine
transporter sites and dopamine levels in the striatum [40,84,85]. There is
some evidence that dopaminergic changes in DLB differ from those ob-
served in PD and may account for the differential clinical response of DLB
patients to dopaminergic agents [40]. Of course, alterations in the dopamine
system have been implicated in psychotic disorders, and disruption of this
system in DLB may also be important in the development of hallucinations
and delusions.
In addition to the abnormalities of the dopaminergic and cholinergic sys-
tems in DLB, the locus coeruleus and its neurotransmitter norepinephrine
are also affected. Neuronal counts of the locus coeruleus in DLB show a
substantial loss of neurons [86]. Norepinephrine levels are also significantly
reduced in the striatum of DLB, and receptors for norepinephrine are
abnormally increased in locus coeruleus projection sites [85,86].
Neurotransmitter system alterations in dementia, and particularly in
DLB, are complex and incompletely understood. Understanding of the
alterations of these systems including potential compensatory mechanisms
530 J.B. Leverenz, I.G. McKeith / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 519–535
Biology of alpha-synuclein
The discovery of alpha-synuclein mutations in a subset of cases with famil-
ial PD and the presence of this protein in LB has led to an exploration of sy-
nuclein biology in both normal brain function and in disease. Alpha-synuclein
appears to play an important role in normal vesicular function in the pre-
synaptic terminals of neurons [87]. Mutation in the gene is associated with a
form of familial PD characterized by LB inclusions [88,89]. Interestingly, in
at least one of these families with an alpha-synuclein mutation, PD motor
symptoms are also associated with a dementing syndrome [90]. Further work
in PD and DLB has also suggested a more widespread dysfunction of all of
the synuclein proteins (alpha-, beta- and gamma-synuclein) [91]. There may,
in fact, be critical interactions between alpha- and beta-synucleins that influ-
ence the development of LB pathology [92]. Interactions between alpha-synu-
clein and other proteins implicated in the synucleinopathies, such as parkin
and synphilin, may also play a role in the development of LB pathology
[93]. Additional recent investigations have suggested an abnormal post-
translational modification of alpha-synuclein in synucleinopathies [94].
Transgenic mouse models are currently under development; these mice
have the mutated form of alpha-synuclein inserted into their genome and
have shown associated alpha-synuclein abnormalities in the brain [95]. It
is unclear at this time whether these animals will serve as useful models for
the clinical-pathologic components of DLB; however, they certainly should
provide important, more general, insights into the pathophysiology of the
synucleinopathies.
Genetics
Mutations in alpha-synuclein and parkin genes in familial PD would
appear to be important in helping understand the pathophysiology of PD
and its relationship to DLB. In these families with alpha-synuclein and par-
kin mutations, individuals have presented with PD symptoms and/or
dementia, suggesting that the same mutation can lead to a variety of clinical
phenotypes [90]. These findings also appear to blur the distinctions between
DLB and PD. The frequent presence of AD and LB pathology together in
familial AD would also suggest a possible pathophysiologic connection
between AD and DLB [80]. In contrast to PD and AD, only a few families
with familial DLB have been described [96]. In these families, dementia was
the predominant presenting symptom and LB pathology was confirmed at
autopsy. At this time, there is no mutation that has been associated with
DLB. Identification of additional cases of familial DLB and of mutations
could be critical to our understanding of the etiologic processes that lead
to the development of this disease.
J.B. Leverenz, I.G. McKeith / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 519–535 531
Summary
DLB is a complex disorder with important associations with PD and AD.
As clinicians, it is important for us to identify these patients because of their
unique responses to medical interventions and to help patients and care-
givers more fully understand this disease process and its implications.
Further research is needed to improve our understanding of the pathophy-
siology of this important dementing disorder, with the ultimate goal of
improving clinical management of this disease.
References
[1] Parkisnson J. An essay on the shaking palsy. In: Critchley M, editor. James Parkinson.
London: Macmillan, 1995.
[2] Troussseau A. Lecture XV: Senile trembing and paralysis agitans. In: Lectures on clinical
medicine delivered at the Hotel-Dieu, Paris. London: New Sydenham Society, 1868.
[3] Charcot JM. Cinquieme lecon: de la paralysie agitante. In: Bourneville, editor. Lecons sur
les maladies du syseme nerveux faites a la Salpetriere. Paris: A. Delahaye, 1872.
[4] Charcot JM, Vulpian A. De la paralysie agitante. Gaz Hebdom Med Chir 1862;8/9:
754,765,816.
[5] Gowers W. A manual of diseases of the nervous system. London: Churchill, 1886–1888.
[6] Lewy FH. Zur pathologischen anatomie der paralysis agitans. Dtsch Z Nervenheikd
1913;50:50–5.
[7] Tretiakoff C. Contribution a l’etude de l’anatomie pathologique du locus niger [Thesis].
Paris: University of Paris; 1919.
[8] Foix MC. Les lesions anatomiques de la maladie de Parkinson. Rev Neurol (Paris) 1921;28:
593–600.
[9] Greenfield JG, Bosanquet FD. The brain-stem lesions in parkinsonism. J Neurol Neuro-
surg Psychiatry 1953;16:213–26.
[10] Hassler R. Zur pathologie der paralysis agitans und des postenzephalitishen parkinso-
nismus. Journal fur Psychologie und Neurologie 1938;49:387.
[11] Klaue R. Parkinsonische krankheit (paralysis agitans) und postencephalitischer parkinso-
nismus. Arch Psychiatr Nervenkr 1940;111:251–321.
[12] McKeith IG, Perry EK, Perry RH. Report of the second dementia with Lewy body
international workshop: diagnosis and treatment. Consortium on Dementia with Lewy
Bodies. Neurology 1999;53:902–5.
[13] McKeith LG, Galasko D, Kosaka K, et al. Consensus guidelines for the clinical and
pathologic diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB): report of the consortium on
DLB international workshop. Neurology 1996;47:1113–24.
[14] Baba M, Nakajo S, Tu PH, et al. Aggregation of alpha-synuclein in Lewy bodies of sporadic
Parkinson’s disease and dementia with Lewy bodies. Am J Pathol 1998;152:879–84.
[15] Galvin JE, Lee VM, Schmidt ML, et al. Pathobiology of the Lewy body. Adv Neurol
1999;80:313–24.
[16] Irizarry MC, Growdon W, Gomez-Isla T, et al. Nigral and cortical Lewy bodies and
dystrophic nigral neurites in Parkinson’s disease and cortical Lewy body disease contain
alpha-synuclein immunoreactivity. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 1998;57:334–7.
[17] Spillantini MG, Crowther RA, Jakes R, et al. Alpha-Synuclein in filamentous inclusions of
Lewy bodies from Parkinson’s disease and dementia with Lewy bodies. Proc Natl Acad Sci
USA 1998;95:6469–73.
[18] Galvin JE, Lee VM, Trojanowski JQ. Synucleinopathies: clinical and pathological impli-
cations. Arch Neurol 2001;58:186–90.
532 J.B. Leverenz, I.G. McKeith / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 519–535
[19] Lewy H. Die Lehre vom tonus und der Bewegung. Zugleich systematische untersuchungen
zur klinik, physiologie, pathologie and pathogenese der paralysis agitans. Berlin: Julius
Springer, 1923, vol 50.
[20] Woodard JS. Concentric hyalin inclusion body formation in mental disease analysis of
twenty-seven cases. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 1962;2:442–9.
[21] Kosaka K. Diffuse Lewy body disease in Japan. J Neurol 1990;237:197–204.
[22] Okazaki H, Lipkin L, Aronson S. Diffuse intracytoplasmic ganglionic inclusions (Lewy
type) associated with progressive dementia and quadriparesis in flexion. J Neuropath Exp
Neurol 1961;20:237–44.
[23] Ditter SM, Mirra SS. Neuropathologic and clinical features of Parkinson’s disease in
Alzheimer’s disease patients. Neurology 1987;37:754–60.
[24] Leverenz J, Sumi M. Parkinson’s disease in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Neurol
1986;43:662–4.
[25] Hansen L, Salmon D, Galasko D, et al. The Lewy body variant of Alzheimer’s disease:
a clinical and pathologic entity. Neurology 1990;40:1–8.
[26] Crystal HA, Dickson DW, Lizardi JE, et al. Antemortem diagnosis of diffuse Lewy body
disease. Neurology 1990;40:1523–8.
[27] Dickson DW, Ruan D, Crystal H, et al. Hippocampal degeneration differentiates diffuse Lewy
body disease (DLBD) from Alzheimer’s disease: light and electron microscopic immuno-
cytochemisty of CA2–3 neurites specific to DLBD. Neurology 1991;41:1402–9.
[28] Dickson DW. Lewy body variant. Neurology 1990;40:1147–50.
[29] McKeith IG, Fairbairn AF, Perry RH, et al. The clinical diagnosis and misdiagnosis of
senile dementia of Lewy body type (SDLT). Br J Psychiatry 1994;165:324–32.
[30] Salmon DP, Galasko D, Hansen LA, et al. Neuropsychological deficits associated with
diffuse Lewy body disease. Brain Cogn 1996;31:148–65.
[31] Simard M, van Reekum R, Cohen T. A review of the cognitive and behavioral symptoms
in dementia with Lewy bodies. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2000;12:425–50.
[32] Ballard C, O’Brien J, Gray A, et al. Attention and fluctuating attention in patients with
dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 2001;58:977–82.
[33] Walker MP, Ayre GA, Cummings JL, et al. Quantifying fluctuation in dementia with Lewy
bodies, Alzheimer’s disease, and vascular dementia. Neurology 2000;54:1616–25.
[34] Cummings JL, Diaz C, Levy M, et al. Neuropsychiatric syndromes in neurodegenerative
disease: frequency and significance. Semin Clin Neuropsychiatry 1996;1:241–7.
[35] Ballard C, Holmes C, McKeith I, et al. Psychiatric morbidity in dementia with Lewy
bodies: a prospective clinical and neuropathological comparative study with Alzheimer’s
disease. Am J Psychiatry 1999;156:1039–45.
[36] Klatka LA, Louis ED, Schiffer RB. Psychiatric features in diffuse Lewy body disease:
a clinicopathologic study using Alzheimer’s disease and Parkinson’s disease comparison
groups. Neurology 1996;47:1148–52.
[37] McShane R, Gedling K, Reading M, et al. Prospective study of relations between cortical
Lewy bodies, poor eyesight, and hallucinations in Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg
Psychiatry 1995;59:185–8.
[38] Del Ser T, McKeith I, Anand R, et al. Dementia with Lewy bodies: findings from an
international multicentre study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2000;15:1034–45.
[39] Louis ED, Klatka LA, Liu Y, et al. Comparison of extrapyramidal features in 31 patho-
logically confirmed cases of diffuse Lewy body disease and 34 pathologically confirmed
cases of Parkinson’s disease. Neurology 1997;48:376–80.
[40] Piggott MA, Marshall EF, Thomas N, et al. Striatal dopaminergic markers in dementia with Lewy
bodies, Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s diseases: rostrocaudal distribution. Brain 1999;122:1449–68.
[41] Weiner MF, Risser RC, Cullum CM, et al. Alzheimer’s disease and its Lewy body variant:
a clinical analysis of postmortem verified cases. Am J Psychiatry 1996;153:1269–73.
[42] Holmes C, Cairns N, Lantos P, et al. Validity of current clinical criteria for Alzheimer’s
disease, vascular dementia and dementia with Lewy bodies. Br J Psychiatry 1999;174:45–50.
J.B. Leverenz, I.G. McKeith / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 519–535 533
[43] Litvan I, MacIntyre A, Goetz CG, et al. Accuracy of the clinical diagnoses of Lewy body
disease, Parkinson disease, and dementia with Lewy bodies: a clinicopathologic study.
Arch Neurol 1998;55:969–78.
[44] Lopez O, Becker J, Kaufer D, et al. Research evaluation and prospective diagnosis of
dementia with Lewy bodies. Arch Neurol, In press.
[45] Luis C, Barker W, Gajaraj K, et al. Sensitivity and specificity of three clinical criteria for
dementia with Lewy bodies in an autopsy-verified sample. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 1999;14:
526–33.
[46] McKeith IG, Ballard CG, Perry RH, et al. Prospective validation of consensus criteria for
the diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies. Neurology 2000;54:1050–8.
[47] McShane RH, Esiri MM, Joachim C, et al. Prospective evaluation of diagnostic criteria for
dementia with Lewy bodies. Neurbiol Aging 1998;19:S204.
[48] Mega MS, Masterman DL, Benson DF, et al. Dementia with Lewy bodies: reliability and
validity of clinical and pathologic criteria. Neurology 1996;47:1403–9.
[49] Papka M, Schiffer R, Rubio A. Diagnosing Lewy body disease: accuracy of clinical criteria
in detecting Lewy body pathology. Neurobiol Aging 1998;19:203.
[50] McKeith IG, Burn D. Spectrum of Parkinson’s disease, Parkinson’s dementia, and Lewy
body dementia. Neurol Clin 2000;18:865–902.
[51] Barber R, McKeith IG, Ballard C, et al. A comparison of medial and lateral temporal lobe
atrophy in dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer’s disease: magnetic resonance
imaging volumetric study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2001;12:198–205.
[52] Harvey GT, Hughes J, McKeith IG, et al. Magnetic resonance imaging differences between
dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer’s disease: a pilot study. Psychol Med 1999;29:181–7.
[53] O’Brien JT, Paling S, Barber R, et al. Progressive brain atrophy on serial MRI in dementia
with Lewy bodies, AD, and vascular dementia. Neurology 2001;56:1386–8.
[54] Lippa CF, Johnson R, Smith TW. The medial temporal lobe in dementia with Lewy
bodies: a comparative study with Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol 1998;43:102–6.
[55] Higuchi M, Tashiro M, Arai H, et al. Glucose hypometabolism and neuropathological
correlates in brains of dementia with Lewy bodies. Exp Neurol 2000;162:247–56.
[56] Imamura T, Ishii K, Hirono N, et al. Occipital glucose metabolism in dementia with Lewy
bodies with and without Parkinsonism: a study using positron emission tomography.
Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2001;12:194–7.
[57] Lobotesis K, Fenwick JD, Phipps A, et al. Occipital hypoperfusion on SPECT in dementia
with Lewy bodies but not AD. Neurology 2001;56:643–9.
[58] Walker Z, Costa DC, Janssen AG, et al. Dementia with Lewy bodies: a study of post-
synaptic dopaminergic receptors with iodine-123 iodobenzamide single-photon emission
tomography. Eur J Nucl Med 1997;24:609–14.
[59] Walker Z, Costa DC, Ince P, et al. In-vivo demonstration of dopaminergic degeneration in
dementia with Lewy bodies. Lancet 1999;354:646–7.
[60] Tschampa HJ, Schulz-Schaeffer W, Wiltfang J, et al. Decreased CSF amyloid b42 and
normal tau levels in dementia with Lewy bodies. Neurology 2001;56:576.
[61] Conn DK. Cholinesterase inhibitors. Comparing the options for mild-to-moderate demen-
tia. Geriatrics 2001;56:56–7.
[62] Samuel W, Alford M, Hofstetter CR, et al. Dementia with Lewy bodies versus pure Alz-
heimer disease: differences in cognition, neuropathology, cholinergic dysfunction, and
synapse density. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 1997;56:499–508.
[63] Liberini P, Valerio A, Memo M, et al. Lewy-body dementia and responsiveness to choline-
sterase inhibitors: a paradigm for heterogeneity of Alzheimer’s disease? Trends Pharmacol
Sci 1996;17:155–60.
[64] Aarsland D, Bronnick K, Karlsen K. Donepezil for dementia with Lewy bodies: a case
study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 1999;14:69–72.
[65] Grace J, Daniel S, Stevens T, et al. Long-term use of rivastigmine in patients with dementia
with Lewy bodies: an open-label trial. Int Psychogeriatr 2001;13:199–205.
534 J.B. Leverenz, I.G. McKeith / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 519–535
[66] Kaufer DI, Catt KE, Lopez OL, et al. Dementia with Lewy bodies: response of delirium-
like features to donepezil. Neurology 1998;51:1512.
[67] Lanctot KL, Herrmann N. Donepezil for behavioural disorders associated with Lewy
bodies: a case series. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2000;15:338–45.
[68] Levy R, Eagger S, Griffiths M, et al. Lewy bodies and response to tacrine in Alzheimer’s
disease. Lancet 1994;343:176.
[69] Maclean LE, Collins CC, Byrne EJ. Dementia with Lewy bodies treated with rivastigmine:
effects on cognition, neuropsychiatric symptoms, and sleep. Int Psychogeriatr 2001;13:277–88.
[70] McKeith IG, Grace JB, Walker Z, et al. Rivastigmine in the treatment of dementia with Lewy
bodies: preliminary findings from an open trial. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2000;15:387–92.
[71] Samuel W, Caligiuri M, Galasko D, et al. Better cognitive and psychopathologic response
to donepezil in patients prospectively diagnosed as dementia with Lewy bodies: a preliminary
study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2000;15:794–802.
[72] Shea C, MacKnight C, Rockwood K. Donepezil for treatment of dementia with Lewy
bodies: a case series of nine patients. Int Psychogeriatr 1998;10:229–38.
[73] McKeith I, Del Ser T, Spano P, et al. Efficacy of rivastigmine in dementia with Lewy bodies:
a randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled international study. Lancet 2000;356:
2031–6.
[74] Goetz CG, Blasucci LM, Leurgans S, et al. Olanzapine and clozapine: comparative effects
on motor function in hallucinating PD patients. Neurology 2000;55:789–94.
[75] Manson AJ, Schrag A, Lees AJ. Low-dose olanzapine for levodopa induced dyskinesias.
Neurology 2000;55:795–9.
[76] Richard IH, Nutt J. Worsening of motor function in Parkinson’s disease: a ‘‘typical’’
response to ‘‘atypical’’ antipsychotic medications. Neurology 2000;55:748–9.
[77] Wolters EC. Dopaminomimetric psychosis in Parkinson’s disease patients: diagnosis and
treatment. Neurology 1999;52:S10–13.
[78] Rich SS, Friedman JH, Ott BR. Risperidone versus clozapine in the treatment of psycho-
sis in six patients with Parkinson’s disease and other akinetic-rigid syndromes. J Clin
Psychiatry 1995;56:556–9.
[79] Hamilton RL. Lewy bodies in Alzheimer’s disease: a neuropathological review of 145 cases
using alpha-synuclein immunohistochemistry. Brain Pathol 2000;10:378–84.
[80] Lippa CF, Fujiwara H, Mann DM, et al. Lewy bodies contain altered alpha-synuclein in
brains of many familial Alzheimer’s disease patients with mutations in presenilin and
amyloid precursor protein genes. Am J Pathol 1998;153:1365–70.
[81] Lippa CF, Schmidt ML, Lee VM, et al. Antibodies to alpha-synuclein detect Lewy bodies
in many Down’s syndrome brains with Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol 1999;45:353–7.
[82] Ballard C, Piggott M, Johnson M, et al. Delusions associated with elevated muscarinic
binding in dementia with Lewy bodies. Ann Neurol 2000;48:868–76.
[83] Shiozaki K, Iseki E, Uchiyama H, et al. Alterations of muscarinic acetylcholine receptor
subtypes in diffuse Lewy body disease: relation to Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg
Psychiatry 1999;67:209–13.
[84] Joyce JN, Smutzer G, Whitty CJ, et al. Differential modification of dopamine transporter
and tyrosine hydroxylase mRNAs in midbrain of subjects with Parkinson’s, Alzheimer’s
with parkinsonism, and Alzheimer’s disease. Mov Disord 1997;12:885–97.
[85] Langlais PJ, Thal L, Hansen L, et al. Neurotransmitters in basal ganglia and cortex of
Alzheimer’s disease with and without Lewy bodies. Neurology 1993;43:1927–34.
[86] Leverenz JB, Miller MA, Dobie DJ, et al. Increased alpha 2-adrenergic receptor binding in locus
coeruleus projection areas in dementia with Lewy bodies. Neurobiol Aging 2001;22:555–61.
[87] Murphy DD, Rueter SM, Trojanowski JQ, et al. Synucleins are developmentally expressed,
and alpha-synuclein regulates the size of the presynaptic vesicular pool in primary
hippocampal neurons. J Neurosci 2000;20:3214–20.
[88] Kruger R, Kuhn W, Muller T, et al. Ala30Pro mutation in the gene encoding alpha-
synuclein in Parkinson’s disease. Nat Genet 1998;18:106–8.
J.B. Leverenz, I.G. McKeith / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 519–535 535
[89] Polymeropoulos M, Lavedan C, Leroy E, et al. Mutation in the alpha-synuclein gene identi-
fied in families with Parkinson’s disease. Science 1997;276:2045–7.
[90] Bostantjopoulou S, Katsarou Z, Papadimitriou A, et al. Clinical features of parkinsonian
patients with the alpha-synuclein (G209A) mutation. Mov Disord 2001;16:1007–13.
[91] Galvin J, Uryu K, Lee V, et al. Axon pathology in Parkinson’s disease and lewy body
dementia hippocampus contains alpha-, beta-, and gamma-synuclein. Proc Natl Acad Sci
USA 1999;96:13450–5.
[92] Hashimoto M, Rockenstein E, Mante M, et al. Beta-synuclein inhibits alpha-synuclein aggre-
gation. A possible role as an anti-parkinsonian factor. Neuron 2001;32:213–23.
[93] Chung K, Zhang Y, Lim K, et al. Parkin ubiquitinates the alpha-synublein-interacting
protein, synphilin-1: implications for Lewy-body formation in Parkinson’s disease. Nat Med
2001;7:1108–9.
[94] Giasson BI, Duda JE, Murray IV, et al. Oxidative damage linked to neurodegeneration by
selective alpha-synuclein nitration in synucleinopathy lesions. Science 2000;290:985–9.
[95] Masliah E, Rockenstein E, Veinbergs I, et al. Dopaminergic loss and inclusion body for-
mation in alpha-synuclein mice: implications for neurodegenerative disorders. Science 2000;
287:1265–9.
[96] Ohara K, Takauchi S, Kokai M, et al. Familial dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB). Clin
Neuropathol 1999;18:232–9.
[97] Bonner LT, Peskind ER. Pharmacological treatments of dementia. Med Clin N Am 2002;
86:655–72.
Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 537–550
AIDS dementia
David B. Clifford, MD*
Department of Neurology, Washington University School of Medicine, Box 8111,
660 South Euclid Street, St. Louis, MO 63110, USA
0025-7125/02/$ - see front matter 2002, Elsevier Science (USA). All rights reserved.
PII: S 0 0 2 5 - 7 1 2 5 ( 0 2 ) 0 0 0 0 5 - 6
538 D.B. Clifford / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 537–550
encephalopathy. The latter is associated with clinical dementia that has been
called AIDS dementia complex (ADC) [3]. Other names for this condition
are HIV-associated cognitive motor complex and HIV-associated dementia.
A milder clinical presentation that may be a precursor of full-blown dementia
is minor cognitive motor disorder, which is recognized by a complaint of
neurologic dysfunction accompanied by changes in neuropsychologic test
performance. Systematic classification of these HIV-associated cognitive
processes is most widely performed using the American Academy of Neurol-
ogy criteria [4].
normal levels, however) and decline of viral loads to levels that are not
detectable by current RNA assays.
A second important aspect of HIV is the rapid and persistent rate of viral
replication that typifies this infection [5]. The virus has a half-life of only a
few days in the plasma compartment, implying that the large amount of
virus correlates with a huge synthesis rate estimated at 109 to 1010 virions
per day. Replication requires a viral reverse transcriptase step in which viral
RNA is transcribed to DNA, which may then be inserted into the host
DNA. This step is a critical target for antiretroviral therapy. Also important
is a high mutation rate that allows rapid change in the genetics of the virus
even in the same individual over time. With the high mutation rate and high
turnover rate, it is possible for this virus to generate resistant versions in a
distressingly rapid and efficient manner. Presumably, this modulating char-
acteristic of the virus explains in part why the immune response alone is
unable to control the infection and why antiviral medications cannot eradi-
cate it.
Epidemiology
ADC is seen primarily in advanced HIV patients in whom immunodefi-
ciency has developed. Although it may be the presenting complaint leading
to the diagnosis of AIDS, this is rarely the case. Instead, it complicates the
disease course of subjects who often have multiple medical problems be-
cause of the broad impact of immunodeficiency. Before effective antiviral
therapy was developed, ADC developed in more than 60% of patients devel-
oping AIDS [3]. The introduction of zidovudine (ZDV) therapy and, subse-
quently, dual nucleoside therapy resulted in a decline in ADC to the range of
20% [6]. The more recent introduction of highly active antiretroviral therapy
(HAART) has further reduced the incidence to considerably less than 10%
of AIDS patients. In HAART-treated patients, ADC has declined less than
other complications, however, now representing a greater proportion of
AIDS-defining clinical events [7]. Further, it is notable that ADC is now
encountered more commonly in patients with a higher CD4 count. Dore
et al [7] report that the median CD4 count in subjects with ADC rose from
70 to 170 cells per cubic millimeter after the introduction of HAART. Addi-
tional factors that are associated with an increased risk of dementia include
anemia, constitutional symptoms, and wasting [8]. Advancing age has been
suspected as a risk but has not been clearly demonstrated as such. Similarly,
it has not been clear that exposure through intravenous drug use is asso-
ciated with a risk different from that of sexual exposure for the development
of ADC [9]. The impact of viral replication is clearly important; this is sup-
ported by the declining incidence of ADC with increasingly effective treat-
ment and by the observation that ADC patients have a higher HIV viral
load in the cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) than patients without ADC [10].
Therapy
HIV therapy has evolved rapidly over the past decade, propelled by the
urgent need to address this devastating viral disease. Improvements in gen-
eral therapy for HIV have led to marked improvements in life expectancy
and quality of life and have been accompanied by improvements in the
neurologic complications. The unique aspects of the brain and CSF com-
partment, which involve barriers to drug penetration and a different immu-
nologic milieu, make it critical to maintain additional vigilance for
incongruity of response in these compartments compared with plasma,
where most observations informing drug development occur. Further,
inability to completely clear this viral infection and the potential of indirect
mechanisms to cause complications even when the replicative viral infection
is largely controlled make it critical to continue to study the evolution of
ADC in the HAART era.
544 D.B. Clifford / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 537–550
HIV therapy is rapidly evolving. Some of the drugs required have com-
plicated and important drug interactions. It is thus best for HIV patients
to be managed by clinicians with extensive practice in this area. HIV specia-
lists have the incentive and opportunity to stay abreast of current therapeu-
tic knowledge. Without this investment, care is often suboptimal, leading to
inferior clinical outcomes.
The best point after infection for initiation of therapy remains uncertain.
This therapeutic decision must balance the rational desire to treat infections
early when involvement may be minimal and complications have not
occurred with the recognition that current therapy has substantial long-term
toxicities, is expensive, and inevitably results in evolution of resistant virus
over long periods of use. The current trend in academic centers is moving
toward delayed initiation of drug therapy because of these concerns. Current
recommendations suggest initiation of therapy in acute infection or sympto-
matic infection. For asymptomatic subjects, therapy should be offered when
viral loads are high (>55,000 copies per milliliter) or the CD4 count has fallen
to levels less than 350 cells per cubic millimeter. Evolving recommendations
may be studied as they are developed by a panel of experts working with the
International AIDS Society—USA (http://www.hivatis.org).
Once therapy is undertaken, the goal is to achieve maximal viral suppres-
sion with minimal toxicity. Most subjects should reach undetectable levels of
HIV virus in the plasma with currently available HAART therapy. Ongoing
research probes the ability of resistance testing to improve drug selection by
genotypic or phenotypic techniques, the role of therapeutic drug monitoring
in dose selection, and tools to enhance compliance, which is absolutely
essential to therapeutic success.
Currently, three classes of antiretroviral medications are approved, with
15 approved drugs available as follows:
Nucleoside reverse transcriptase inhibitors (NRTIs)
• Abacavir (Ziagen)
• Didanosine (Videx)
• Lamivudine (Epivir, Combivir)
• Stavudine (Zerit)
• Zalcitabine (Hivid)
• Zidovudine (Retrovir, Combivir)
Nonnucleoside reverse transcriptase inhibitors (NNRTIs)
• Delavirdine (Rescriptor)
• Efavirenz (Sustiva)
• Nevirapine (Viramune)
Protease inhibitors (PIs)
• Amprenavir (Agenerase)
• Indinavir (Crixivan)
D.B. Clifford / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 537–550 545
The first class of drugs developed was the NRTI class, which includes
ZDV (also known as AZT), didanosine (ddI), zalcitabine (ddC), lamivudine,
stavudine (d4T), and abacavir (ABC). These drugs are important compo-
nents of most standard treatment combinations, and exploration of a triple-
nucleoside HAART regimen also seems to be acceptable [50]. Because resis-
tance tends to develop within classes of drugs, delaying exposure to other
classes of drugs has a significant theoretical benefit. Major toxicity of NRTI
drugs includes bone marrow toxicity (ZDV), pancreatitis (d4T, ddC, ddI),
rashes (most serious hypersensitivity reaction with ABC), distal sensory per-
ipheral neuropathy (ddI, d4T, ddC), and lactic acidosis. Other less common
neurologic toxicities include myopathy (ZDV) and headache (especially
ZDV).
The second class of drugs developed was the NNRTI class. These drugs
specifically block the active site of HIV-1 reverse transcriptase. These are
potent compounds that are highly specific for HIV-1 alone. They are drugs
useful only in combinations, because resistance develops quite rapidly in
monotherapy. They are extremely important components of HAART, how-
ever. The chief toxicities of this class are hypersensitivity reactions with
rashes, and hepatic toxicity. Approved drugs in this class include nevirapine,
delavirdine, and efavirenz. Neurologic toxicity has been the most common
issue with efavirenz, with more than half of subjects reporting a detectable
change in sleep, dreaming, attention, or alertness [51]. Fortunately, this is
mild in most cases and tolerated well enough to allow continuation of the
therapy. Symptoms tend to recede over the initial weeks of therapy.
The third class of antiretroviral agents is the PI class. These drugs, includ-
ing saquinavir, ritonavir, indinavir, nelfinavir, amprenavir, and lopinavir,
revolutionized HIV therapy when they were introduced. Their marked effi-
cacy when combined with NRTI drugs vastly increased the ability to achieve
prolonged viral inhibition, resulting in fewer opportunistic complications,
longer life, and improved quality of life. The combination therapies origi-
nally including PIs were referred to as HAART. As potent NNRTI and
NRTI drugs have been introduced, the concept of HAART has been broad-
ened to include the numerous triple or more drug-containing regimens that
typically can achieve undetectable HIV RNA viral loads.
Improvements in the overall health of HIV patients associated with
HAART are not without a price in toxicity. Long-term complications asso-
ciated with these drugs in conjunction with the other antiretroviral agents
are a growing source of concern. Gastrointestinal toxicity with nausea,
diarrhea, and hepatic toxicity has been seen with drug regimens that contain
PI drugs. Metabolic complications, including insulin resistance and the
546 D.B. Clifford / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 537–550
Major questions
HIV infection has resulted in many opportunities to learn more about
medicine and neurology. The treatments devised have already resulted in
longer and healthier lives for our patients. We still are challenged by an
incomplete understanding of the pattern of trafficking of the virus across the
blood-brain barrier and by the potential pathologic mechanisms resulting in
ADC and other neurologic consequences of infection. Because the CNS
compartment represents a challenge to treatment, it remains a concern that
over the extended survival time now achieved by our patients, the brain may
become a recurrent source for infection or that accelerated degenerative pro-
cesses may result in more serious and common neurologic disease later in the
course of infection. Ongoing studies focusing on the CNS and CSF are con-
tinuing to unravel the virology, pharmacology, and pathologic conse-
quences in the brain and CSF.
References
[1] Sepkowitz KA. AIDS—the first 20 years. N Engl J Med 2001;344:1764–72.
[2] Snider WD, Simpson DM, Nielsen S, et al. Neurological complications of acquired immune
deficiency syndrome: analysis of 50 patients. Ann Neurol 1983;14:403–18.
[3] Navia BA, Jordan BD, Price RW. The AIDS dementia complex: I. Clinical features. Ann
Neurol 1986;19:517–24.
[4] Working Group of the American Academy of Neurology AIDS Task Force. Nomenclature
and research case definitions for neurologic manifestations of human immunodeficiency
virus-type 1 (HIV-1) infection. Neurology 1991;41:778–85.
548 D.B. Clifford / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 537–550
[5] Young B, Kuritzkes DR. Viral kinetics: implications for treatment. AIDS 1999;13(Suppl):
S11–7.
[6] Sacktor N, Lyles RH, McFarlane G, et al. HIV-1-related neurological disease incidence
changes in the era of highly active antiretroviral therapy [abstract]. Neurology 1999;52:
A252–3.
[7] Dore GJ, Correll PK, Li Y, et al. Changes to AIDS dementia complex in the era of highly
active antiretroviral therapy. AIDS 1999;13:1249–53.
[8] McArthur JC, Hoover DR, Bacellar H, et al. Dementia in AIDS patients: incidence and
risk factors. Neurology 1993;43:2245–52.
[9] Concha M, Graham NMH, Munoz A, et al. Effect of chronic substance abuse on neuro-
psychological performance in intravenous drug users with a high prevalence of HIV-1 sero-
positivity. Am J Epidemiol 1992;136:1338–48.
[10] McArthur JC, McClernon DR, Cronin MF, et al. Relationship between human im-
munodeficiency virus-associated dementia and viral load in cerebrospinal fluid and brain.
Ann Neurol 1997;42:689–98.
[11] Anders KH, Guerra WF, Tomiyasu U, et al. Review article: the neuropathology of
AIDS—UCLA experience and review. AIDS Neuropathol 1986;124:537–58.
[12] Bell JE. The neuropathology of adult HIV infection. Rev Neurol (Paris) 1998;154:816–29.
[13] Burns DK, Risser RC, White CL III. The neuropathology of human immunodeficiency
virus infection. Arch Pathol Lab Med 1991;115:1112–24.
[14] Everall IP, Heaton RK, Marcotte TD, et al. Cortical synaptic density is reduced in mild to
moderate human immunodeficiency virus neurocognitive disorder. Brain Pathol 1999;9:209–17.
[15] Masliah E, Heaton RK, Marcotte TD, et al. Dendritic injury is a pathological substrate for
human immunodeficiency virus-related cognitive disorder. Ann Neurol 1997;42:963–72.
[16] Sá MJ, Madeira MD, Ruela C, et al. AIDS does not alter the total number of neurons in
the hippocampal formation but induces cell atrophy: a stereological study. Acta Neuro-
pathol (Berl) 2000;99:643–53.
[17] Wiley CA, Masliah E, Morey M, et al. Neocortical damage during HIV infection. Ann
Neurol 1991;29:651–7.
[18] Everall IP, Luthert PJ, Lantos PL. Neuronal loss in the frontal cortex in HIV infection.
Lancet 1991;337:1119–21.
[19] Gelman BB, Guinto FC Jr. Morphometry, histopathology, and tomography of cerebral
atrophy in the acquired immunodeficiency syndrome. Ann Neurol 1992;32:31–40.
[20] Fischer CP, Gundersen HJG, Pakkenberg B. Preferential loss of large neocortical neurons
during HIV infection: a study of the size distribution of neocortical neurons in the human
brain. Brain Res 1999;828:119–26.
[21] Gray F, Mohr M, Rozenberg F, et al. Varicella-zoster virus encephalitis in acquired immu-
nodeficiency syndrome: report of four cases. Neuropathol Appl Neurobiol 1992;18:502–14.
[22] Kaul M, Garden GA, Lipton SA. Pathways to neuronal injury and apoptosis in HIV-
associated dementia. Nature 2001;410:988–94.
[23] Thompson KA, McArthur JC, Wesselingh SL. Correlation between neurological progres-
sion and astrocyte apoptosis in HIV-associated dementia. Ann Neurol 2001;49:745–52.
[24] Rottenberg DA, Moeller JR, Strother SC, et al. The metabolic pathology of the AIDS
dementia complex. Ann Neurol 1987;22:700–6.
[25] Aylward EH, Henderer JD, McArthur JC, et al. Reduced basal ganglia volume in HIV-1-
associated dementia: results from quantitative neuroimaging. Neurology 1993;43:2099–104.
[26] Takahashi K, Wesselingh SL, Griffin DE, et al. Localization of HIV-1 in human brain
using polymerase chain reaction/in situ hybridization and immunocytochemistry. Ann
Neurol 1996;39:705–11.
[27] McClernon DR, Lanier R, Gartner S, et al. HIV in the brain—RNA levels and patterns of
zidovudine resistance. Neurology 2001;57:1396–401.
[28] Brenneman DE, Westbrook GL, Fitzgerald SP, et al. Neuronal cell killing by the envelope
protein of HIV and its prevention by vasoactive intestinal peptide. Nature 1988;335:639–42.
D.B. Clifford / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 537–550 549
[29] Magnuson DSK, Knudsen BE, Geiger JD, et al. Human immunodeficiency virus type 1 Tat
activates non-N-methyl-D-aspartate excitatory amino acid receptors and causes neuro-
toxicity. Ann Neurol 1995;37:373–80.
[30] Nath A, Geiger J. Neurobiological aspects of human immunodeficiency virus infection:
neurotoxic mechanisms. Prog Neurobiol 1998;54:19–33.
[31] Rappaport J, Joseph J, Croul S, et al. Molecular pathway involved in HIV-1-induced CNS
pathology: role of viral regulatory protein, Tat. J Leukoc Biol 1999;65:458–65.
[32] Liu Y, Jones M, Hingtgen CM, et al. Uptake of HIV-1 Tat protein mediated by low-density
lipoprotein receptor-related protein disrupts the neuronal metabolic balance of the receptor
ligands. Nat Med 2000;6:1380–7.
[33] Wesselingh SL, Power C, Glass JD, et al. Intracerebral cytokine messenger RNA ex-
pression in acquired immunodeficiency syndrome dementia. Ann Neurol 1993;33:576–82.
[34] Brew BJ, Bhalla RB, Paul M, et al. Cerebrospinal fluid b2-microglobulin in patients with
AIDS dementia complex: an expanded series including response to zidovudine treatment.
AIDS 1992;6:461–5.
[35] McArthur JC, Nance-Sproson TE, Griffin DE, et al. The diagnostic utility of elevation in
cerebrospinal fluid b2-microglobulin in HIV-1 dementia. Neurology 1992;42:1707–12.
[36] Brew BJ, Bhalla RB, Paul M, et al. Cerebrospinal fluid neopterin in human immuno-
deficiency virus type 1 infection. Ann Neurol 1990;28:556–60.
[37] Griffin DE. Perspectives series: cytokines and the brain. J Clin Invest 1997;100:2948–51.
[38] Heyes MP, Brew BJ, Martin A, et al. Quinolinic acid in cerebrospinal fluid and serum
in HIV-1 infection: relationship to clinical and neurological status. Ann Neurol 1991;29:
202–9.
[39] Pulliam L, Gascon R, Stubblebine M, et al. Unique monocyte subset in patients with AIDS
dementia. Lancet 1997;349:692–5.
[40] Gartner S. AIDS: HIV infection and dementia. Science 2000;287:602–4.
[41] Pomara N, Crandall DT, Choi SJ, et al. White matter abnormalities in HIV-1 infection:
a diffusion tensor imaging study. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging 2001;106:15–24.
[42] Arribas JR, Storch GA, Clifford DB, et al. Cytomegalovirus encephalitis. Ann Intern Med
1996;125:577–87.
[43] Ellis RJ, Hsia K, Spector SA, et al. Cerebrospinal fluid human immunodeficiency virus
type 1 RNA levels are elevated in neurocognitively impaired individuals with acquired
immunodeficiency syndrome. Ann Neurol 1997;42:679–88.
[44] Sabri F, DeMilito A, Pirskanen R, et al. Elevated levels of soluble fas and fas ligand
in cerebrospinal fluid of patients with AIDS dementia complex. J Neuroimmunol 2001;114:
197–206.
[45] Chang L, Ernst T, Leonido-Yee M, et al. Cerebral metabolite abnormalities correlate with
clinical severity of HIV-1 cognitive motor complex. Neurology 1999;52:100–8.
[46] Chang L, Ernst T, Leonido-Yee M, et al. Highly active antiretroviral therapy reverses brain
metabolite abnormalities in mild HIV dementia. Neurology 1999;53:782–9.
[47] Meyerhoff DJ, Bloomer C, Cardenas V, et al. Elevated subcortical choline metabolites in
cognitively and clinically asymptomatic HIV+ patients. Neurology 1999;52:995–1003.
[48] Tracey I, Carr CA, Guimaraes AR, et al. Brain choline-containing compounds are elevated
in HIV-positive patients before the onset of AIDS dementia complex: a proton magnetic
resonance spectroscopic study. Neurology 1996;46:783–8.
[49] Chang L, Ernst T, Leonido-Yee M, et al. Perfusion MRI detects rCBF abnormalities in
early stages of HIV-cognitive motor complex. Neurology 2000;54:389–96.
[50] Staszewski S, Keiser P, Montaner J, et al. Abacavir-lamivudine-zidovudine vs indinavir-
lamivudine-zidovudine in antiretroviral-naive HIV-infected adults: a randomized equiva-
lence trial. JAMA 2001;285:1155–63.
[51] Staszewski S, Morales-Ramirez J, Tashima K, et al. Efavirenz plus zidovudine and
lamivudine, efavirenz plus indinavir, and indinavir plus zidovudine and lamivudine in the
treatment of HIV-1 infection in adults. N Engl J Med 1999;341:1865–73.
550 D.B. Clifford / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 537–550
[52] Foudraine NA, Hoetelmans RMW, Lange JMA, et al. Cerebrospinal-fluid HIV-1 RNA
and drug concentrations after treatment with lamivudine plus zidovudine or stavudine.
Lancet 1998;351:1547–51.
[53] Martin C, Sönnerborg A, Svensson JO, et al. Indinavir-based treatment of HIV-1 infected
patients: efficacy in the central nervous system. AIDS 2000;13:1227–32.
[54] Glynn SL, Yazdanian M. In vitro blood-brain barrier permeability of nevirapine compared
to other HIV antiretroviral agents. J Pharm Sci 1998;87:306–10.
[55] Tashima K, Caliendo AM, Ahmad M, et al. Cerebrospinal fluid human immunodeficiency
virus type 1 (HIV-1) suppression and efavirenz drug concentrations in HIV-1-infected
patients receiving combination therapy. J Infect Dis 1999;180:862–4.
[56] Sidtis JJ, Gatsonis C, Price RW, et al. Zidovudine treatment of the AIDS dementia com-
plex: results of a placebo-controlled trial. Ann Neurol 1993;33:343–9.
[57] Lanier ER, Sturge G, McClernon D, et al. HIV-1 reverse transcriptase sequence in plasma
and cerebrospinal fluid of patients with AIDS dementia complex treated with Abacavir.
AIDS 2001;15:747–51.
[58] Dreyer EB, Kaiser PK, Offermann JT, et al. HIV-1 coat protein neurotoxicity prevented by
calcium channel antagonists. Science 1990;248:364–7.
[59] Lipton SA. Neuronal injury associated with HIV-1: approaches to treatment. Annu Rev
Pharmacol Toxicol 1998;38:159–77.
[60] Lipton SA, Gendelman HE. Dementia associated with the acquired immunodeficiency syn-
drome. N Engl J Med 1999;332:934–40.
[61] Navia BA, Dafni U, Simpson D, et al. A phase I/II trial of nimodipine for HIV-related
neurologic complications. Neurology 1998;51:221–8.
[62] Schifitto G, Sacktor N, Marder K, et al. Randomized, placebo-controlled trial of the PAF
antagonist lexipafant in HIV-associated cognitive impairment. Neurology 1999;53:391–96.
[63] Heseltine PNR, Goodkin K, Atkinson JH, et al. Randomized double-blind placebo-
controlled trial of peptide T for HIV-associated cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol 1998;
55:41–51.
[64] Sacktor N, Schifitto G, McDermott MP, et al. Transdermal selegiline in HIV-associated
cognitive impairment: pilot, placebo-controlled study. Neurology 2000;54:233–5.
[65] The Dana Consortium on the Therapy of HIV Dementia and Related Cognitive Disorders.
A randomize double-blind, placebo-controlled trial of deprenyl and thioctic acid in human
immunodeficiency virus-associated cognitive impairment. Neurology 1998;50:645–51.
[66] Clifford D, McArthur JC, Schifitto G, Cohen B, Ellis R, Marder K, et al. Phase II trial of
CPI-1189 for HIV-associated motor cognitive impairment [abstract]. In: Proceedings of the
Eighth Conference on Retroviruses and Opportunistic Infections. Chicago: Foundation for
Retrovirology of Human Health; 2001. p. 224.
[67] Navia BA, Yiannoutsos CT, Change L, Marra CM, Miller E, Nath A, et al. for the ACTG
301 Protocol Team. ACTG 301: a phase II randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled
trial of memantine for AIDS dementia complex (ADC) [abstract]. In: Proceedings of the
American Academy of Neurology 53rd Annual Meeting. Philadelphia: JB hippincott; 2001.
p. A474–5.
Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 551–571
* Corresponding author.
E-mail address: [email protected] (M.-S. Sy).
0025-7125/02/$ - see front matter Ó 2002, Elsevier Science (USA). All rights reserved.
PII: S 0 0 2 5 - 7 1 2 5 ( 0 2 ) 0 0 0 0 4 - 4
552 M.-S. Sy et al / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 551–571
familial form, as in GSS and FFI cases and some CJD cases; the infectious
form, as in kuru and iatrogenic CJD; and the sporadic form, as in 85% of
human TSE cases, which occurs sporadically via unknown mechanisms
(Table 1). In animals, major TSE diseases include scrapie in sheep and goats,
transmissible mink encephalopathy in mink, bovine spongiform encephalop-
athy (BSE) in cattle, and chronic wasting disease (CWD) in deer and elks (see
Table 1). TSE has also been found in captive animals in the zoo as well as in
domestic cats.
A major breakthrough in our understanding of these diseases was the ser-
endipitous discovery of histopathologic similarities between the brains of
kuru patients and the brains of sheep and goats with scrapie, a TSE disease
[1]. Scrapie has been known in the United Kingdom for more than 200 years.
It was commonly thought to be a genetic disease [7], that is, some herds of
sheep were susceptible, whereas others were resistant. In 1936, however,
Cuille and Chelle demonstrated that scrapie was a transmissible disease [8].
Based on this lead, Gibbs and Gajdusek demonstrated the transmissibility of
kuru and then of CJD in the 1960s, followed by that of GSS in 1981, from
human beings to primates [1,3]. Experimental transmission was most effi-
ciently achieved by injecting homogenates into the brain of recipient ani-
mals. The homogenates were prepared using brains from affected subjects.
Subsequent studies revealed that the natural transmission of kuru most
likely occurred through cannibalism, which was practiced as part of the
funereal ceremony common to the Fore linguistic group [1]. The incubation
period of kuru could be as long as four to five decades [1]. Cessation of
Table 1
Transmissible spongiform encephalopathy diseases in human beings and animals
Form Phenotype
Human beings
Sporadic Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease
Fatal familial insomnia
Inherited Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease
Fatal familial insomnia
Gerstmann-Sträussler-Scheinker disease
Undefined or mixed
Acquired by infection ‘‘Iatrogenic’’ Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease (tissue grafts,
surgical instruments, hormones)
Kuru (contaminated food)
Variant Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease (contaminated food?)
Animals
Probably all acquired Scrapie
by infection Bovine spongiform encephalopathy
Chronic wasting disease
Transmissible mink encephalopathy
Transmissible spongiform encephalopathy
of domestic and captive animals
M.-S. Sy et al / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 551–571 553
cannibalism since the 1950s has gradually eliminated the disease. Like other
infectious diseases caused by microbes or viruses, kuru can be acquired exo-
genously, specifically by the ingestion of contaminated foods. The infectious
agent spreads from the digestive tract to the CNS, a process known as neu-
roinvasion. The mechanisms of neuroinvasion are not clear. It is believed
that the infectious agent can travel from the peripheral to the CNS either via
the peripheral nervous system or via the circulatory system [3].
Scrapie has a 200-year history in the United Kingdom and is endemic [7].
No transmission of scrapie to human beings has yet been reported, however.
In the past, experimental animal models showed that the infectious agent is
highly species specific [3]. Hence, transmission of scrapie was thought to be
impossible because of the existence of this ‘‘species barrier’’ between the
agent and the host. It is now clear that this species barrier can be violated
in experimental animals, and it can also be penetrated outside the laboratory
[8]. The barrier is not permanent, because the agent of TSE can cross the
species barrier after repeat passages in the host.
Recently, a novel form of CJD called variant CJD (vCJD) that is believed
to be acquired from cattle affected by BSE (commonly known as mad cow
disease) has emerged in the United Kingdom [9]. The development of BSE in
England in the 1980s has been traced to the consumption of meat and bone
meal fed to cattle [9]. This dietary supplement was contaminated by scrapie-
infected ovine. It became infectious in the 1980s when rendering plants aban-
doned the use of organic solvents that inactivated the infectious agent. By
jumping the species barrier, the infectious agent has transmitted scrapie from
sheep to cattle and from cattle to human beings. Dietary supplement contami-
nated with scrapie-infected ovine is also the culprit for transmissible mink
encephalopathy in mink and for most of the TSE cases in captive animals
in the zoo. Despite the relative rarity of the cattle-to-human transmission
route, TSE diseases have captured considerable attention because of their
unique pathogenic mechanism and their potential threat to public health.
Overall, TSE diseases are rare in human beings. The annual rate is
approximately 1.5 per 1 million people per year, a rate that has remained
stable over the last few decades [2,10]. Many case-control studies have
attempted to identify the risk factors in these diseases [8]. So far, there is
no conclusive evidence to suggest that the frequency of the disease is linked
to gender, occupation, geographic location, or environmental factors. The
exception occurs in cases where there is a familial link between affected indi-
viduals. These cases represent the familial form of the TSE diseases.
All patients with sporadic TSE diseases suffer from cognitive deficits,
including psychiatric and behavior abnormalities. For sporadic diseases, the
onset of disease most frequently occurs between the fifth and seventh de-
cades of life. Clinical laboratory results reveal no evidence of inflammation,
abnormal blood chemistry, or malfunctions of hepatic or renal systems.
Immune responses also have not been detected in affected individuals. The
mean survival time for patients with sporadic diseases is approximately
554 M.-S. Sy et al / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 551–571
5 months, and most patients die within 1 year. The inherited form of the dis-
eases has an earlier age of onset and a more protracted disease course than
the sporadic form [2].
At variance with other neurodegenerative diseases, human TSE diseases
have been acquired via infectious mechanisms, as most vividly demonstrated
by kuru. Physicians have also inadvertently transmitted the disease to pa-
tients as a result of medical procedures. These cases are commonly referred
to as iatrogenic CJD [2]. Human transmission was most convincingly
demonstrated when TSE disease developed in two young patients who
underwent stereotactic electroencephalographic exploration with silver elec-
trodes. The electrodes had previously been implanted in a patient with TSE
disease [2]. The electrodes were ‘‘sterilized’’ with 70% alcohol and formalde-
hyde vapor, which was sufficient to eliminate microbes and viruses but did
not destroy the TSE agent. Furthermore, more than 80 cases of iatrogenic
diseases have been reported after neurologic transplants of human cadaver-
derived dura mater. More than 100 cases of iatrogenic diseases have also been
linked to injection of cadaver-derived growth hormone and gonadotropic
hormone [2]. It is presumed that the dura maters and the hormones were
harvested from donors who might have died of unrecognized TSE diseases.
More recently, a cluster of seven TSE cases surfaced in a small village
with a population of approximately 70,000 in Australia (C.L. Masters, per-
sonal communication, 2001). All seven patients are unrelated, and the com-
mon feature of the group is that three of the seven patients have visited the
same ophthalmologist. It is clear that TSE can be transmitted by invasive med-
ical procedures under some conditions. Nevertheless, based on the number of
surgical procedures that have been carried out and the number of iatrogenic
TSE cases that have been reported, the risk of contracting TSE as a result of
surgery must be low.
The onset, frequency, and duration of the iatrogenic form of the diseases
are much more variable than those of either the sporadic form or the inher-
ited form. The incubation period for iatrogenic diseases ranges from
6 months to 19 years. These variations reflect differences in the levels of
exposure as well as in the route of infection. Intracranial infections, as in the
cases of electrode implantation or dura mater transplants, have a shorter
incubation period. Extracranial infections, as in the cases of growth hor-
mone injection, tend to have a much longer incubation period. Similar to
patients with sporadic disease, patients with iatrogenic disease develop
ataxia and movement disorders followed by dementia.
Human TSE is clearly transmissible, but the rate of transmission from
human beings to primates varies from disease to disease. Based on studies
carried out over a span of 30 years involving 300 cases of human TSE and
close to 2000 primates, Brown and colleagues [11] found that the transmis-
sion rates were higher for iatrogenic CJD (100%), kuru (95%), and sporadic
CJD (sCJD) (90%) and lower for the familial form of diseases (68%). Trans-
mission best correlated with the presence of spongiform pathologic findings
M.-S. Sy et al / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 551–571 555
a self-replicating and infectious agent that lacks nucleic acid. Although the
conversion hypothesis provides a mechanism for the propagation of PrPSc,
the mechanisms by which PrPSc causes neurodegeneration remain unclear.
In addition to the CNS, PrPC is expressed at high levels in many non-CNS
tissues, including the heart, lymphoid tissues, and muscle [3]. In all human
prion diseases, with the exception of vCJD, no PrPSc has ever been detected
in non-CNS tissues. Therefore, the brain is the only site where the conver-
sion from PrPC to PrPSc could occur. Alternatively, conversion may occur
in other tissues, but only the CNS has the capacity to accumulate PrPSc. The
mechanism by which PrPSc targets the CNS is not known.
Two different lines of evidence provide the strongest support for the prion
concept. The first line of evidence came from studies in transgenic animals.
PrPC knockout mice that do not express PrPC are resistant to infection [14].
Therefore, the presence of the PrPC is essential for PrPSc propagation. The sec-
ond line of evidence came from clinical studies in human beings. It has been
known for decades that some human spongiform diseases, such as GSS and,
more recently, FFI, can be inherited in an autosomal dominant fashion [2].
The identification of the prion gene suddenly permits investigators to deter-
mine whether any of the inherited forms of human prion disease are linked
to the PrP gene. Indeed, more than 20 pathogenic mutations have been found
in the coding sequence of the PrP gene in patients with the inherited form of the
diseases [3]. Pathogenic mutation represents approximately 15% of the human
prion diseases. Pathogenic mutations are broadly divided into those that cause
point mutations and those that cause insertion mutations in the coding region.
Whereas it is generally accepted that normal cellular PrPC is critical for
the pathogenesis of prion diseases, it should be noted that the prion concept
or the ‘‘protein-only hypothesis’’ is not universally accepted. Some skeptical
investigators still question whether prion by itself is sufficient for the patho-
genesis of the diseases [15].
(Fig. 2). The N-link glycans are complex, as exemplified by the murine
PrPSc, which has over 60 different carbohydrate structures [17]. This variety
indicates that the glycans play a complex role in the physiology of PrPC and
may be important in the pathogenesis of the diseases [18].
Nuclear magnetic resonance analysis of recombinant PrPC shows that the
N-terminus region is highly flexible. It also shows that the central region is
partially structured and hydrophobic. In contrast, the C-terminus region
includes three a-helices and two short antiparallel b-strands, which form a
well-structured core domain when combined (see Fig. 1) [19,20]. PrPC knock-
out mice are normal; thus, expression of PrPC is not obligatory for survival
in mice [21]. Recent in vitro and in vivo studies suggest that PrPC is a metal-
binding protein and may be important in oxidative stress responses [22].
Fig. 2. Ribbon models of the tertiary structures of hamster cellular prion protein (PrPC) (A)
and scrapie prion protein (PrPSc) (B) based on nuclear magnetic resonance studies of PrP108–
231 and major biochemical differences between PrPC and PrPSc. The PrPC-to-PrPSc conversion
is based on the change in conformation from the predominantly a-helical form of PrPC (light
ribbons) to the predominantly b-sheet structure of PrPSc (dark ribbons). (From Prusiner SB. An
introduction to prion biology and diseases. In: Prusiner SB, editor. Prion biology and diseases.
Cold Spring, NY: Cold Spring Harbor Press; 1999. p. 1–68; with permission.)
is believed to occur first in the plasma membrane and then in the endosome
[24]. The presence of the GPI anchor is important for the conversion pro-
cesses to occur [16]. The GPI anchor may be needed, because GPI-anchored
proteins occupy microdomains on the cell membrane known as detergent-
insoluble complex in a concentrated and multimeric form, and the deter-
gent-insoluble complex may facilitate the initial interaction [25]. Disulfide
bonding is also important in the conversion process [26] and seems to be
inversely related to the glycosylation in PrPC [18]. Extensive cell model stud
ies support this interpretation [27]. Indirect evidence suggests that the con-
version process requires PrPSc to interact with PrPC either directly or through
the ‘‘help’’ of protein X or chaperones [28]. To date, a molecular chaperone
has not been identified. Conversion of PrPC by exogenous PrPSc has been
demonstrated in vitro in a cell-free system [29]; however, the process is ineffi-
cient because of the absence of cofactors or the chaperone protein [30].
The conformational changes in PrPSc also result in drastic alterations in
its biochemical properties. Some of these changes are summarized in Table 2.
One of the consequences of the conformational change is that although the
N-terminal region of PrPSc remains sensitive to treatment with proteases,
the C-terminal region from residues 80 to 231, also known as PrP27–30,
becomes protease resistant. In contrast, the entire PrPC is protease sensitive.
The accumulation in the brain of PrPSc that is resistant to proteases has
M.-S. Sy et al / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 551–571 559
Table 2
Differences between normal human cellular prion protein and scrapie prion protein
PrPC PrPSc
Monomer Oligomeric or polymeric
More a-helix More b-sheet
Relatively more soluble in detergent Less soluble in detergent
Sensitive to in vitro digestion with proteinase K Relatively more resistant to proteinase K
Noninfectious Infectious
Present in CNS and many non-CNS tissues Only in CNS
Abbreviations: PrPC, normal human cellular prion protein; PrPSc, scrapie prion protein;
CNS, central nervous system.
provided a useful diagnostic test for prion diseases [6,31]. The commonly
used protease is proteinase K (PK) (Fig. 3). Resistance to PK is the only
in vitro test by which one can distinguish PrPSc from PrPC. Sensitivity to
PK in vitro is relative rather than absolute, and the levels of PK-resistant
PrPSc vary greatly between different prion diseases. In a small number of
cases, no PK-resistant PrPSc was detected [32]. Therefore, the presence of PK-
resistant PrPSc might not be required for pathogenesis in some situations.
Brown and colleagues [11] have also evaluated the relation between trans-
missibility of diseases and the presence of PK-resistant PrPSc in human
Fig. 3. Resistance of scrapie prion protein (PrPSc) but not cellular prion protein (PrPC) to
proteinase K (PK) digestion in vitro. Brain homogenates were prepared from two non-
Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease (CJD) cases and two confirmed CJD cases. Each sample was divided
into two tubes: one sample was either not treated (lanes 1 and 2 from non-CJD, lanes 5 and
6 from CJD) or treated with PK (lanes 3 and 4 from non-CJD, lanes 7 and 8 from CJD). After
treatment, all samples were separated by sodium dodecyl sulfate polyacrylamide gel elec-
trophoresis and immunoblotted with an anti-PrPC monoclonal antibody as described [47]. No
immunoreactivity is detected in non-CJD samples after PK digestion (lanes 3 and 4). In
contrast, strong immunoreactivity remained detectable after PK treatment of samples from
CJD cases (lanes 7 and 8) (unpublished results) [47].
560 M.-S. Sy et al / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 551–571
attachment of the GPI anchors to the protein and is absent in the mature
protein. Collectively, these results suggest that there are multiple pathways
in the pathogenesis of the inherited form of human prion diseases.
Sixty percent of pathogenic mutations found in human prion diseases
involve residue 200, which results in a change from glutamine (E) to lysine (K).
Microsatellite analysis searching for E200K mutation–associated haplotypes
reveals that this mutation is derived from two families, one in Spain and the
other in Slovakia. All E200K cases reported in the United States and Canada
are descendants of the Slovakia family, whereas the Spanish family is the
origin of the cases in Europe (L.G. Goldfarb, personal communication, 2001).
Conversely, insertion mutations have only been found in the octapeptide
repeat region [33]. The number of insertions ranges from one to nine octa-
peptide repeats. It is interesting to note that although insertion mutations in
the octapeptide repeat region cause diseases, deletion mutation in the same
region does not. As a matter of fact, a single octapeptide repeat deletion is a
polymorphism in 1% to 2.5% of the population [2]. Also noteworthy, trans-
genic mice lacking the N-terminus are susceptible to infectious PrPSc, albeit
at a reduced efficiency [34].
One of the most intriguing findings regarding the inherited form of prion
diseases comes from studies revealing that a common polymorphism in amino-
acid residue 129 of the human PrP gene can greatly modulate disease
phenotypes [4]. An individual can be homozygous with methionine (M/M)
or valine (V/V) or heterozygous (M/V). Two groups of patients were identi-
fied with an identical mutation in residue 178 with a change from aspartic
acid to asparagine. The clinical presentations and histopathologies of the
two groups differed dramatically, however. In the group with the 129 V/V
genotype, the phenotype was more consistent with CJD, with extensive
spongiosis, neuronal loss, and astrogliosis in the cerebral cortex region.
Lesions were less severe in the thalamus. In the group with the 129 M/M
genotype, the phenotype was that of FFI, with preferential thalamic and oli-
vary atrophy. These findings show that a difference in one amino acid can
drastically alter disease phenotype. The mechanisms by which residue 129
influences the disease phenotype are not clear.
Experimental mutations of the PrP gene in some transgenic mice have led
to neurodegeneration in the absence of detectable PrPSc [35]. It is unclear
whether PrPSc is solely responsible for all aspects of the pathogenesis and
whether all hereditary prion diseases share the same pathogenic mechanism.
It should be noted that another transgenic mouse line bearing the same
human pathogenic mutation failed to develop neurodegeneration [36]. The
reason for this discrepancy is not known. These results suggest that a muta-
tion in the PrP gene may not be sufficient to cause neurodegeneration and
that other factors unique to human brain may be required for the manifes-
tation of the disease.
Sporadic prion diseases are thought to be triggered by a rare and stochas-
tic change of the PrPC conformation, which leads to the formation of PrPSc.
562 M.-S. Sy et al / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 551–571
Even so, one cannot rule out the possibility that some sporadic prion diseases
may be caused by non-germline mutations or by unrecognized infectious
mechanisms. Interestingly, the 129 polymorphism also influences the incidence
of sCJD in Caucasian patients. Most of the sCJD patients are homozygous
with M/M or V/V [10]. In the general population, the frequency of M/M is
37%, V/V represents 12% of cases, and the remaining 51% of cases are hetero-
zygous (Table 3). Accumulated results suggest that homozygosity at residue
129 predisposes an individual to prion diseases. Homozygosity presumably
provides more PrPC substrates with similar conformation for conversion.
In contrast, heterozygous individuals have two different PrPCs with different
conformations. If this interpretation is correct, it suggests that residue 129 is
important either directly or indirectly in the overall conformation of PrPC.
Prion diseases contracted by an infectious mechanism are believed to
result from exposure to exogenous PrPSc, which binds to endogenous PrPC,
causing its conversion to PrPSc [3]. Accumulated evidence in animal models
suggests that the successful transmission of a prion disease depends on sev-
eral factors: (1) the degree of homology between the exogenous PrPSc and
the PrPC of the recipient, which may be necessary for the initial binding;
(2) the dose of the PrPSc; and (3) the expression of PrPC at the portal entry
of the exogenous PrPSc as well as in the various intermediate stations
between the portal entry and the CNS of the recipient. The mechanism by
which the PrPSc travels from the peripheral to the CNS is not clear. Some
investigators believe that the peripheral nervous system is the route of PrPSc
entry into the CNS. Other investigators believe that blood cells are involved
in the process. Once the initial conversion occurs, it is the endogenous PrPSc
that propagates and accumulates, causing the disease. Interestingly, all vCJD
cases so far were from individuals homozygous with M/M at residue 129 [9].
Either individuals with M/M are uniquely susceptible to vCJD or other geno-
types are also susceptible but with a much longer incubation period.
Table 3
Polymorphism at amino-acid residue 129 and incidences of sporadic Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease
129 Genotype M/M M/V V/V
Normal 37% 51% 12%
Sporadic 71.6% 11.7% 16.7%
Abbreviations: M, methionine; V, valine.
Data from Parchi P, Giese A, Capellari S, Brown P, Schulz-Schaeffer W, Windl O, et al.
Classification of sporadic Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease based on molecular and phenotypic
analysis of 300 subjects. Ann Neurol 1999;46:224–33; with permission.
M.-S. Sy et al / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 551–571 563
10 years in vCJD. Since 1996, close to 100 confirmed cases of vCJD have
been reported, most of which occurred in the United Kingdom with a few
exceptions [9]. The origin of the infection in vCJD has not been firmly es-
tablished, but considerable evidence points to BSE [37]. Four major lines
of evidence associate vCJD with BSE: (1) the outbreak of vCJD with a time
delay from and in the same geographic location as the BSE epidemic; (2) the
distinct clinical features of vCJD, which set this disease apart from conven-
tional CJD; (3) the similar histopathologic lesion profile in mice inoculated
with either BSE or vCJD preparations; and (4) the similar ‘‘molecular signa-
tures’’ shared between BSE and vCJD.
The histopathologic changes seen in the vCJD cases are reminiscent of
those seen in kuru, suggesting that similar pathologic mechanisms are
involved in these two diseases (ie, consumption of contaminated meat or
meat products). Early on, it was recognized that the clinical signs, age at
onset of the disease, and CNS pathologic findings seen in patients with vCJD
differed greatly from those seen in patients with conventional CJD. The un-
usual clinical signs of vCJD patients are prominent behavior changes at the
time of clinical presentation, followed by ataxia, dementia, and myoclonus
at the terminal stage of disease. In addition, PK-resistant PrPSc is detectable
in non-CNS tissues (eg, tonsils, appendix) of vCJD patients but not in
patients with conventional CJD [8,38]. Interestingly, PK-resistant PrPSc is
detectable in tonsil before the manifestation of clinical signs. Some of the dif-
ferences between vCJD and conventional CJD are summarized in Table 4.
No vCJD has been reported in the United States; however, four cases of
prion disease in unusually young patients have been reported [39]. At least
three of these patients (whose tissue has been examined by us) had regularly
consumed deer or elk meat. Available evidence does not indicate that the
prion disease was acquired via exposure to CWD through the consumption
of contaminated deer or elk meat, however. CWD was first recognized as
a prion disease in deer and elks in 1980. CWD is endemic in north central
Colorado and southeastern Wyoming.
Table 4
Differences between sporadic Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease and variant Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease
sCJD vCJD
Residue 129 genotype M/M, M/V, or V/V M/M only
Disease onset (mean age) 60 years old 29 years old
Mean duration of disease 5 months 14 months
CNS histopathology No ‘‘daisy’’ plaques Plaques with
‘‘daisy’’-like feature
PrPSc type Type 1 or 2 Type 2 only
PrPSc outside of CNS None In tonsil and appendix
Abbreviations: sCJD, sporadic Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease; vCJD, variant Creutzfeldt-Jakob
disease; M, methionine; V, valine; CNS, central nervous system; PrPC, normal human cellular
prion protein; PrPSc, scrapie prion protein.
564 M.-S. Sy et al / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 551–571
Fig. 5. Molecular typing of human prion stains. (A) Brain lysates were prepared and separated
by sodium dodecyl sulfate polyacrylamide gel electrophoresis. Separated proteins were then
transferred onto nitrocellulose membranes and blotted with anti-prion protein (PrP) mono-
clonal antibody 8H4 as described [47]. PrP from four of the sporadic Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease
cases was indistinguishable from that of the control sample. (B) Aliquots of lysates were treated
with 50 lg/mL of proteinase K (PK), and immunoblots were carried out as described. After PK
treatment, the unglycosylated form of scrapie prion protein type 1 migrated at approximately 21
kd, whereas that of type 2 migrated at approximately 19 kd. (From Wong BS, Chen SG, Colucci
M, Xie Z, Li R, Pan T, et al. Aberrant metal binding by prion protein in human prion disease.
J Neurochem, in press; with permission.)
M.-S. Sy et al / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 551–571 565
Table 5
Preferential association of type 1 diseases with methionine/methionine homozygous and Type 2
diseases with valine/valine homozygous
129 genotype M/M M/V V/V
Type 1 94.9% 3.7% 1.4%
Type 2 14.0% 31.4% 54.6%
Abbreviations: M, methionine; V, valine.
Data from Parchi P, Giese A, Capellari S, Brown P, Schulz-Schaeffer W, Windl O, et al.
Classification of sporadic Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease based on molecular and phenotypic anal-
ysis of 300 subjects. Ann Neurol 1999;46:224–33; with permission.
566 M.-S. Sy et al / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 551–571
Table 6
Some of the clinical features at presentation and immunohistochemical staining patterns of the
five subgroups of sporadic Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease and sporadic fatal familial insomnia cases
based on 129 genotype and type of scrapie prion protein
electron transport chain. Because of their highly reactive nature, ROS are
potentially quite toxic to cells. They can readily combine with other mole-
cules, such as proteins, lipids, and nucleic acids. Attack by ROS can result
in breakage of proteins and nucleic acids. ROS can also chemically modify
proteins and lipids, which results in the loss of their normal functions. In
normal conditions, two families of molecules tightly regulate the levels of
ROS: the pro-oxidants and the antioxidants. Overproduction of ROS may
be attributed to either an increase in pro-oxidant activity or a decrease in anti-
oxidant availability. One of the most important antioxidants is superoxide-
dismutase (SOD). This family of enzymes converts damaging superoxide
anion free radicals to H2O.
Both recombinant PrP and brain-derived PrP have been reported to have
SOD-like activity when they carry bound copper (Cu) [42]. In contrast, this
SOD-like activity is lost when Cu is replaced with manganese (Mn) [43].
Protein oxidation and lipid peroxidation are two of the best-characterized
markers of oxidative stress. Recently, we investigated whether these markers
are detectable in sporadic human prion diseases. We found that the levels of
protein oxidation and lipid peroxidation were greatly increased in the brain
of all sCJD cases [44]. More interestingly, we observed a striking elevation
of Mn accompanied by a significant reduction of Cu bound to PrP in all
M.-S. Sy et al / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 551–571 567
sCJD patients. These metal imbalances varied according to the PrP 129
genotype and PrPSc type [44]. Cu and Mn changes were pronounced in sCJD
subjects homozygous for methionine at codon 129 and carrying PrPSc type 1
(sCJD M/M1). Furthermore, correlating with the increases in the markers of
oxidative stress, the PrP-associated SOD-like activity was reduced by
approximately 85% in each of the sCJD variants. The loss of antioxidant
function associated with increased oxidative stress suggests a change in the
metal-ion occupancy of PrP. This change might play a pivotal role in
the pathogenesis of prion diseases. Because the metal change is different
in the sCJD variants, it may also contribute to the diversity of PrPSc and dis-
ease phenotype in sCJD [44]. Whether alteration in metal binding is the
cause of the diseases or the consequence of the diseases remains to be deter-
mined. Recent studies in an animal model suggest that alterations in metal
binding can be observed in experimentally infected mice at a presymptom-
atic stage (Wong et al, manuscript submitted).
Future challenges
Like all other human diseases, the challenge for human prion diseases is
in the diagnosis, cure, and eventual prevention of the diseases. Of these three
challenges, the development of a more reliable and rapid diagnostic test is
the easiest task. Since the appearance of vCJD, significant progress has been
made in the diagnosis of prion diseases. Conversely, like all other neurode-
generative diseases, finding a cure for prion diseases is likely to be difficult.
This is because most patients with sCJD do not have obvious clinical signs
until they are close to the terminal stage of the diseases. Many attempts have
been made to identify compounds that might interfere with the PrPC-to-
PrPSc conversion in in vitro and in vivo experimental models. Only time can
tell whether any of these approaches reaches clinical application.
To find a cure and prevent human prion diseases, we must focus on a bet-
ter understanding of the molecular mechanisms that are responsible for the
diseases. Human prion diseases belong to a group of heterogeneous diseases.
Identification of the infectious agent is only the first step in a long and ardu-
ous process. We must endeavor to unravel the mechanisms by which this
Table 7
Cases of human prion diseases received and diagnosed by the National Prion Diseases
Surveillance Center at Case Western Reserve University
Human prior diseases
Year Sporadic Familiar Iatrogenic Total
1997 54 4 0 58
1998 45 5 1 51
1999 65 9 0 74
2000 97 12 2 111
2001 (3/1) 31 1 0 32
M.-S. Sy et al / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 551–571 569
small protein causes diseases. The control and containment of prion diseases
is likely to remain a major challenge for scientists, clinicians, and public
health officials in the days ahead.
Acknowledgment
Dr. Sy dedicates this manuscript to Pearl Ling.
References
[1] Gajdusek DC. Unconventional viruses and the origin and disappearance of kuru. Science
1977;197:943–60.
[2] Parchi P, Gambetti P. Human prion diseases. Curr Opin Neurol 1995;8:286–93.
[3] Prusiner SB. Prions. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1998;95:13363–83.
[4] Goldfarb LG, Petersen RB, Tabaton M, Brown P, LeBlanc AC, Montagna P, et al. Fatal
familial insomnia and familial Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease: disease phenotype determined by
a DNA polymorphism. Science 1992;258:806–8.
[5] Parchi P, Capellari S, Chen SG, Petersen RB, Gambetti P, Kopp N, et al. Typing prion
isoforms. Nature 1997;386:232–4.
[6] Parchi P, Zou W, Wang W, Brown P, Capellari S, Ghetti B, et al. Genetic influence on the
structural variations of the abnormal prion protein. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2000;97:
10168–72.
[7] Woolhouse ME, Coen P, Matthews L, Foster JD, Elsen J, Lewis RM, et al. A centuries-
long epidemic of scrapie in British sheep? Trends Microbiol 2001;9:67–70.
[8] Culle J, Chelle P-L. La maladie dite ‘‘tremblante du mouton’’: est-elle inoculable? CR Acad
Sci 1936;203:1552–54.
[9] Brown P, Will RG, Bradley R, Asher DM, Detwiler L. Bovine spongiform encephalopathy
and variant Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease: background, evolution, and current concerns.
Emerg Infect Dis 2001;7:6–16.
[10] Parchi P, Giese A, Capellari S, Brown P, Schulz-Schaeffer W, Windl O, et al. Classification
of sporadic Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease based on molecular and phenotypic analysis of 300
subjects. Ann Neurol 1999;46:224–33.
[11] Brown P, Gibbs CJ Jr, Rodgers-Johnson P, Asher DM, Sulima MP, Bacote A, et al.
Human spongiform encephalopathy: the National Institutes of Health series of 300 cases of
experimentally transmitted disease. Ann Neurol 1994;35:513–29.
[12] Griffith JS. Self-replication and scrapie. Nature 1967;215:1043–4.
[13] Basler K, Oesch B, Scott M, Westaway D, Walchli M, Groth DF, et al. Scrapie and cellular
PrP isoforms are encoded by the same chromosomal gene. Cell 1986;46:417–28.
[14] Bueler H, Aguzzi A, Sailer A, Greiner RA, Autenried P, Aguet M, et al. Mice devoid of PrP
are resistant to scrapie. Cell 1993;73:1339–47.
[15] Manuelidis L, Sklaviadis T, Manuelidis EE. Evidence suggesting that PrP is not the infec-
tious agent in Creutzfeldt- Jakob disease. EMBO J 1987;6:341–7.
[16] Stahl N, Borchelt DR, Hsiao K, Prusiner SB. Scrapie prion protein contains a phos-
phatidylinositol glycolipid. Cell 1987;51:229–40.
[17] Stimson E, Hope J, Chong A, Burlingame AL. Site-specific characterization of the N-linked
glycans of murine prion protein by high-performance liquid chromatography/electrospray
mass spectrometry and exoglycosidase digestions. Biochemistry 1999;38:4885–95.
[18] Rudd PM, Wormald MR, Wing DR, Prusiner SB, Dwek RA. Prion glycoprotein: struc-
ture, dynamics, and roles for the sugars. Biochemistry 2001;40:3759–66.
[19] Donne DG, Viles JH, Groth D, Mehlhorn I, James TL, Cohen FE, et al. Structure of the
recombinant full-length hamster prion protein PrP(29-231): the N terminus is highly
flexible. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1997;94:13452–7.
570 M.-S. Sy et al / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 551–571
[20] Zahn R, Liu A, Luhrs T, Riek R, von Schroetter C, Lopez Garcia F, et al. NMR solution
structure of the human prion protein. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2000;97:145–50.
[21] Bueler H, Fischer M, Lang Y, Bluethmann H, Lipp HP, DeArmond SJ, et al. Normal
development and behaviour of mice lacking the neuronal cell-surface PrP protein. Nature
1992;356:577–82.
[22] Wong BS, Pan T, Liu T, Li R, Petersen RB, Jones IM, et al. Prion disease: a loss of
antioxidant function? Biochem Biophys Res Commun 2000;275:249–52.
[23] Pan KM, Baldwin M, Nguyen J, Gasset M, Serban A, Groth D, et al. Conversion of alpha-
helices into beta-sheets: features in the formation of the scrapie prion protein. Proc Natl
Acad Sci USA 1993;90:10962–6.
[24] Caughey B, Race RE, Ernst D, Buchmeier MJ, Chesebro B. Prion protein biosynthesis in
scrapie-infected and -uninfected neuroblastoma cells. J Virol 1989;63:175–81.
[25] Gorodinsky A, Harris DA. Glycolipid-anchored proteins in neuroblastoma cells form
detergent-resistant complexes without caveolin. J Cell Biol 1995;129:619–27.
[26] Herrmann LM, Caughey B. The importance of the disulfide bond in prion protein
conversion. Neuroreport 1998;9:2457–61.
[27] Lehmann S, Harris DA. Blockade of glycosylation promotes acquisition of scrapie-like
properties by the prion protein in cultured cells. J Biol Chem 1997;272:21479–87.
[28] Kaneko K, Zulianello L, Scott M, Cooper CM, Wallace AC, James TL, et al. Evidence for
protein X binding to a discontinuous epitope on the cellular prion protein during scrapie
prion propagation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1997;94:10069–74.
[29] Kocisko DA, Come JH, Priola SA, Chesebro B, Raymond GJ, Lansbury PT, et al. Cell-
free formation of protease-resistant prion protein. Nature 1994;370:471–4.
[30] Saborio GP, Soto C, Kascsak RJ, Levy E, Kascsak R, Harris DA, et al. Cell-lysate con-
version of prion protein into its protease-resistant isoform suggests the participation of a
cellular chaperone. Biochem Biophys Res Commun 1999;258:470–5.
[31] Wong BS, Green AJ, Li RL, Xie Z, Pan T, Liu T, et al. Absence of protease resistant prion
protein in the cerebrospinal fluid of Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease. J Pathol 2001;194:9–14.
[32] Lasmezas CI, Deslys JP, Robain O, Jaegly A, Beringue V, Peyrin JM, et al. Transmission
of the BSE agent to mice in the absence of detectable abnormal prion protein. Science
1997;275:402–5.
[33] Goldfarb LG, Brown P, McCombie WR, Goldgaber D, Swergold GD, Wills PR, et al.
Transmissible familial Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease associated with five, seven, and eight extra
octapeptide coding repeats in the PRNP gene. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1991;88:10926–30.
[34] Flechsig E, Shmerling D, Hegyi I, Raeber AJ, Fischer M, Cozzio A, et al. Prion protein
devoid of the octapeptide repeat region restores susceptibility to scrapie in PrP knockout
mice. Neuron 2000;27:399–408.
[35] Hsiao K, Baker HF, Crow TJ, Poulter M, Owen F, Terwilliger JD, et al. Linkage of a prion
protein missense variant to Gerstmann-Straussler syndrome. Nature 1989;338:342–5.
[36] Manson JC, Jamieson E, Baybutt H, Tuzi NL, Barron R, McConnell I, et al. A single
amino acid alteration (101L) introduced into murine PrP dramatically alters incubation
time of transmissible spongiform encephalopathy. EMBO J 1999;18:6855–64.
[37] Bruce ME, Will RG, Ironside JW, McConnell I, Drummond D, Suttie A, et al. Trans-
missions to mice indicate that ‘new variant’ CJD is caused by the BSE agent [see comments].
Nature 1997;389:498–501.
[38] Ironside JW, Hilton DA, Ghani A, Johnston NJ, Conyers L, McCardle LM, et al. Retro-
spective study of prion-protein accumulation in tonsil and appendix tissues. Lancet 2000;
355:1693–4.
[39] Belay ED, Gambetti P, Schonberger LB, Parchi P, Lyon DR, Capellari S, McQuiston JH,
Bradley K, Dowdle G, Crutcher JM, Nichols CR. Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease in unusually
young patients venison who consumed. Arch Neurol 2001;58:1673–78.
[40] Ministry of Agriculture, Fisheries, and Food. BSE in Great Britain. A progress report. London:
Department for Environment, Food, and Rural Affairs Publications; 1998.
M.-S. Sy et al / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 551–571 571
[41] Halliwell B, Gutteridge JMC. Free radicals in biology and medicine. 3rd edition.
Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1999.
[42] Brown DR, Wong BS, Hafiz F, Clive C, Haswell S, Jones IM. Normal prion protein has an
activity like that of superoxide dismutase. Biochem J 1999;344:1–5.
[43] Brown DR, Hafiz F, Glassmith LL, Wong BS, Jones IM, Clive C, et al. Consequences of
manganese replacement of copper for prion protein function and proteinase resistance.
EMBO J 2000;19:1180–6.
[44] Wong BS, Chen SG, Colucci M, Xie Z, Li R, Pan T, et al. Aberrant metal binding by prion
protein in human prion disease. J Neurochem 2001;78:1400–08.
[45] Zerr I, Pocchiari M, Collins S, Brandel JP, de Pedro Cuesta J, Knight RS, et al. Analysis of
EEG and CSF 14–3-3 proteins as aids to the diagnosis of Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease.
Neurology 2000;55:811–5.
[46] Green AJ, Knight RS, Macleod MA, Lowman A, Will RG. Misleading results with the
14–3-3 assay for the diagnosis of Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease. Neurology 2001;56:986–7.
[47] Li R, Lio T, Wong BS, Pan T, Morillas M, Swietnicki W, O’Rourke K, Gambetti P,
Surewicz WK, Sy M-S. Identification of an epitope in the C terminus of normal prion
protein whose expression is modulated by binding events in the N terminus. J Mol Biol
2000;301:567–73.
Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 573–590
Dementia epidemiology
Walter A. Kukull, PhDa,*, James D. Bowen, MDb
a
Department of Epidemiology, University of Washington, Box 357236, Seattle,
WA 98195-7286, USA
b
Department of Neurology, University of Washington School of Medicine,
Box 356465, Seattle, WA 98195-7286, USA
Dementia has become one of the leading public health concerns facing
our society. The past few decades have seen remarkable success in treating
some acute illnesses and in reducing the incidence of others, such as stroke.
Epidemiology has played a central role in identifying the risk factors and
potential causes of these acute illnesses, whereas clinical medicine has pro-
vided new and more effective treatments. This has allowed us to develop a
wide array of interventions, such as antibiotics, safer working conditions,
and improved sanitation. Our success in preventing and controlling the
acute diseases that strike between childhood and early middle age has led
to greater longevity and the aging of our population. In the developed
world, chronic diseases are the leading causes of morbidity and mortality.
Of these chronic illnesses, dementia is one of the most common, extracting
an enormous social, emotional, and economic burden on society. At least
6% to 10% of persons in the United States aged 65 years or older may suffer
from some form of dementia [1]. Approximately 2.32 million persons in the
United States were estimated to be living with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) in
1997 [2]. The enormous burden that dementia inflicts on individuals,
families, and society underlies the increased emphasis medical science has
placed on the study of these diseases. As with other scientific fields, there
is great interest in studying causes and risk factors for dementia with all the
tools that epidemiology can bring to bear. The epidemiologic study of
dementia has some unique challenges, however.
Case detection and identification (or diagnosis) are critical to the epide-
miologic study of dementia. In other words, how are new dementia cases
found, and once found, how accurately are they diagnosed? The diagnosis
of dementia is difficult because specific antemortem biologic markers for
* Corresponding author.
E-mail address: [email protected] (W.A. Kukull).
0025-7125/02/$ - see front matter Ó 2002, Elsevier Science (USA). All rights reserved.
PII: S 0 0 2 5 - 7 1 2 5 ( 0 2 ) 0 0 0 1 0 - X
574 W.A. Kukull, J.D. Bowen / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 573–590
most dementias, and specifically for AD, do not exist. Instead, the diagnosis
must be made on the basis of clinical evidence. There can be considerable
variation in making the diagnosis of dementia based on a clinical examina-
tion, potentially leading to diagnostic heterogeneity and misclassification of
those patients with dementia. Fortunately, some of this heterogeneity has
been reduced by the recent introduction of clinical criteria for the diagnosis
of various types of dementia. Standardization of clinical criteria can limit
the amount of misclassification; however, in practice, standardization is dif-
ficult to achieve.
Case detection is a further problem for most epidemiologic studies. Cases
identified using death certificates alone are simply not comparable to those
identified by direct patient examination; there are too many extraneous fac-
tors affecting whether dementia might or might not be entered as a cause of
death. The method of identifying and including cases, if associated with expo-
sure history, could cause serious selection bias and lead to spurious conclu-
sions. Can all identified cases from a particular study base be enrolled in a
study? Usually, this does not happen, because many persons (or their families)
decline to participate in research studies. Frailty, age, ethnicity, gender, edu-
cation, and a host of other factors also influence participation. If any of these
participation factors is systematically related to exposure status, an uncon-
trolled bias may result. Case identification methods are also critical to cohort
studies (and intervention trials). Failure to start with a cohort that is free of
the disease of interest can potentially bias results. Lack of sensitivity or spec-
ificity in screening or diagnosing disease during follow-up leads to incorrect
estimates of incidence and to distorted risk factor relations.
Exposure measurement (valid determination of risk factor information) is
critical for analytic risk factor studies. For dementia, determining when,
relative to disease onset, an exposure may have been able to influence dis-
ease is complicated by the often insidious and indeterminate onset of dis-
ease. Long past exposure histories are difficult to construct and validate,
especially in diseases that affect memory. Self-reported histories and those
obtained from proxies are often the basis for risk factor inference but may
be flawed by distorted recollection or recall bias. Actual records, for exam-
ple, of medication history or occupational exposures are seldom available.
Biologic markers of exposure (except for genotype) are difficult to obtain;
and some may be affected by the disease itself. For example, low serum cho-
lesterol levels in existing/prevalent cases may be a function of the impact of
disease on diet or may be influenced by physical wasting. Potential markers
obtained from analysis of peripheral blood may not correspond to marker
exposure levels in neuronal tissue. Biopsy of the affected tissue is not avail-
able in most dementia diagnoses, and autopsy, which is the gold standard
for the diagnosis of many dementias, may reflect cumulative disease pro-
cesses, leaving the picture additionally confusing.
As one leaves major research institutions or attempts to begin epidemio-
logic research studies in less developed countries, the problems grow in mag-
W.A. Kukull, J.D. Bowen / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 573–590 575
nitude. Differences in available facilities and local practices are likely the
easiest to overcome. Addressing political concerns and suspicions to gain the
cooperation necessary to begin a study may take additional time and pre-
paration. Case detection, case identification, and risk factor exposure mea-
surement remain critical to valid research in these settings, but the difficulty
in obtaining acceptable levels of each is increased substantially.
Clinical overview
Dementia can sometimes occur suddenly as the result of a stroke. More
often, it presents with a slowly progressive loss of cognitive function as with
AD. Cognitive loss may begin with mild memory problems, language diffi-
culties, or problems with activities of daily living that many might ascribe to
aging but progress to more serious socially and functionally debilitating cog-
nitive impairment. Memory may not always be the first domain affected.
The usually insidious nature of most dementia makes it difficult to deter-
mine the point of disease onset. Moreover, it is especially difficult to deter-
mine whether dementia is present in people with only some mild indications
of cognitive impairment and whether that mild impairment is benign and
nonprogressive or a prodrome of the progressive disease. Valid early identi-
fication of true disease would be most helpful in linking exposures and risk
factors to the disease. Behavioral changes may be prominent, including
agitation, wandering, personality change, and depression. In late stages,
patients may be completely dependent on others. Various definitions of
dementia have been used in past research studies, but that of the Diagnostic
and Statistical Manual: Edition IV (DSM-IV) is the most commonly used
now [3]. The DSM-IV criteria for dementia require memory impairment and
one or more additional cognitive disturbances. These include aphasia (lan-
guage disturbance), apraxia (impaired ability to carry out motor activities
despite intact motor function), agnosia (failure to recognize or identify
objects despite intact sensory function), and disturbances in executive func-
tioning (ie, planning, organizing, sequencing, abstracting). The cognitive
deficits must be severe enough to cause significant impairment in social or
occupational functioning and represent a significant decline from a previous
level of functioning. Dementia must be differentiated from delirium (eg,
caused by illness, medications, or intoxicants). The causes of dementia are
listed in Table 1. There is some controversy concerning whether memory
loss should be the cardinal feature of dementia (ie, ‘‘necessary’’ for the diag-
nosis) or whether loss in a combination of other cognitive domains, without
memory loss, should be considered sufficient for diagnosis. Leaders in the
study of vascular dementia have raised interest and debate concerning this
question. Beyond the umbrella category of ‘‘dementia,’’ there are clinical
and sometimes histopathologic criteria for differentiation of dementia
subtypes. To date, diagnostic criteria have been developed for ‘‘mild cogni-
tive impairment,’’ AD, vascular dementia, dementia with Lewy bodies,
576 W.A. Kukull, J.D. Bowen / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 573–590
Table 1
Causes of dementia
Idiopathic
Alzheimer’s disease
Frontal temporal dementia
Focal/central nervous system pathologic findings
Multi-infarct dementia
Binswanger’s disease
Multiple sclerosis
Mass lesions
Tumors, multiple sites
Tumors, single site
Gliomatosis cerebri
Abscess
Subdural malformation
Hydrocephalus
Infections
AIDS (HIV)
Chronic meningitis
Encephalitis
Progressive multifocal leukoencephalopathy
Subacute sclerosing panencephalitis
Syphilis
Lyme disease
Prion disease (kuru, Creutzfeldt-Jacob)
Toxins
Drugs
Alcohol
Heavy metals
Industrial toxins
Domoic acid
Inherited disease
Huntington’s disease
Gerstmann-Sträussler syndrome
Porphyria
Propionic aciduria
Adult-onset lysosomal storage diseases
Hexosaminidase
Arylsulfatase (metachromatic leukodystrophy, or MLD)
Kuf disease
Adrenoleukodystrophy
Others
Myotonic muscular dystrophy
Down’s syndrome
Hereditary ataxias
Hereditary spastic paraplegias
Cerebrotendinous xanthomatosis
Systemic disease
Cardiac
Pulmonary
Renal
Renal failure
(continued on next page)
W.A. Kukull, J.D. Bowen / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 573–590 577
Table 1 (continued )
Dialysis dementia
Hepatic
Hepatic failure
Hepatocerebral degeneration
Wilson’s disease
Endocrine
Hyper/hypothyroid
Hyper/hypoparathyroid
Hyper/hypoadrenalism
Syndrome of inappropriate secretion of antidiuretic hormone (SIADH)
Rheumatologic
Vasculitis (including systemic lupus erythematosus, or SLE)
Giant cell arteritis
Sarcoid
Amyloid
Neoplastic
Metastasis
Carcinomatous meningitis
Paraneoplastic (limbic encephalitis)
Associated movement disorder
Huntington’s disease
Parkinsonian diseases
Parkinson’s disease
Progressive supranuclear palsy
Postencephalitic dementia
Posttraumatic (dementia pugilistica)
Diffuse Lewy body disease
Myoclonus
Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease
Alzheimer’s disease
Metabolic derangement
Other movement disorder
Hereditary ataxias
Hereditary spastic paraplegia
Kuru
Wilson’s disease
Seizures
Kuf disease
Deficiency
Vitamin B12 deficiency
Thiamine
Niacin (Pellagra)
and HIV infection. Two commonly used criteria for the diagnosis of AD
have been proposed [3,4].
Vascular dementia is difficult to differentiate from AD because of the
common occurrence of cerebrovascular disease and stroke with AD in the
elderly. The more sophisticated the search for stroke, the more likely it is
that some strokes are found. The clinical identification of stroke is surpassed
by CT, which is surpassed by MRI. The false-positive identification of
stroke also increases, however. Many different criteria have been developed
to diagnose vascular dementia [3,5,6]. Much of the pioneering work in the
definition and recognition of vascular dementia can be attributed to
Hachinski and his colleagues [7–15].
Recently, two additional types of dementia, dementia with Lewy bodies
[16] and frontotemporal dementia [17], have been separated from AD based
on their clinical presentations and pathologic characteristics. Dementia with
Lewy bodies presents with cognitive losses. In addition, fluctuating cognitive
performance, visual hallucinations, and parkinsonism are suggestive of this
disease. Memory impairment may not necessarily be prominent in the early
stages of the disease. Deficits in attention, frontal subcortical skills, and
visuospatial ability predominate.
In frontotemporal dementia, changes in behavior dominate the early
course of the disease These include loss of personal awareness, loss of social
graces, disinhibition, overactivity, restlessness, impulsivity, distractibility,
hyperorality, withdrawal from social contact, apathy or inertia, and stereo-
typed or perseverative behaviors. Speech output changes occur, including
progressive reduction of speech, stereotypy of speech, perseveration, and
echolalia. Physical signs include early or prominent primitive or ‘‘frontal’’
reflexes, early incontinence, late akinesia, rigidity, and tremor. Deficits in
social comportment, behavior, judgment, or language are out of proportion
to the memory deficit. The memory loss is variable and often seems to be
caused by lack of concern or effort. Frontal lobe impairments are notable,
including abstraction, planning, and self-regulation of behavior. There are
several pathologic findings that may lead to frontotemporal dementia, includ-
ing some with dominantly inherited mutations related to the protein tau.
cases per 1000 persons older than 65 years of age in 1999) or as a ‘‘density’’
or instantaneous rate (eg, 12 new cases per 1000 person-years). Incidence is
less useful in determining societal burden, and prevalence is not useful in
determining disease risk, but kept in their own arena, they are useful
descriptors of disease occurrence, and they are linked by disease duration.
Evans et al [18] reported the results of a community study in East Boston.
The prevalence estimates for dementia and AD were based on a complex
community sampling scheme [19]. Because of the diagnostic criteria and
screening procedures, the results of this study have been somewhat contro-
versial, but they are widely accepted as an observed but high estimate of the
potential dementia prevalence in East Boston. Prevalence proportion rose
from 3% among those 65 to 74 years of age to 47% in those older than 85 years
of age. More than 80% of the observed dementia cases were classified as AD.
Evans later applied the observed rates to census data, projecting that 10.3
million persons would have AD in the year 2050. Recently, a meta-analysis
of prevalence studies worldwide was conducted [20], which concluded that
the prevalence proportions and incidence rates observed across studies were
geographically consistent, except for variations caused by methodologic dif-
ferences. Prevalence proportions rose from 0.3% to 1.0% in 60- to 64-year-
olds to 43% to 68% in persons aged 95 years or older. As a summary figure,
prevalence is often reported as 6% to 10% among persons aged 65 years or
older in North America [1]. Brookmeyer et al [2] have estimated that if disease
age-specific incidence curves could be shifted generally to delay onset by
2 years, that would result in 2 million fewer cases in the future.
The dementia prevalence proportion in the population is a function of
disease incidence and disease duration (or survival). Jorm and Jolley [21]
gathered data from 23 studies and produced a meta-analysis of dementia in-
cidence. Incidence was estimated for Europe, the United States, and East
Asia; dementia, AD, and vascular dementia incidence rates were computed.
Incidence rates for the United States and Europe were quite similar: ‘‘mod-
erate’’ dementia incidence rose from 3.6 per 1000 person-years (in persons
aged 65–69 years) to 37.7 per 1000 person-years (in persons aged 85–89 years)
in Europe and from 2.4 to 27.5 per 1000 person-years for the same age groups
in the United States. ‘‘Mild’’ AD incidence was also computed, ranging from
2.5 per 1000 person-years (in persons aged 65–69 years) to 46.1 per 1000
person-years for Europe compared with 6.1 to 74.5 per 1000 person-years for
the United States and 0.7 to 39.7 per 1000 person-years for East Asia.
Rocca et al [22] reanalyzed dementia and AD incidence data for 1975
through 1984 based on charted data from the Rochester Epidemiology
Project at the Mayo Clinic. The results showed dementia incidence overall
as 2.2 per 1000 person-years in 65- to 69-year-olds rising to 40.8 per 1000
person-years in those aged 90 years or more. Similarly, for AD, rates rose
from 1.2 to 33.9 per 1000 person-years. These investigators noted that
annual incidence seemed to stay rather stable during the 1975 to 1984 time
interval. After disaggregating the data for the oldest people, Rocca et al [22]
580 W.A. Kukull, J.D. Bowen / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 573–590
also reported that rates seemed to continue to rise with age after the age of
84 years. They also noted that rates were similar for men and women.
The combined analysis of four large ongoing European cohort studies of
dementia and AD was recently reported by Launer et al [23]. Cohorts
enrolled in Denmark, France, the Netherlands, and the United Kingdom
totalled more than 16,000 members aged 65 years or older at enrollment.
After a mean follow-up of 2.2 years (comprising approximately 28,600
person-years), the overall incidence of dementia was 14.6 per 1000 person-
years. Approximately two thirds of these cases were attributed to AD. In-
cidence of dementia was 2.5 per 1000 person-years in 65- to 69-year olds and
rose to 85.6 per 1000 person-years in those aged 90 years and older. Simi-
larly, AD rose from 1.2 per 1000 person-years to 63.5 per 1000 person-
years across the same age groups. The report by Launer et al [23] is one
of the first using data from the large cohort studies that are now underway
in Europe and the United States. As more cohort studies begin to report inci-
dence, consistent estimates are likely to emerge.
Most of the difference between dementia and AD incidence rates reported
here likely represents the next most common form of dementia, vascular
dementia. We cannot be sure of this at present, however. Despite the com-
pilation of several competing diagnostic criteria for vascular dementia [5,6],
this syndrome remains an area of controversy and uncertainty [24–29].
Application of the diagnostic criteria has been shown to be difficult and
unreliable in practice, even by experienced research investigators [30]. Many
of the reliability and validity problems experienced by investigators in clas-
sifying a case as ‘‘vascular’’ or AD may stem from the mutual exclusion of
the two conditions imposed principally by the DSM-IV diagnostic criteria.
There is a growing realization that a vascular component can contribute to
the dementia seen in AD [26,31–36].
Late-stage dementia and AD hold little hope for treatment or the identifi-
cation of consistent risk factors; as a result, interest has continued to shift
toward early and valid identification of disease. Early forms of pre-AD or
predementia are difficult to distinguish from the relatively benign cognitive
decline associated with aging. Nevertheless, when mild cognitive decline can
be identified, it seems that perhaps 50% of such cases may progress to become
dementia [37–41]. Distinguishing between normal persons, those with mild
cognitive impairment, and those with incipient dementia/AD may provide
important clues about risk factors and critical periods of exposure before dis-
ease onset. Reliable and valid distinctions may also help to determine whether
mild cognitive impairment is a treatable and reversible phenomenon.
inantly tends to affect persons younger than 60 years of age. Familial AD,
so defined, seems to account for less than 5% of all AD, but important clues
may be learned from the study of familial disease, which may apply to the
more common form (often called sporadic, but it may also have undiscovered
genetic causes). Several current reviews of AD genetics are available [90–99].
The largest proportion of familial AD is attributed to mutations in the
presenilin 1 gene (chromosome 14), and the next largest known contribution
is from mutations in a homologous gene on chromosome 1, presenilin 2. A
small proportion of cases are caused by specific mutations in the amyloid
precursor protein gene (chromosome 21). It is abnormal cleavage of the
amyloid precursor protein that results in the formation of amyloid-b pro-
tein(1–42). Amyloid-b protein aggregates in the brain, forming the charac-
teristic plaques of AD. Recently, important work has been published
concerning the identification of enzymes that cleave the precursor protein
abnormally, forming the amyloid-b 1–42 protein. This work may ultimately
help to identify sites for drug intervention not only for familial AD but for
nonfamilial AD [93,100–105]. Perhaps one quarter to one half of the genes
for familial AD have yet to be identified [95–98].
Arguably, the strongest and most consistent risk factor for nonfamilial
AD (other than age) is apolipoprotein E (APOE) genotype. The association
was first described from Allen Roses’ laboratory [106–111]. ApoE naturally
occurs as three different alleles (e2, e3, and e4), which pair to form one of six
genotypes for each individual. Genotypes containing the e4 allele are asso-
ciated with an increased risk of AD; homozygous e4 greatly increases the
risk (>8-fold). Since the initial description of increased risk associated with
the e4 allele, many investigators have observed the association. Discussion
of ApoE genotype is now included in most risk factor studies of AD either
as a focus or as a potential confounder/effect modifier of an association.
Farrer et al [112] provided a meta-analysis of the age and gender effects, and
Mayeux et al [113] later described caveats for the potential value of ApoE
genotype in AD diagnosis. Despite the huge volume of studies that include
ApoE genotyping, relatively little is known concerning how the e2, e3, and
e4 alleles actually work to influence the risk of AD.
Evidence for the effects of other genes on AD has also been presented. a2-
Macroglobulin was first shown as a potential risk factor by Blacker et al
[117], but a number of other investigators failed to replicate the association
[114–116,118]. Other genetic associations have been studied but with limited
impact to date [96,119–129]. Progress continues, and there is considerable
hope for the discovery of important genes that may provide indications for
prevention or therapy.
Summary
Determining the incidence and prevalence of dementia is an inexact
science. Dementia is difficult to define and detect in the population. Even
584 W.A. Kukull, J.D. Bowen / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 573–590
References
[1] Hendrie H. Epidemiology of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry
1998;6(Suppl):S3–18.
[2] Brookmeyer R, Gray S, Kawas C. Projections of Alzheimer’s disease in the United States
and the public health impact of delaying disease onset. Am J Public Health 1998;88:1337–42.
[3] American Psychiatric Association Task Force on DSM-IV. Diagnostic and statistical
manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV. 4th edition. Washington, DC: American Psy-
chiatric Association; 1994.
[4] McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, Katzman R, Price D, Stadlan E. Clinical
diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the
auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s
Disease. Neurology 1984;34:939–44.
[5] Roman G, Tatemichi T, Erkinjuntti T, Cummings J, Masdeu J, Garcia J, et al. Vascular
dementia: diagnostic criteria for research studies. Report of the NINDS-AIREN
International Workshop. Neurology 1993;43:250–60.
[6] Chui H, Victoroff J, Margolin D, Jagust W, Shankle R, Katzman R. Criteria for the
diagnosis of ischemic vascular dementia proposed by the State of California Alzheimer’s
Disease Diagnostic and Treatment Centers. Neurology 1992;42:473–80.
[7] Wade J, Hachinski V. Revised ischemic score for diagnosing multi-infarct dementia. J Clin
Psychiatry 1986;47:437–8.
[8] Larson D, Lyons J, Bareta J, Burns B, Blazer D, Goldstrom I. The construct validity of
the ischemic score of Hachinski for the detection of dementias. J Neuropsychiatry Clin
Neurosci 1989;1:181–7.
[9] Pantoni L, Inzitari D. Hachinski’s ischemic score and the diagnosis of vascular dementia:
a review. Ital J Neurol Sci 1993;14:539–46.
[10] Rockwood K, Bowler J, Erkinjuntti T, Hachinski V, Wallin A. Subtypes of vascular
dementia. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 1999;13(Suppl 3):S59–65.
W.A. Kukull, J.D. Bowen / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 573–590 585
[36] Meyer J, Rauch G, Rauch R, Haque A, Crawford K. Cardiovascular and other risk factors
for Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia. Ann NY Acad Sci 2000;903:411–23.
[37] Wolf H, Grunwald M, Ecke G, Zedlick D, Bettin S, Dannenberg C, et al. The prognosis
of mild cognitive impairment in the elderly. J Neural Transm 1998;54(Suppl):S31–50.
[38] Petersen R, Smith G, Waring S, Ivnik R, Tangalos E, Kokmen E. Mild cognitive
impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol 1999;56:303–8.
[39] Celsis P. Age-related cognitive decline, mild cognitive impairment or preclinical Alz-
heimer’s disease? Ann Med 2000;32:6–14.
[40] Almkvist O, Basun H, Backman L, Herlitz A, Lannfelt L, Small B, et al. Mild cognitive
impairment—an early stage of Alzheimer’s disease? J Neural Transm 1998;54(Suppl):S21–9.
[41] Almkvist O, Winblad B. Early diagnosis of Alzheimer dementia based on clinical and
biological factors. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 1999;249:3–9.
[42] Moceri V, Kukull W, Emanuel I, van Belle G, Larson E. Early-life risk factors and the
development of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 2000;54:415–20.
[43] Lee P. Smoking and Alzheimer’s disease: a review of the epidemiological evidence.
Neuroepidemiology 1994;13:131–44.
[44] Rothman K, Greenland S. Modern epidemiology. 2nd edition. Philadelphia: Lippincott-
Raven; 1998.
[45] Gordis L. Epidemiology. Philadelphia: WB Saunders; 1996.
[46] MacMahon B, Trichopoulos D. Epidemiology: principles and methods. 2nd edition.
Boston: Little Brown; 1996.
[47] Wang H, Fratiglioni L, Frisoni G, Vitanen M, Winblad B. Smoking and the occurrence of
Alzheimer’s disease: cross-sectional and longitudinal data in a population-based study.
Am J Epidemiol 1999;149:640–4.
[48] Merchant C, Tang M, Albert S, Manly J, Stern Y, Mayeux R. The influence of smoking
on the risk of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 1999;52:1408–12.
[49] Doll R, Peto R, Boreham J, Sutherland I. Smoking and dementia in male British doctors:
prospective study. BMJ 2000;320:1097–102.
[50] Brayne C. The EURODEM collaborative re-analysis of case-control studies of Alz-
heimer’s disease: implications for public health. Int J Epidemiol 1991;20(Suppl):S68–71.
[51] O’Meara E, Kukull W, Sheppard L, Bowen J, McCormick W, Teri L, et al. Head injury and
risk of Alzheimer’s disease by apolipoprotein E genotype. Am J Epidemiol 1997;146:373–84.
[52] NIH. NIH Consensus Statement: Rehabilitation of persons with traumatic brain injury.
NIH Consens Statement 1998;16:1–41.
[53] Nee L, Lippa C. Alzheimer’s disease in 22 twin pairs—13-year follow-up: hormonal,
infectious and traumatic factors. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 1999;10:148–51.
[54] Mehta K, Ott A, Kalmijn S, Slooter A, van Duijn C, Hofman A, et al. Head trauma and risk
of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. The Rotterdam Study. Neurology 1999;53:1959–62.
[55] Tang M, Maestre G, Tsai W, Liu X, Feng L, Chung W, et al. Effect of age, ethnicity, and
head injury on the association between APOE genotypes and Alzheimer’s disease. Ann
NY Acad Sci 1996;802:6–15.
[56] Schofield P, Tang M, Marder K, Bell K, Dooneief G, Chun M, et al. Alzheimer’s disease after
remote head injury: an incidence study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1997;62:119–24.
[57] Stern Y, Albert S, Tang M, Tsai W. Rate of memory decline in AD is related to education
and occupation: cognitive reserve? Neurology 1999;53:1942–7.
[58] Riley K, Snowdon D, Saunders A, Roses A, Mortimer J, Nanayakkara N. Cognitive
function and apolipoprotein E in very old adults: findings from the Nun Study. J Gerontol
B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2000;55(Suppl):S69–75.
[59] Ott A, Breteler M, van Harskamp F, Stijnen T, Hofman A. Incidence and risk of
dementia. The Rotterdam Study. Am J Epidemiol 1998;147:574–80.
[60] Munoz D, Ganapathy G, Eliasziw M, Hachinski V. Educational attainment and
socioeconomic status of patients with autopsy-confirmed Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol
2000;57:85–9.
W.A. Kukull, J.D. Bowen / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 573–590 587
[61] Muller-Spahn F, Hock C. Risk factors and differential diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease.
Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 1999;249:37–42.
[62] Hall K, Gao S, Unverzagt F, Hendrie H. Low education and childhood rural residence:
risk for Alzheimer’s disease in African Americans. Neurology 2000;54:95–9.
[63] Geerlings M, Schmand B, Jonker C, Lindeboom J, Bouter L. Education and incident
Alzheimer’s disease: a biased association due to selective attrition and use of a two-step
diagnostic procedure? Int J Epidemiol 1999;28:492–7.
[64] Geerlings M, Deeg D, Penninx B, Schmand B, Jonker C, Bouter L, et al. Cognitive reserve
and mortality in dementia: the role of cognition, functional ability and depression.
Psychol Med 1999;29:1219–26.
[65] Gainotti G, Marra C, Villa G, Parlato V, Chiarotti F. Sensitivity and specificity of some
neuropsychological markers of Alzheimer dementia. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 1998;
12:152–62.
[66] Braak E, Griffing K, Arai K, Bohl J, Bratzke H, Braak H. Neuropathology of Alzheimer’s
disease: what is new since A. Alzheimer? Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 1999;249:
14–22.
[67] Stratman N, Carter D, Sethy V. Ibuprofen: effect on inducible nitric oxide synthase. Brain
Res Mol Brain Res 1997;50:107–12.
[68] Stewart W, Kawas C, Corrada M, Metter E. Risk of Alzheimer’s disease and duration of
NSAID use. Neurology 1997;48:626–32.
[69] Mortimer J. New findings consistent with Alzheimer’s-NSAIDs link. Neurobiol Aging
1998;19:615–6.
[70] McGeer P, Schulzer M, McGeer E. Arthritis and anti-inflammatory agents as possible
protective factors for Alzheimer’s disease: a review of 17 epidemiologic studies. Neurology
1996;47:425–32.
[71] in ’t Veld B, Launer L, Hoes A, Ott A, Hofman A, Breteler M, et al. NSAIDs and incident
Alzheimer’s disease. The Rotterdam Study. Neurobiol Aging 1998;19:607–11.
[72] Combs C, Johnson D, Karlo J, Cannady S, Landreth G. Inflammatory mechanisms in
Alzheimer’s disease: inhibition of beta-amyloid-stimulated proinflammatory responses
and neurotoxicity by PPARgamma agonists. J Neurosci 2000;20:558–67.
[73] Breitner J. Inflammatory processes and antiinflammatory drugs in Alzheimer’s disease:
a current appraisal. Neurobiol Aging 1996;17:789–94.
[74] Yaffe K, Sawaya G, Lieberburg I, Grady D. Estrogen therapy in postmenopausal women:
effects on cognitive function and dementia. JAMA 1998;279:688–95.
[75] Waring S, Rocca W, Petersen R, O’Brien P, Tangalos E, Kokmen E. Postmenopausal
estrogen replacement therapy and risk of AD: a population-based study. Neurology
1999;52:965–70.
[76] Slooter A, Bronzova J, Witteman J, Van Broeckhoven C, Hofman A, van Duijn C.
Estrogen use and early onset Alzheimer’s disease: a population-based study. J Neurol
Neurosurg Psychiatry 1999;67:779–81.
[77] Nourhashemi F, Gillette-Guyonnet S, Andrieu S, Ghisolfi A, Ousset P, Grandjean H, et al.
Alzheimer disease: protective factors. Am J Clin Nutr 2000;71(Suppl):643S–9S.
[78] Mulnard R, Cotman C, Kawas C, van Dyck C, Sano M, Doody R, et al. Estrogen
replacement therapy for treatment of mild to moderate Alzheimer disease: a randomized
controlled trial. Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study. JAMA 2000;283:1007–15.
[79] McEwen B, Alves S. Estrogen actions in the central nervous system. Endocr Rev
1999;20:279–307.
[80] Kawas C, Resnick S, Morrison A, Brookmeyer R, Corrada M, Zonderman A, et al. A
prospective study of estrogen replacement therapy and the risk of developing Alzheimer’s
disease: the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Neurology 1997;48:1517–21.
[81] Henderson V, Paganini-Hill A, Miller B, Elble R, Reyes P, Shoupe D, et al. Estrogen for
Alzheimer’s disease in women: randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Neuro-
logy 2000;54:295–301.
588 W.A. Kukull, J.D. Bowen / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 573–590
[82] Henderson V. Estrogen, cognition, and a woman’s risk of Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Med
1997;103(Suppl):11S–8S.
[83] Henderson V. The epidemiology of estrogen replacement therapy and Alzheimer’s disease.
Neurology 1997;48(Suppl):S27–35.
[84] Haskell S, Richardson E, Horwitz R. The effect of estrogen replacement therapy on
cognitive function in women: a critical review of the literature. J Clin Epidemiol 1997;50:
1249–64.
[85] Costa M, Reus V, Wolkowitz O, Manfredi F, Lieberman M. Estrogen replacement
therapy and cognitive decline in memory-impaired post-menopausal women. Biol
Psychiatry 1999;46:182–8.
[86] Brenner D, Kukull W, Stergachis A, van Belle G, Bowen J, McCormick W, et al. Post-
menopausal estrogen replacement therapy and the risk of Alzheimer’s disease: a popu-
lation-based case-control study. Am J Epidemiol 1994;140:262–7.
[87] Birge S. Hormones and the aging brain. Geriatrics 1998;53(Suppl 1):S28–30.
[88] Baldereschi M, Di Carlo A, Lepore V, Bracco L, Maggi S, Grigoletto F, et al. Estrogen-
replacement therapy and Alzheimer’s disease in the Italian Longitudinal Study on Aging.
Neurology 1998;50:996–1002.
[89] Morris M, Beckett L, Scherr P, Hebert L, Bennett D, Field T, et al. Vitamin E and vitamin
C supplement use and risk of incident Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord
1998;12:121–6.
[90] Tilley L, Morgan K, Kalsheker N. Genetic risk factors in Alzheimer’s disease. Mol Pathol
1998;51:293–304.
[91] Tanzi R, Kovacs D, Kim T, Moir R, Guenette S, Wasco W. The gene defects responsible
for familial Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Dis 1996;3:159–68.
[92] Tanzi R. A genetic dichotomy model for the inheritance of Alzheimer’s disease and
common age-related disorders. J Clin Invest 1999;104:1175–9.
[93] Steiner H, Capell A, Leimer U, Haass C. Genes and mechanisms involved in beta-amyloid
generation and Alzheimer’s disease. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 1999;249:266–70.
[94] St. George-Hyslop P. Molecular genetics of Alzheimer’s disease. Biol Psychiatry 2000;47:
183–99.
[95] Sisodia S. Alzheimer’s disease: perspectives for the new millennium. J Clin Invest
1999;104:1169–70.
[96] Shastry B, Giblin F. Genes and susceptible loci of Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Res Bull
1999;48:121–7.
[97] Price D, Tanzi R, Borchelt D, Sisodia S. Alzheimer’s disease: genetic studies and
transgenic models. Annu Rev Genet 1998;32:461–93.
[98] Levy-Lahad E, Tsuang D, Bird T. Recent advances in the genetics of Alzheimer’s disease.
J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 1998;11:42–54.
[99] Hardy J, Duff K, Hardy K, Perez-Tur J, Hutton M. Genetic dissection of Alzheimer’s disease
and related dementias: amyloid and its relationship to tau. Nat Neurosci 1998;1:355–8.
[100] Hussain I, Powell D, Howlett D, Tew D, Meek T, Chapman C, et al. Identification of a
novel aspartic protease (Asp 2) as beta-secretase. Mol Cell Neurosci 1999;14:419–27.
[101] Octave J, Essalmani R, Tasiaux B, Menager J, Czech C, Mercken L. The role of
presenilin-1 in the gamma-secretase cleavage of the amyloid precursor protein of
Alzheimer’s disease. J Biol Chem 2000;275:1525–8.
[102] Phimister B. Four companies announce discovery of beta-secretase gene. Nat Biotechnol
2000;18:16.
[103] Sinha S, Anderson J, Barbour R, Basi G, Caccavello R, Davis D, et al. Purification and
cloning of amyloid precursor protein beta-secretase from human brain. Nature 1999;402:
537–40.
[104] Vassar R, Bennett B, Babu-Khan S, Kahn S, Mendiaz E, Denis P, et al. Beta-secretase
cleavage of Alzheimer’s amyloid precursor protein by the transmembrane aspartic
protease BACE. Science 1999;286:735–41.
W.A. Kukull, J.D. Bowen / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 573–590 589
[125] Lilius L, Froelich Fabre S, Basun H, Forsell C, Axelman K, Mattila K, et al. Tau gene
polymorphisms and apolipoprotein E epsilon4 may interact to increase risk for
Alzheimer’s disease. Neurosci Lett 1999;277:29–32.
[126] Hutchin T, Heath P, Pearson R, Sinclair A. Mitochondrial DNA mutations in
Alzheimer’s disease. Biochem Biophys Res Commun 1997;241:221–5.
[127] Hirano M, Shtilbans A, Mayeux R, Davidson M, DiMauro S, Knowles J, et al. Apparent
mtDNA heteroplasmy in Alzheimer’s disease patients and in normals due to PCR amp-
lification of nucleus-embedded mtDNA pseudogenes. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1997;94:
14894–9.
[128] Ghetti B, Murrell J, Spillantini M. Mutations in the tau gene cause frontotemporal
dementia. Brain Res Bull 1999;50:471–2.
[129] Bullido M, Aldudo J, Frank A, Coria F, Avila J, Valdivieso F. A polymorphism in the tau
gene associated with risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Neurosci Lett 2000;278:49–52.
Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 591–614
Genetics of dementia
Debby W. Tsuang, MD, MSca,b,c,*,
Thomas D. Bird, MDb,c,d
a
Departments of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences and Epidemiology,
University of Washington, Seattle, WA, USA
b
Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center, University of Washington, Seattle, WA, USA
c
Veterans Affairs Puget Sound Health Care System, Mental Illness Research, Clinical, and
Education Center, 116MIRECC, 1660 South Columbian Way, Seattle, WA 98108, USA
d
Departments of Neurology, Medical Genetics, and Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences,
University of Washington, Seattle, WA, USA
Alzheimer’s disease
Clinical features
Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is the most common cause of dementia [2]. It is
a slowly progressive disease that initially presents with short-term memory
loss. Additional symptoms include executive dysfunction, confusion, apha-
sia, gait, and behavioral disturbances. The typical age of onset is older than
65 years. The average duration of illness ranges from 4 to 20 years. More
women than men are affected even after adjustment for the greater longevity
of women.
Pathologic features
Pathologically, AD is characterized by diffuse cerebral atrophy associated
with b-amyloid (Ab) neuritic plaques, neurofibrillary tangles, and amyloid
* Corresponding author. Veterans Affairs Puget Sound Health Care System, Mental Illness
Research, Clinical, and Education Center, 116MIRECC, 1660 South Columbian Way, Seattle,
WA 98108, USA.
E-mail address: [email protected] (D.W. Tsuang).
0025-7125/02/$ - see front matter 2002, Elsevier Science (USA). All rights reserved.
PII: S 0 0 2 5 - 7 1 2 5 ( 0 2 ) 0 0 0 0 3 - 2
592 D.W. Tsuang, T.D. Bird / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 591–614
Epidemiology
The risk for AD increases with advancing age. Approximately 10% of the
white population over the age of 70 years have dementia, and more than half
of these patients have AD [5]. In addition, approximately 20% to 40% of
individuals older than 85 years have clinically significant dementia.
The next most important risk factor for AD is family history. Epidemio-
logic studies show that individuals who have an affected first-degree relative
with AD have an approximately fourfold greater risk of developing AD and
a total lifetime risk of 23% to 48% [6], although more recent European stud-
ies do not report such high estimates [7]. To date, reports on monozygotic
and dizygotic twin pairs have suggested higher concordance rates in mono-
zygotic twins than in dizygotic twins [8]. Although the sample sizes are
small, they suggest that genetic components play an important role. The
lack of complete concordance in monozygotic twins suggests that environ-
mental components are also important in the etiology of AD.
The risk for AD is even higher if there are individuals in more than one
generation with the disease, especially when the disease is of early onset (age
<65 years). In some of these rare families, AD occurs as a single-gene auto-
somal dominant trait. Further proof for a genetic basis in AD is that all per-
sons with trisomy 21 (Down syndrome) who survive beyond the age of
40 years invariably demonstrate the neuropathologic features of AD [9].
These observations led to the finding of mutations in the amyloid precursor
protein (APP) gene on chromosome 21, the first documented genetic cause
of AD [10]. Although there are fewer than 20 families worldwide with APP
mutations, the discovery of these mutations confirmed that genetic factors
are important in AD.
Fig. 1. Schematic representation of the amyloid b (Ab) peptide portion of the amyloid
precursor protein (APP) demonstrating mutation sites associated with familial Alzheimer’s
disease (positions 670-671 and 717) and hereditary cerebral hemorrhagic amyloidosis of the
Dutch type (positions 692 and 693). The three normally occurring sites for processing this
portion of the APP are also indicated by the a-, b-, and c-secretases. Note that cleavage by the
a-secretase interrupts the Ab peptide, whereas cleavage by only the b- and c-secretases allows
the Ab peptide to remain intact. (From Levy-Lahad E, Bird TD. Genetic factors in Alzheimer’s
disease. Ann Neurol 1996;40:829–40; with permission.)
594 D.W. Tsuang, T.D. Bird / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 591–614
Fig. 2. Schematic representation of one possible form of the transmembrane proteins encoded
by the presenilin-1 (PS-1) gene on chromosome 14 and presenilin-2 (PS-2) gene on chromosome 1.
This diagram shows eight transmembrane domains, but other configurations remain possible.
Several known mutations are indicated, including PS-1 (filled circles) and PS-2 (open circles).
Not all mutations are shown. The arrow at the top left of the figure points to the Volga German
PS-2 N141I mutation, the first PS-2 mutation discovered. The arrow at the bottom of the figure
points to a PS-1 mutation (an exon 9 deletion) that is often associated with early spasticity.
nus, which appear relatively early in the course of the disease [23,24]. One
mutation in the PS-1 gene (an exon 9 deletion) is often associated with early
spasticity (see arrow in Fig. 2) [25].
pedigrees, which is consistent with the founder effect hypothesis. All PS-2
mutations are also missense mutations.
Clinical features associated with PS-2 mutations have been reported pri-
marily in the VG families. The mean age of onset in these families is
54.9 ± 8.5 years, and mean disease duration is 7.6 ± 3.2 years. Within the
VG families, there is high variability in age of onset, ranging from 40 to
75 years [26]. Ths PS-2 mutation is highly penetrant (>95%). The dementia
in PS-2 AD is clinically and neuropathologically indistinguishable from that
of sporadic AD.
Apolipoprotein E
The APOE gene was initially identified as a genetic risk factor in AD by
genetic linkage analysis of late-onset FAD pedigrees [28]. Because APOE
was known to be present in amyloid plaques and neurofibrillary tangles,
these observations made APOE a plausible candidate gene. A strong allelic
association between APOE e4 and AD was established in 1993 [29,30]
and was rapidly confirmed in autopsy-proven sporadic and familial late-
onset AD cases. AD risk associated with APOE is dose dependent
[29,31]. The presence of the e4 allele seems to modify the age of onset of
AD [32]. Compared with the most common APOE genotype (e3/e3), odds
ratios range from 2.8 to 4.4 for AD subjects with one e4 allele compared
with normal controls; the odds ratio increases from 7.0 to 19.3 for subjects
with two e4 alleles [33,34]. These risk estimates are not as strongly observed
in blacks or Hispanics (reviewed by Farrer et al [33]), although Hispanics
with an e4 allele and blacks who are e4 homozygous remain at increased
risk for developing AD [35]. Studies suggest a different e4 allele effect in
men than in women. In men, only e4/e4 homozygotes have a younger age
of onset; whereas one e4 allele is sufficient to reduce the age of onset in
women [36]. The APOE e4 risk seems to be more pronounced in women
[32]. This study reported that women with the e4/e4 genotype (approxi-
mately 1% of the general population) have a 40% risk of developing AD
by the age of 73 years. Not all studies support these findings, however. In
addition, several studies suggest a reduced frequency of the APOE e2 allele
in AD patients [37,38].
Vascular dementia
Clinical features
Binswanger (1894) and Alzheimer (1911) described behavioral disorders
related to arteriosclerosis. Initially, these conditions were categorized as
598 D.W. Tsuang, T.D. Bird / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 591–614
Notch3
Linkage analysis mapped the CADASIL gene to the short arm of chro-
mosome 19. Mutations in the Notch3 gene were first reported in 1996.
Subsequent studies reported that these mutations may be present in indivi-
duals without a positive family history [56]. The frequency of Notch3 muta-
tions in sporadic and familial cases with vascular dementia remains unclear
but seems to be rare. In addition, mutations in the APP and cystatin-b genes
are rare causes of cerebral amyloid angiopathy and hemorrhagic strokes.
D.W. Tsuang, T.D. Bird / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 591–614 599
Huntington’s disease
Clinical features
The clinical triad in HD includes chorea, cognitive impairment, and beha-
vioral disturbances. Chorea is the main motor sign in HD. These involuntary
movements are present only during waking hours. Typically, they cannot
be voluntarily suppressed and can increase with stress. Some patients may
develop bradykinesia, rigidity, and dystonia. Aspiration secondary to dys-
phagia is the most common cause of mortality and morbidity. A typical pat-
tern of cognitive decline includes slowness of thought and impaired ability
to integrate new knowledge, particularly new motor skills, and a lack of aware-
ness of one’s own disability. Visuospatial memory is particularly affected,
whereas verbal memory remains preserved until late in the course of the dis-
ease. Patients may be able to remember facts, stories, and words but have dif-
ficulty copying designs on the Mini-Mental State Examination. Orientation
to time and place remains intact until late in the illness. Changes in mood and
personality are common, ranging from irritability to prolonged periods of
depression as well as psychosis [57]. Suicide is more common in patients with
HD than in the general population. Psychiatric symptoms may precede motor
signs and symptoms by many years and do not necessarily relate to the sever-
ity of chorea or dementia. Other symptoms commonly observed include
apathy, aggressive behavior, sexual disinhibition, and alcohol abuse.
The diagnosis of HD is indicated by the presence of a positive family his-
tory of an autosomal dominant degenerative disorder consistent with HD;
presence of progressive motor disability, including voluntary and involun-
tary movements; cognitive decline; and behavioral disturbances. Caudate
atrophy on CT or MRI provides additional support for the diagnosis. Finally,
DNA analysis confirms the diagnosis. The mean age of onset in HD is
approximately 40 years [58], although the age of onset ranges from 2 to
80 years. The duration of HD is typically 15 years, with the age of death
ranging from 51 to 63 years.
Pathologic features
The primary pathologic feature in HD is neuronal loss in the corpus stria-
tum, occurring first in the caudate and later in the globus pallidus. Caudate
600 D.W. Tsuang, T.D. Bird / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 591–614
Epidemiology
Many epidemiologic studies have been performed worldwide. There is
general agreement that the prevalence of HD in Western European coun-
tries is between 3 and 10 cases per 100,000 persons. The rates are lower in
Japan, China, and Finland as well as in African black populations. George
Huntington, who first described the syndrome in 1872 [61], noted clear
familial aggregation in HD.
Genetics
HD is inherited as an autosomal dominant trait. In 1983, HD became the
first genetic disorder to be linked by restriction fragment length polymor-
phism markers to a locus on chromosome 4. [62] Ten years later, an interna-
tional research consortium reported the successful cloning and sequencing of
the HD gene [63]. A novel gene containing a trinucleotide repeat (CAG) that
is repeated beyond the normal range is associated with HD. This highly poly-
morphic CAG repeat is located in the 5¢ region of the HD gene (Fig. 3).
Individuals with symptomatic HD have more than 36 CAG repeats in the
HD gene. Individuals with the greatest number of repeats (>60) are more likely
to have juvenile-onset illness. Most adult-onset HD cases have 38 to 50 CAG
repeats. A significant correlation between the number of CAG repeats and age
of onset of HD has been demonstrated [64–67]. The association was the great-
est in patients with higher CAG repeats (>60), who tended to have a lower age
of onset. But the number of CAG repeats is not useful in predicting the age
of onset or type of symptoms at presentation in individual patients, however.
HD is the first of numerous neurodegenerative disorders associated with a
trinucleotide repeat expansion. Several other neuropsychiatric disorders (eg,
fragile X mental retardation, hereditary ataxias) that exhibit anticipation are
also trinucleotide repeat disorders [68,69]. Genetic testing issues in HD are
complex and require careful thought and counseling [70,71].
Interestingly, the discovery of dynamic repeat mutations helps to account
for the long-observed clinical phenomenon of anticipation. Anticipation is
D.W. Tsuang, T.D. Bird / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 591–614 601
Fig. 3. The Huntington’s disease (HD) gene on chromosome 4. Genetic markers are indicated
at the top of the figure. D4S10 was the initial marker linked to HD. The bold lines indicate the
HD gene, which was called Interesting Transcript-15. This novel gene contains highly
polymorphic trinucleotide repeats, (CAG)n, at the 5¢ end of the HD gene. Individuals with
symptomatic HD have more than 36 CAG repeats at this locus. (Courtesy of Elisabeth
Almqvist, PhD, Stockholm, Sweden).
the observation that a disease becomes more severe and appears earlier with
each successive generation. As in other trinucleotide repeat disorders, this is
a result of the unstable expansion of the CAG trinucleotide repeats when the
disorder is passed from parent to offspring. In addition, paternal HD alleles
are more likely to undergo significant expansion than maternal alleles,
resulting in the observation that those individuals with larger repeat sizes are
more likely to have affected fathers.
Frontotemporal dementia
Clinical features
Initially, Pick described a clinical syndrome with dementia, progressive
aphasia, and frontal cortical atrophy [72]. Neuronal cytoplasmic inclusions
(Pick bodies) were observed later in neuropathologic studies of some cases.
Because most patients with dementia and prominent frontal lobe dysfunction
do not have Pick bodies, confusion has reigned in the nosology of frontotem-
poral dementia (FTD). Terminology has included Pick’s disease, Pick com-
plex, non-AD dementia, disinhibition-dementia-parkinsonism-amyotrophy
complex, and frontal lobe degeneration with spinal motor degeneration.
In the 1970s, clinical and pathologic studies helped to solidify consensus
diagnostic criteria for FTD [73–75]. Specifically, the discovery of genetic
mutations in some FTD families with taupathies (eg, FTD, parkinsonism-
linked to chromosome 17 [FTDP-17]) has resulted in new diagnostic cate-
gories (see below).
Typically, the clinical presentation of FTD includes personality or behav-
ioral change (often disinhibition) with a relatively intact memory. Later in the
course of disease, there may be marked confusion, mutism, and parkinsonian
features. There are at least three subtypes of FTD, including progressive
602 D.W. Tsuang, T.D. Bird / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 591–614
Pathologic features
Pathologically, there is frontal or temporal lobar atrophy. Many cases
have only gliosis and neuronal loss without distinctive features. Other
cases have cytoplasmic inclusions that may be typical Pick bodies or other
varieties of tau-positive material, sometimes resembling neurofibrillary tan-
gles. Classic Pick’s disease with Pick bodies is considered a subtype of FTD.
Some cases also have anterior horn cell loss in the spinal cord.
Epidemiology
Incidence and prevalence estimates of FTD are not well established,
partly due to the clinical heterogeneity of the disorder. The only available
sample estimating disease incidence is based on clinician referrals. In this
study [77], the prevalence rises from 1.2 to 28 cases per 1 million persons from
the third decade to the sixth decade. But because this was not a population-
based study, this is probably an underestimate. Among all patients with
dementia, FTD is thought to comprise approximately 10% of cases (and prob-
ably more in younger age groups).
FTD is the most common syndrome with prominent frontal lobe degen-
eration. It is commonly misdiagnosed as AD, and less commonly as DLB
or AD with vascular disease. Most FTD cases are misdiagnosed as AD. In
the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease neuropatho-
logic studies, FTD-like pathologic findings were observed in 3% to 9% of
patients with a clinical diagnosis of AD [78]. The frequency of FTD in
autopsy case series with dementia varies from 0% to 15% [79].
Genetics
Familial aggregation was the first feature of FTD suggesting that there
may be an underlying genetic cause. Several groups reported a positive
family history in 10% to 60% of cases [77,80]. Segregation analyses have sug-
gested that first-degree relatives of FTD patients are 3.5 times more likely to
develop dementia than first-degree relatives of normal controls. It has also
been suggested that age of onset in relatives of FTD patients is, on average,
D.W. Tsuang, T.D. Bird / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 591–614 603
11 years younger than in other dementia patients [77]. There are clearly a
few large families with multiple affected individuals in which FTD seems
to segregate in a highly penetrant and autosomal dominant fashion. The
success of gene identification (see below) in families with atypical dementia
not only confirmed the genetic basis of FTD but established it as a distinct
clinical and pathologic entity.
Tau gene
The modified product of the tau gene is a major component of NFTs seen
in AD. The tau gene is large, with 100,000 base pairs of DNA and 15 exons.
It also exhibits complex splicing (see Fig. 4). There are commonly six alter-
natively spliced isoforms of the tau gene involving exons 2, 3, and 10. All but
one of the currently identified mutations affect microtubule binding do-
mains. Most of these mutations are missense, appearing in the coding re-
gions as well as in the noncoding regions (introns). Some mutations are
believed to cause disease by producing functional changes that interfere with
the normal binding of microtubules, whereas other mutations appear to
change the ratio of tau isoforms in the brain (3 and 4 repeat tau). The discov-
ery of tau mutations in families with FTDP-17 has confirmed the fact that
genetics plays a role in a subgroup of FTD cases. More work needs to be
done to determine the range of tau mutations in FTD and related disorders.
604 D.W. Tsuang, T.D. Bird / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 591–614
Fig. 4. The tau gene on chromosome 17 has a complex genomic structure with more than 15
exons spread over 100,000 base pairs of genomic DNA (top of figure). It undergoes complex
differential splicing. Exons 2, 3, and 10 are alternatively spliced, making up six commonly
alternatively spliced isoforms of the tau gene. The large bold arrow points to the first tau
mutation (V337M) associated with frontotemporal dementia, which was discovered in exon 12.
(Courtesy of Parvoneh Poorkaj, PhD, Seattle, WA).
Prion disease
Although prion diseases are relatively uncommon, they exemplify both
transmissible and heritable forms of dementia. What we now know as prion
diseases were first described in the 1800s, with reports of scrapie in sheep.
Scrapie was shown to be experimentally transmissible in 1936 [96]. Human
prion diseases were recognized in the 1920s by Creutzfeldt and Jakob and
D.W. Tsuang, T.D. Bird / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 591–614 605
Fig. 5. The human prion diseases include Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease, kuru, Gerstmann-
Sträussler-Scheinker disease, and fatal familial insomnia. The animal prion diseases include
scrapie, transmissible mink encephalopathy, and bovine spongiform encephalopathy. Because
these diseases all share the property of transmissibility and the characteristic pathologic finding
of spongiform changes, they are often collectively referred to as transmissible spongiform
encephalopathies.
606 D.W. Tsuang, T.D. Bird / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 591–614
Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease
Sporadic CJD most often affects patients in their 50s and 60s. The typical
clinical presentation includes rapid progressive cognitive decline (<2 years to
death) accompanied by a variety of neurologic signs (most commonly rigid-
ity, ataxia, and myoclonus) and characteristic synchronous spikes on the
electroencephalogram in an afebrile individual. The classic symptoms occur
in less than 60% of cases, however. Other clinical features may include psy-
chotic symptoms resembling schizophrenia as well as extrapyramidal and
cerebellar dysfunction or akinetic mutism.
The diagnosis of CJD should be entertained in individuals with rapidly
progressive neuropsychiatric disorders. Clinical diagnostic criteria have
been established by a large CJD surveillance group in Europe [103],
although definitive diagnosis can only be made on neuropathologic or bio-
chemical examination of the brain. The neuropathologic hallmarks of CJD
include spongiform degeneration, neuronal loss, and astrocytic gliosis.
PrPSc-positive kuru plaques and other PrP-containing amyloid plaques are
pathognomic of prion disease. They are almost exclusively found in familial
CJD cases with PRNP mutations. A cerebrospinal fluid test for the 14-3-3
protein has been found to be diagnostically useful.
ataxia, and myoclonus. The age of onset is in the 50s and 60s, with the dis-
ease duration ranging from 9 months to 4 years. The electroencephalogram
shows generalized slowing rather than periodic triphasic waves. Neuro-
pathologic findings include diffuse spongiform degeneration in the cerebral
cortex and basal ganglia with relative sparing of the thalamus.
Alternatively, in families with the same D178N mutation but with
methionine at position 129, the phenotype is that of FFI. These patients
often present with insomnia and dysautonomia. They may later show signs
of ataxia, dysarthria, myoclonus, and pyramidal tract dysfunction. In the
later stages, patients exhibit complete insomnia, dementia, rigidity, dysto-
nia, and mutism. The duration of illness is short (mean ¼ 13 months).
Neuropathologically, FFI is characterized by neuronal loss and astrocy-
tic gliosis preferentially affecting the thalamus. At least 21 families with FFI-
D178N mutations have been reported [106]. It is unclear why the codon
129 genotype dramatically influences the phenotype associated with the
D178N mutation, but it presumably affects the three-dimensional structure
of PrP.
Another phenotype, the GSS syndrome, is caused by several different
mutations in the PRNP gene [107]. Clinical symptoms include early ataxia,
dementia, dysphagia, dysarthria, and hyporeflexia. Patients with GSS are
more likely to exhibit ataxia than patients with CJD. Conversely, patients
with CJD are more likely to have dementia and myoclonus. Clinical symp-
toms often overlap, however, and do not always ‘‘breed true’’ within families.
As such, family members with the same PRNP mutation may have either
phenotype. GSS is always considered to be solely genetic and often has a
longer disease duration than CJD. Neuropathologically, GSS is distinct from
CJD in that GSS is characterized by the presence of large multicentric PrP-
containing amyloid plaques with variable spongiform changes.
The most common mutation associated with GSS is the P102L mutation.
More than 30 affected families in the Northern Hemisphere have been
described to date [108]. This was the first mutation to be formally linked
to a human prion disease and is the causative mutation originally described
by Gerstmann, Straüssler, and Scheinker [109].
The clinical and neuropathologic characteristics of these families are indis-
tinguishable from those of sporadic CJD. Interestingly, when infected brain
tissue from CJD E200K patients was injected into primates, the disease was
transmitted in most trials, but transmission has not been demonstrated in
other families with different mutations [110]. This finding is similar to the
results observed in experiments using brain tissue from sporadic CJD cases.
Mitochondrial disorders
Mitochondrial disorders are clinically diverse and are defined by structu-
ral or functional abnormalities in the mitochondria or mitochondrial DNA
608 D.W. Tsuang, T.D. Bird / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 591–614
Summary
Many neurodegenerative diseases are exceedingly complex disorders
(Fig. 6). In the past decade, we have made tremendous advances in our under-
D.W. Tsuang, T.D. Bird / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 591–614 609
Fig. 6. This diagram demonstrates the concept that the Alzheimer’s disease (AD) phenotype (as
well as the other neurodegenerative conditions) is phenotypically heterogeneous. On the left are
the four known genetic factors associated with AD. There are likely other early-onset as well
as late-onset genes yet to be discovered. On the right are four potential nongenetic causes of
AD that presently remain speculative. In summary, the bottom arrow shows that most cases of
AD in the general population may be the result of a complex interplay between environment,
genetic predisposition, and aging.
Acknowledgements
The authors thank Lillian DiGiacomo, BA, and Charisma Eugenio,
BS, for their editorial assistance and Molly Wamble, BA, for her technical
assistance.
References
[1] Pulst SM. Neurogenetics. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2000.
[2] Terry RD, Katzman R, Bick KL, et al. Alzheimer disease. 2nd edition. Philadelphia:
Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 1999.
[3] Alzheimer A. Ueber eigenartigen Krankheitsfalle des spateren Alters. Zentralblatt fur die
gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie 1911;4:356–8.
[4] Adams JH, Duchen LW. Greenfield’s neuropathology. 5th edition. New York: Oxford
University Press; 1992. p. 1557.
[5] Seshadri S, Wolf PA, Beiser A, et al. Lifetime risk of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease.
The impact of mortality on risk estimates in the Framingham Study. Neurology 1997;49:
1498–504.
[6] Levy-Lahad E, Tsuang D, Bird TD. Recent advances in the genetics of Alzheimer’s
disease. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 1998;11:42–54.
[7] Launer LJ, Andersen K, Dewey ME, et al. Rates and risk factors for dementia and
Alzheimer’s disease: results from EURODEM pooled analyses. EURODEM Incidence
Research Group and Work Groups. European Studies of Dementia. Neurology 1999;
52:78–84.
[8] Bergem AL, Engedal K, Kringlen E. The role of heredity in late-onset Alzheimer disease
and vascular dementia. A twin study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1997;54:264–70.
[9] Mann DMA. Association between Alzheimer’s disease and Down syndrome: neuro-
pathological observations. In: Berg JM, Karlinksy J, Holland AJ, editors. Alzheimer
disease, Down syndrome, and their relationship. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1993.
p. 71–92.
[10] Goate A, Chartier-Harlin MC, Mullan M, et al. Segregation of a missense mutation in the
amyloid precursor protein gene with familial Alzheimer’s disease. Nature 1991;349:704–6.
[11] Bird T, Sumi S, Nemens E. Phenotypic heterogeneity of familial Alzheimer’s disease:
a study of 24 kindreds. Ann Neurol 1989;24:12–25.
[12] Sisodia SS, Koo EH, Beyreuther K, et al. Evidence that beta-amyloid protein in
Alzheimer’s disease is not derived by normal processing. Science 1990;248:492–5.
[13] Selkoe DJ. The cell biology of beta-amyloid precursor protein and presenilin in
Alzheimer’s disease. Trends Cell Biol 1998;8:447–53.
[14] Nunan J, Small DH. Regulation of APP cleavage by alpha-, beta- and gamma-secretases.
FEBS Lett 2000;483:6–10.
[15] Golde TE, Eckman CB, Younkin SG. Biochemical detection of Abeta isoforms: impli-
cations for pathogenesis, diagnosis, and treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. Biochim Biophys
Acta 2000;1502:172–87.
[16] Selkoe DJ. Alzheimer’s disease: genes, proteins, and therapy. Physiol Rev 2001;81:741–66.
[17] Van Broeckhoven C, Haan J, Bakker E, et al. Amyloid beta protein precursor gene and
hereditary cerebral hemorrhage with amyloidosis [in Dutch]. Science 1990;248:1120–2.
[18] Schellenberg GD, Bird TD, Wijsman EM, et al. Genetic linkage evidence for a familial
Alzheimer’s disease locus on chromosome 14. Science 1992;258:668–71.
[19] Sherrington R, Rogaev E, Liang Y, et al. Cloning of a gene bearing missense mutations in
early-onset familial Alzheimer’s disease. Nature 1995;375:754–60.
[20] Cruts M, Van Broeckhoven C. Presenilin mutations in Alzheimer’s disease. Hum Mutat
1998;11:183–90.
D.W. Tsuang, T.D. Bird / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 591–614 611
[21] Selkoe D, Wolfe M. In search of gamma-secretase: presenilin at the cutting edge. Proc
Natl Acad Sci USA 2000;97:5690–2.
[22] Steinbart EJ, Smith CO, Poorkaj P, et al. Impact of DNA testing for early-onset familial
Alzheimer disease and frontotemporal dementia. Arch Neurol 2001;58:1828–31.
[23] Haltia M, Viitanen M, Sulkava R, et al. Chromosome 14-encoded Alzheimer’s disease:
genetic and clinicopathological description. Ann Neurol 1994;36:362–7.
[24] Lampe TH, Bird TD, Nochlin D, et al. Phenotype of chromosome 14-linked familial
Alzheimer’s disease in a large kindred. Ann Neurol 1994;36:368–78.
[25] Verkkoniemi A, Somer M, Rinne JO, et al. Variant Alzheimer’s disease with spastic
paraparesis: clinical characterization. Neurology 2000;54:1103–9.
[26] Levy-Lahad E, Wijsman EM, Nemens E, et al. A familial Alzheimer’s disease locus on
chromosome 1. Science 1995;269:970–3.
[27] Levy-Lahad E, Poorkaj P, Wang K, et al. Genomic structure and expression of STM2, the
chromosome 1 familial Alzheimer disease gene. Genomics 1996;34:198–204.
[28] Pericak-Vance MA, Bebout JL, Gaskell PC, Jr, et al. Linkage studies in familial
Alzheimer disease: evidence for chromosome 19 linkage. Am J Hum Genet 1991;48:
1034–50.
[29] Corder E, Saunders A, Strittmatter W, et al. Gene dose of apolipoprotein E type 4 allele
and the risk of Alzheimer’s disease in late onset families. Science 1993;261:921–3.
[30] Saunders A, Strittmatter W, Schmechel D, et al. Association of apolipoprotein E allele
epsilon 4 with late-onset familial and sporadic Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 1993;43:
1467–72.
[31] Tsai MS, Tangalos EG, Petersen RC, et al. Apolipoprotein E: risk factor for Alzheimer
disease. Am J Hum Genet 1994;54:643–9.
[32] Breitner JC, Wyse BW, Anthony JC, et al. APOE-epsilon4 count predicts age when preva-
lence of AD increases, then declines: the Cache County Study. Neurology 1999;53:321–31.
[33] Farrer LA, Cupples LA, Haines JL, et al. Effects of age, sex, and ethnicity on the asso-
ciation between apolipoprotein E genotype and Alzheimer disease. A meta-analysis. APOE
and Alzheimer Disease Meta Analysis Consortium. JAMA 1997;278:1349–56.
[34] Mayeux R, Stern Y, Ottman R, et al. The apolipoprotein epsilon 4 allele in patients with
Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol 1993;34:752–4.
[35] Tang MX, Maestre G, Tsai WY, et al. Relative risk of Alzheimer disease and age-at-onset
distributions, based on APOE genotypes among elderly African Americans, Caucasians,
and Hispanics in New York City. Am J Hum Genet 1996;58:574–84.
[36] Payami H, Montee KR, Kaye JA, et al. Alzheimer’s disease, apolipoprotein E4, and
gender. JAMA 1994;271:1316–7.
[37] Corder EH, Saunders AM, Risch NJ, et al. Protective effect of apolipoprotein E type 2
allele for late onset Alzheimer disease. Nat Genet 1994;7:180–4.
[38] Panza F, Solfrizzi V, Torres F, et al. Apolipoprotein E in Southern Italy: protective effect
of epsilon 2 allele in early- and late-onset sporadic Alzheimer’s disease. Neurosci Lett
2000;292:79–82.
[39] Roses AD, Saunders AM. Apolipoprotein E genotyping as a diagnostic adjunct for
Alzheimer’s disease. Int Psychogeriatr 1997;9(Suppl 1):277–88.
[40] Tsuang D, Larson EB, Bowen J, et al. The utility of apolipoprotein E genotyping in the
diagnosis of Alzheimer disease in a community-based case series. Arch Neurol 1999;
56:1489–95.
[41] Schellenberg G, D’Souza I, Poorkaj P. The genetics of Alzheimer’s disease. Current
Psychiat Reports 2000;2:158–64.
[42] Blacker D, Wilcox MA, Laird N, et al. Alpha-2 macroglobulin is genetically associated
with Alzheimer disease. Nat Genet 1998;19:357–60.
[43] Lendon C, Talbot C, Craddock N, et al. Genetic association studies between dementia of
the Alzheimer’s type and three receptors of apolipoprotein E in a Caucasian population.
Neurosci Lett 1997;222:187–90.
612 D.W. Tsuang, T.D. Bird / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 591–614
[44] Blennow K, Ricksten A, Prince JA, et al. No association between the alpha-2 macro-
globulin (A2M) deletion and Alzheimer’s disease, and no change in A2M mRNA, protein,
or protein expression. J Neural Transm 2000;107:1065–79.
[45] Rudrasingham V, Wavrant-De Vrieze F, Lambert JC, et al. Alpha-2 macroglobulin gene
and Alzheimer disease. Nat Genet 1999;22:17–9.
[46] Bertram L, Blacker D, Mullin K, et al. Evidence for genetic linkage of Alzheimer’s disease
to chromosome 10q. Science 2000;290:2302–3.
[47] Ertekin-Taner N, Graff-Radford N, Younkin LH, et al. Linkage of plasma Abeta42 to a
quantitative locus on chromosome 10 in late-onset Alzheimer’s disease pedigrees. Science
2000;290:2303–4.
[48] McKeith IG, Galasko D, Kosaka K, et al. Consensus guidelines for the clinical and
pathologic diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB): report of the Consortium on
DLB International Workshop. Neurology 1996;47:1113–24.
[49] Ballard C, Holmes C, McKeith I, et al. Psychiatric morbidity in dementia with Lewy
bodies: a prospective clinical and neuropathological comparative study with Alzheimer’s
disease. Am J Psychiatry 1999;156:1039–45.
[50] Denson MA, Wszolek ZK, Pfeiffer RF, et al. Familial parkinsonism, dementia, and Lewy
body disease: study of family G. Ann Neurol 1997;42:638–43.
[51] Ohara K, Takauchi S, Kokai M, et al. Familial dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB). Clin
Neuropathol 1999;18:232–9.
[52] Kruger R, Kuhn W, Leenders KL, et al. Familial parkinsonism with synuclein pathology:
clinical and PET studies of A30P mutation carriers. Neurology 2001;56:1355–62.
[53] Spira PJ, Sharpe DM, Halliday G, et al. Clinical and pathological features of a
Parkinsonian syndrome in a family with an Ala53Thr alpha-synuclein mutation. Ann
Neurol 2001;49:313–9.
[54] Lucking CB, Durr A, Bonifati V, et al. Association between early-onset Parkinson’s
disease and mutations in the parkin gene. French Parkinson’s Disease Genetics Study
Group. N Engl J Med 2000;342:1560–7.
[55] Salloway S, Hong J. CADASIL syndrome: a genetic form of vascular dementia. J Geriatr
Psychiatry Neurol 1998;11:71–7.
[56] Joutel A, Dodick DD, Parisi JE, et al. De novo mutation in the Notch3 gene causing
CADASIL. Ann Neurol 2000;47:388–91.
[57] Folstein MF. Differential diagnosis of dementia. The clinical process. Psychiatr Clin
North Am 1997;20:45–57.
[58] Harper PS. Huntington’s disease. London: W.B. Saunders; 1991.
[59] Vonsattel J-P, Myers R, Stevens T, et al. Neuropathological classification of Huntington’s
disease. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 1985;44:559–77.
[60] Davies SW, Turmaine M, Cozens BA, et al. Formation of neuronal intranuclear
inclusions underlies the neurological dysfunction in mice transgenic for the HD mutation.
Cell 1997;90:537–48.
[61] Huntington G. On chorea. Medical and Surgical Reporter 1872;26:320–1.
[62] Gusella JF, MacDonald ME, Ambrose CM, et al. Molecular genetics of Huntington’s
disease. Arch Neurol 1993;50:1157–63.
[63] Huntington’s Disease Collaborative Research Group. A novel gene containing a
trinucleotide repeat that is expanded and unstable on Huntington’s disease chromosomes.
The Huntington’s Disease Collaborative Research Group. Cell 1993;72:971–83.
[64] Andrew S, Goldberg Y, Kremer B, et al. The relationship between trinucleotide (CAG)
repeat length and clinical features of Huntington’s disease. Nat Genet 1993;4:398–403.
[65] Duyao M, Ambrose C, Myers R, et al. Trinucleotide repeat length instability and age of
onset in Huntington’s disease. Nat Genet 1993;4:387–92.
[66] Norremolle A, Riess O, Epplen J, et al. Trinucleotide repeat elongation in the Huntington
gene in Huntington disease patients from 71 Danish families. Hum Mol Genet 1993;2:
1475–6.
D.W. Tsuang, T.D. Bird / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 591–614 613
[91] Baker M, Litvan I, Houlden H, et al. Association of an extended haplotype in the tau gene
with progressive supranuclear palsy. Hum Mol Genet 1999;8:711–5.
[92] Higgins I, Litvan I, Pho L, et al. Progressive supranuclear gaze palsy in linkage
disequilibrium with tau and not the alpha-synuclein gene. Neurology 1998;50:270–3.
[93] Poorkaj P, Tsuang D, Wijsman E, et al. Tau as a susceptibility gene for amyotropic lateral
sclerosis-parkinsonism dementia complex of Guam. Arch Neurol 2001;58:1871–8.
[94] Houlden H, Baker M, Adamson J, et al. Frequency of tau mutations in three series of
non-Alzheimer’s degenerative dementia. Ann Neurol 1999;46:243–8.
[95] Poorkaj P, Grossman M, Steinbart E, et al. Frequency of tau gene mutations in familial
and sporadic cases of non-Alzheimer dementia. Arch Neurol 2001;58:383–7.
[96] Cuille J, Chelle P-L. La maladie dite tremblante du mouton est-elle inoculable? C R Acad
Sci III 1936;203:1552–4.
[97] Creutzfeldt HG. Uber eine eigenartige herdformige Erkrankung des Zentralnervensys-
tems. Z Ges Neurol Psychiatrie 1920;57:1–18.
[98] Jakob A. Uber eigenartige Erkrankungen des Zentralnervensystems mit bemerkenswertem
anatomischem Befunde (spastische Pseudosklerose-Encephalomyelopathie mit dissemi-
nierten Degenerationsherden). Dtsch Z Nervenheilkd 1921;70:132–46.
[99] Kretzschmar HA, Prusiner SB, Stowring LE, et al. Scrapie prion proteins are synthesized
in neurons. Am J Pathol 1986;122:1–5.
[100] Manson J, West JD, Thomson V, et al. The prion protein gene: a role in mouse
embryogenesis? Development 1992;115:117–22.
[101] Moser M, Colello RJ, Pott U, et al. Developmental expression of the prion protein gene in
glial cells. Neuron 1995;14:509–17.
[102] Prusiner S. Molecular biology and genetics of prion diseases. Cold Spring Harb Symp
Quant Biol 1996;61:473–93.
[103] Concerted Action of the EU. Surveillance of Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease in the European
community. Rome: Concerted Action of the EU; 1993/1994.
[104] Mastrianni JA. The prion diseases: Creutzfeldt-Jakob, Gerstmann-Straussler-Scheinker,
and related disorders. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 1998;11:78–97.
[105] Gabizon R, Rosenmann H, Meiner Z, et al. Mutation and polymorphism of the prion
protein gene in Libyan Jews with Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease (CJD). Am J Hum Genet
1993;53:828–35.
[106] Gambetti P, Lugaresi E. Conclusions of the symposium. Brain Pathol 1998;8:571–5.
[107] Windl O, Kretzschmar HA. Prion diseases. In: Pulst SM, editor. Neurogenetics. Oxford:
Oxford University Press; 2000. p. 198–218.
[108] Young K, Jones CK, Piccardo P, et al. Gerstmann-Straussler-Scheinker disease with
mutation at codon 102 and methionine at codon 129 of PRNP in previously unreported
patients. Neurology 1995;45:1127–34.
[109] Hsiao K, Baker HF, Crow TJ, et al. Linkage of a prion protein missense variant to
Gerstmann-Straussler syndrome. Nature 1989;338:342–5.
[110] Chapman J, Brown P, Rabey J, et al. Transmission of spongiform encephalopathy from a
familial Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease patient of Jewish Libyan origin carrying the PRNP
codon 200 mutation. Neurology 1992;42:1249–50.
[111] Bonilla E, Tanji K, Hirano M, et al. Mitochondrial involvement in Alzheimer’s disease.
Biochim Biophys Acta 1999;1410:171–82.
[112] Chinnery PF, Taylor GA, Howell N, et al. Point mutations of the mtDNA control region
in normal and neurodegenerative human brains. Am J Hum Genet 2001;68:529–32.
Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 615–627
Growing evidence over the past decade has led to the realization that
many sporadic and familial neurodegenerative diseases are characterized
by distinct hallmark brain lesions formed by filamentous deposits of abnor-
mal brain proteins, and a group of heterogeneous disorders characterized
neuropathologically by prominent intracellular accumulations of abnormal
tau filaments may share common disease mechanisms (for recent reviews,
see [1–3]). Despite their diverse phenotypic manifestations, these disorders
are collectively known as neurodegenerative tauopathies (Table 1), and the
absence of other disease-specific neuropathological abnormalities in several
tauopathies provided circumstantial evidence implicating tau abnormalities
in the onset/progression of neurodegeneration [1–3]. However, this was con-
troversial until 1998 when multiple tau gene mutations were discovered in
families with frontotemporal dementia and parkinsonism linked to chromo-
some 17 (FTDP-17) and provided unequivocal evidence that tau abnormal-
ities alone are sufficient to cause neurodegenerative disease [4–8]. This
opened up new avenues for investigating the role of tau abnormalities in
mechanisms of brain dysfunction and degeneration in Alzheimer’s disease
(AD) and related neurodegenerative disorders [1–3]. Indeed, these dramatic
discoveries re-focused attention on the role of tau pathologies in mecha-
nisms of brain degeneration in AD, and here we review recent insights into
these aspects of the pathobiology of AD.
* Corresponding author.
E-mail address: [email protected] (J.Q. Trojanowski).
0025-7125/02/$ - see front matter 2002, Elsevier Science (USA). All rights reserved.
PII: S 0 0 2 5 - 7 1 2 5 ( 0 2 ) 0 0 0 0 2 - 0
616 J.Q. Trojanowski, V.M.-Y. Lee / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 615–627
Table 1
Neurodegenerative disease with tau pathology
Alzheimer’s disease
Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis/parkinsonism-dementia complex*
Argyrophilic grain dementia*
Corticobasal degeneration*
Dementia pugilistica*
Diffuse neurofibrillary tangles with calcification*
Down’s syndrome
Frontotemporal dementia with parkinsonism linked to chromosome 17*
Multiple system atrophy
Myotonic dystrophy
Neurodegeneration with brain iron accumulation type 1 (formerly Hallevorden-Spatz disease)
Niemann-Pick disease, type C
Pick’s disease*
Post-encephalitic parkinsonism
Prion diseases
Progressive subcortical gliosis*
Progressive supranuclear palsy*
Subacute sclerosing panencephalitis
Tangle only dementia*
* Diseases in which tau pathologies are the most predominant neurodegenerative brain
lesions.
neurons, and they also are found in axons of peripheral nervous system
(PNS) neurons, but they are barely detectable in CNS astrocytes and oligo-
dendrocytes [9,10–14]. Human tau is encoded by a single gene consisting of
16 exons on chromosome 17q21, and the CNS isoforms are generated by
alternative mRNA splicing of 11 exons [15–17]. In the adult human brain,
alternative splicing of exons (E) 2 (E2), 3 (E3) and 10 (E10) generates six tau
isoforms ranging from 352 to 441 amino acids in length which differ by the
presence of either three (3R-tau) or four (4R-tau) carboxy-terminal tandem
repeat sequences of 31 or 32 amino acids that are encoded by E9, E10, E11
and E12 [15,18,19]. Additionally, the three 3R-tau and 4R-tau isoforms dif-
fer due to alternative splicing of E2 and E3 that results in tau isoforms with-
out (0N) or with either 29 (1N) or 58 (2N) amino acid inserts of unknown
functions. In the adult human brain, the ratio of 3R-tau to 4R-tau isoforms
is 1, but the 1N, 0N and 2N tau isoforms comprise about 54%, 37% and 9%
of total tau, respectively [20,21]. Further, tau is developmentally regulated
leading to expression of only the shortest tau isoform (3R/0N) in fetal brain,
but all six isoforms are expressed in the adult human brain. In the PNS,
inclusion of E4a in the amino-terminal half results in the expression of the
largest tau isoform [12,22,23].
Several functions of tau have been extensively characterized (for review
[1–3]). For example, tau binds to and stabilizes microtubules (MTs), in addi-
tion to promoting MT polymerization through MT binding domains in
the carboxy-terminal half of tau that are composed of highly conserved
J.Q. Trojanowski, V.M.-Y. Lee / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 615–627 617
Pathological tau in AD
In contrast to tauopathies, wherein filamentous neuronal and/or glial tau
inclusions are the only defining neuropathological features in affected brain
regions, tau-rich NFTs and neuropil threads co-occur with deposits of Ab
fibrils in the extracellular space as diffuse and senile plaques as well as in
blood vessel walls of AD brains (reviewed in [1–3]). The neurofibrillary tau
lesions in AD brains were first shown to be stained with anti-tau antibodies
more than 15 years ago [74,75], but more than 20 years earlier [76] electron
microscopical (EM) studies had revealed that the major structural compo-
nents of NFTs were PHFs and to a lesser extent straight filaments (SFs).
Subsequent EM studies demonstrated that PHFs contain two strands of
fibrils twisted around one another with a periodicity of 80 nm and a width
varying from 8 to 20 nm, while SFs do not demonstrate a similar helical
periodicity [77]. However, after the appearance of a number of conflicting
reports on the composition of these filaments in the early 1980s, the building
block subunits of both PHFs and SFs were shown to be abnormally phos-
phorylated tau proteins [16,68,78–83]. For example, biochemical analysis of
J.Q. Trojanowski, V.M.-Y. Lee / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 615–627 619
Summary
Despite earlier uncertainties about the role of tau patholgy in AD, the
discovery of multiple mutations in the tau gene that lead to the abnormal
aggregation of tau and the onset/progression of FTDP-17 demonstrates that
tau dysfunction is sufficient to produce neurodegenerative disease. The
mutations lead to specific cellular alterations, including altered expression,
function and biochemistry of tau. The finding that specific tau gene mu-
tations lead to diverse FTDP-17 phenotypes raises the possibility that the
clinical and pathological expression of hereditary and related sporadic
tauopathies may be influenced by tau gene polymorphisms, other genetic
factors and epigenetic events. However, the precise mechanisms whereby tau
assembles into filaments and causes neurodegeneration in the human brain
remain to be elucidated, but further investigation into the mechanisms of
tau dysfunction, as well as the identification of potential disease-modifying
J.Q. Trojanowski, V.M.-Y. Lee / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 615–627 621
factors, will provide additional insight into novel strategies for the treatment
and prevention of AD and related disorders. Moreover, development of
additional animal models of tauopathies that more closely recapitulate
human diseases will facilitate this undertaking, and this is likely to have
implications for other neurodegenerative disorders since the aggregation
of tau in AD and and related tauopathies is an example of abnormal pro-
tein–protein interactions resulting in the intracellular accumulation of fila-
mentous proteins that is a common feature of many fatal CNS diseases
characterized by relentlessly progressive brain degeneration [1–3]. Thus, the
fibrillization and aggregation of proteins in the brain is a common theme in
a diverse group of neurodegenerative disorders and insight into the patho-
genesis of any one of these disorders may have implications for understand-
ing the mechanisms that underlie all these diseases as well as for the
discovery of better strategies to treat them [1–3].
Acknowledgements
V.M.-Y.L. is the John H. Ware 3rd Chair of Alzheimer’s disease research
at the University of Pennsylvania. Work done in the laboratories of the
authors is supported by grants from the National Institute of Aging of the
National Institutes of Health and the Alzheimer’s Association. We also
thank our many collaborators and past as well as current members of the
Center for Neurodegenerative Disease Research (CNDR) for contributions
to the research from CNDR summarized here. Finally, the support of the
families of our patients have made this research possible.
References
[1] Buée L, Bussière T, Buée-Scherrer V, et al. Tau protein isoforms, phosphorylation and
role in neurodegenerative disorders. Brain Res Rev 2000;33:95–130.
[2] Forman MS, Lee VM-Y, Trojanowski JQ. New insights into genetic and molecular
mechanisms of brain degeneration in tauopathies. J Chem Neuroanat 2000;20:225–44.
[3] Lee VM-Y, Goedert M, Trojanowski JQ. Neurodegenerative tauopathies. Annu Rev
Neurosci 2001;24:1121–59.
[4] Clark LN, Poorkaj P, Wszolek Z, et al. Pathogenic implications of mutations in the tau
gene in pallido-ponto-nigral degeneration and related neurodegenerative disorders linked
to chromosome 17. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1998;95(22):13103–7.
[5] Dumanchin C, Camuzat A, Campion D, et al. Segregation of a missense mutation in the
microtubule-associated protein tau gene with familial frontotemporal dementia and
parkinsonism. Hum Mol Genet 1998;7(11):1825–9.
[6] Hutton M, Lendon CL, Rizzu P, et al. Association of missense and 5¢-splice-site mutations
in tau with the inherited dementia FTDP-17. Nature 1998;393(6686):702–5.
[7] Poorkaj P, Bird TD, Wijsman E, et al. Tau is a candidate gene for chromosome 17
frontotemporal dementia. Ann Neurol 1998;43(6):815–25.
[8] Spillantini MG, Murrell JR, Goedert M, et al. Mutation in the tau gene in familial mul-
tiple system tauopathy with presenile dementia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1998;95(13):
7737–41.
622 J.Q. Trojanowski, V.M.-Y. Lee / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 615–627
[9] Binder LI, Frankfurter A, Rebhun LI. The distribution of tau in the mammalian central
nervous system. J Cell Biol 1985;101(4):1371–8.
[10] Cleveland DW, Hwo SY, Kirschner MW. Physical and chemical properties of purified tau
factor and the role of tau in microtubule assembly. J Mol Biol 1977;116(2):227–47.
[11] Cleveland DW, Hwo SY, Kirschner MW. Purification of tau, a microtubule-associated
protein that induces assembly of microtubules from purified tubulin. J Mol Biol
1977;116(2):207–25.
[12] Couchie D, Mavilia C, Georgieff IS, et al. Primary structure of high molecular weight
tau present in the peripheral nervous system. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1992;89(10):
4378–81.
[13] LoPresti P, Szuchet S, Papasozomenos SC, et al. Functional implications for the
microtubule-associated protein tau: localization in oligodendrocytes. Proc Natl Acad Sci
USA 1995;92(22):10369–73.
[14] Trojanowski JQ, Schuck T, Schmidt ML, et al. The distribution of tau in the normal
human nervous system. J Histochem Cytochem 1989;37:209–15.
[15] Andreadis A, Brown WM, Kosik KS. Structure and novel exons of the human tau gene.
Biochem 1992;31(43):10626–33.
[16] Goedert M, Wischik CM, Crowther RA, et al. Cloning and sequencing of the cDNA
encoding a core protein of the paired helical filament of Alzheimer disease: identification
as the microtubule-associated protein tau. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1988;85(11):
4051–5.
[17] Neve RL, Harris P, Kosik KS, et al. Identification of cDNA clones for the human
microtubule-associated protein tau and chromosomal localization of the genes for tau and
microtubule-associated protein 2. Brain Res 1986;387(3):271–80.
[18] Goedert M, Spillantini MG, Jakes R, et al. Multiple isoforms of human microtubule-
associated protein tau: sequences and localization in neurofibrillary tangles of Alzheimer’s
disease. Neuron 1989;3(4):519–26.
[19] Goedert M, Spillantini MG, Potier MC, et al. Cloning and sequencing of the cDNA
encoding an isoform of microtubule-associated protein tau containing four tandem
repeats: differential expression of tau protein mRNAs in human brain. EMBO J 1989;8(2):
393–9.
[20] Goedert M, Jakes R. Expression of separate isoforms of human tau protein: correlation
with the tau pattern in brain and effects on tubulin polymerization. EMBO J 1990;9(13):
4225–30.
[21] Hong M, Zhukareva V, Vogelsberg-Ragaglia V, et al. Mutation-specific functional im-
pairments in distinct tau isoforms of hereditary FTDP-17. Science 1998;282(5395):1914–7.
[22] Georgieff IS, Liem RK, Mellado W, et al. High molecular weight tau: preferential
localization in the peripheral nervous system. J Cell Sci 1991;100:55–60.
[23] Goedert M, Spillantini MG, Crowther RA. Cloning of a big tau microtubule-associated
protein characteristic of the peripheral nervous system. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1992;
89(5):1983–7.
[24] Butner KA, Kirschner MW. Tau protein binds to microtubules through a flexible array of
distributed weak sites. J Cell Biol 1991;115(3):717–30.
[25] Himmler A, Drechsel D, Kirschner MW, et al. Tau consists of a set of proteins with
repeated C-terminal microtubule-binding domains and variable N-terminal domains. Mol
Cell Biol 1989;9(4):1381–8.
[26] Lee G, Neve RL, Kosik KS. The microtubule binding domain of tau protein. Neuron
1989;2(6):1615–24.
[27] Weingarten MD, Lockwood AH, Hwo SY, et al. A protein factor essential for
microtubule assembly. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1975;72(5):1858–62.
[28] Goode BL, Denis PE, Panda D, et al. Functional interactions between the proline-rich and
repeat regions of tau enhance microtubule binding and assembly. Mol Biol Cell 1997;8(2):
353–65.
J.Q. Trojanowski, V.M.-Y. Lee / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 615–627 623
[29] Goode BL, Feinstein SC. Identification of a novel microtubule binding and assembly
domain in the developmentally regulated inter-repeat region of tau. J Cell Biol 1994;124(5):
769–82.
[30] Gustke N, Trinczek B, Biernat J, et al. Domains of tau protein and interactions with
microtubules. Biochem 1994;33(32):9511–22.
[31] Bramblett GT, Goedert M, Jakes R, et al. Abnormal tau phosphorylation at Ser396 in
Alzheimer’s disease recapitulates development and contributes to reduced microtubule
binding. Neuron 1993;10(6):1089–99.
[32] Goedert M, Jakes R, Crowther RA, et al. The abnormal phosphorylation of tau protein at
Ser-202 in Alzheimer disease recapitulates phosphorylation during development. Proc
Natl Acad Sci USA 1993;90(11):5066–70.
[33] Kanemaru K, Takio K, Miura R, et al. Fetal-type phosphorylation of the tau in paired
helical filaments. J Neurochem 1992;58(5):1667–75.
[34] Mawal-Dewan M, Henley J, Van dV. The phosphorylation state of tau in the developing
rat brain is regulated by phosphoprotein phosphatases. J Biol Chem 1994;269(49):
30981–7.
[35] Watanabe A, Hasegawa M, Suzuki M, et al. In vivo phosphorylation sites in fetal and
adult rat tau. J Biol Chem 1993;268(34):25712–7.
[36] Drechsel DN, Hyman AA, Cobb MH, et al. Modulation of the dynamic instability of
tubulin assembly by the microtubule-associated protein tau. Mol Biol Cell 1992;3(10):
1141–54.
[37] Yoshida H, Ihara Y. Tau in paired helical filaments is functionally distinct from fetal tau:
assembly incompetence of paired helical filament-tau. J Neurochem 1993;61(3):1183–6.
[38] Biernat J, Gustke N. Drewes, et al. Phosphorylation of Ser262 strongly reduces binding of
tau to microtubules: distinction between PHF-like immunoreactivity and microtubule
binding. Neuron 1993;11(1):153–63.
[39] Seubert P, Mawal-Dewan M, Barbour R, et al. Detection of phosphorylated Ser262 in
fetal tau, adult tau, and paired helical filament tau. J Biol Chem 1995;270(32):18917–22.
[40] Drewes G, Lichtenberg-Kraag B, Doring F, et al. Mitogen activated protein (MAP)
kinase transforms tau protein into an Alzheimer-like state. EMBO J 1992;11(6):2131–8.
[41] Goedert M, Cohen ES, Jakes R, et al. p42 MAP kinase phosphorylation sites in
microtubule-associated protein tau are dephosphorylated by protein phosphatase 2A1.
Implications for Alzheimer’s disease. FEBS Lett 1992;312(1):95–9.
[42] Hanger DP, Hughes K, Woodgett JR, et al. Glycogen synthase kinase-3 induces
Alzheimer’s disease-like phosphorylation of tau: generation of paired helical filament
epitopes and neuronal localisation of the kinase. Neurosci Lett 1992;147(1):58–62.
[43] Hong M, Chen DC, Klein PS, et al. Lithium reduces tau phosphorylation by inhibition of
glycogen synthase kinase-3. J Biol Chem 1997;272(40):25326–32.
[44] Hong M, Lee VM-Y. Insulin and insulin-like growth factor-1 regulate tau phosphoryla-
tion in cultured human neurons. J Biol Chem 1997;272(31):19547–53.
[45] Mandelkow EM, Drewes G, Biernat J, et al. Glycogen synthase kinase-3 and the Alzheimer-
like state of microtubule-associated protein tau. FEBS Lett 1992;314(3):315–21.
[46] Baumann K, Mandelkow EM, Biernat J, et al. Abnormal Alzheimer-like phosphorylation
of tau-protein by cyclin-dependent kinases cdk2 and cdk5. FEBS Lett 1993;336(3):
417–24.
[47] Kobayashi S, Ishiguro K, Omori A, et al. A cdc2-related kinase PSSALRE/cdk5 is
homologous with the 30 kDa subunit of tau protein kinase II, a proline-directed protein
kinase associated with microtubule. FEBS Lett 1993;335(2):171–5.
[48] Litersky JM, Johnson GV. Phosphorylation by cAMP-dependent protein kinase inhibits
the degradation of tau by calpain. J Biol Chem 1992;267(3):1563–8.
[49] Baudier J, Cole RD. Phosphorylation of tau proteins to a state like that in Alzheimer’s
brain is catalyzed by a calcium/calmodulin-dependent kinase and modulated by phos-
pholipids. J Biol Chem 1987;262(36):17577–83.
624 J.Q. Trojanowski, V.M.-Y. Lee / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 615–627
[50] Drewes G, Ebneth A, Preuss U, et al. MARK, a novel family of protein kinases that
phosphorylate microtubule-associated proteins and trigger microtubule disruption. Cell
1997;89(2):297–308.
[51] Goedert M, Hasegawa M, Jakes R, et al. Phosphorylation of microtubule-associated
protein tau by stress-activated protein kinases. FEBS Lett 1997;409(1):57–62.
[52] Reynolds CH, Nebreda AR, Gibb GM, et al. Reactivating kinase/p38 phosphorylates tau
protein in vitro. J Neurochem 1997;69(1):191–8.
[53] Reynolds CH, Utton MA, Gibb GM, et al. Stress-activated protein kinase/c-jun
N-terminal kinase phosphorylates tau protein. J Neurochem 1997;68(4):1736–44.
[54] Ishiguro K, Kobayashi S, Omori A, et al. Identification of the 23 kDa subunit of tau
protein kinase II as a putative activator of cdk5 in bovine brain. FEBS Lett 1994;342(2):
203–8.
[55] Lovestone S, Hartley CL, Pearce J, et al. Phosphorylation of tau by glycogen synthase
kinase-3 beta in intact mammalian cells: the effects on the organization and stability of
microtubules. Neurosci 1996;73(4):1145–57.
[56] Singh TJ, Zaidi T, Grundke-Iqbal I, et al. Modulation of GSK-3-catalyzed phosphory-
lation of microtubule-associated protein tau by non-proline-dependent protein kinases.
FEBS Lett 1995;358(1):4–8.
[57] Takahashi M, Tomizawa K, Ishiguro K, et al. Involvement of tau protein kinase I in
paired helical filament-like phosphorylation of the juvenile tau in rat brain. J Neurochem
1995;64(4):1759–68.
[58] Leost M, Schultz C, Link A, et al. Paullones are potent inhibitors of glycogen
synthase kinase-3beta and cyclin-dependent kinase 5/p25. Eur J Biochem 2000;267(19):
5983–94.
[59] Lovestone S, Davis DR, Webster MT, et al. Lithium reduces tau phosphorylation: effects
in living cells and in neurons at therapeutic concentrations. Biol Psychiat 1999;45(8):
995–1003.
[60] Munoz-Montano JR, Moreno FJ, Avila J, et al. Lithium inhibits Alzheimer’s disease-like
tau protein phosphorylation in neurons. FEBS Lett 1997;411(2–3):183–8.
[61] Takahashi M, Yasutake K, Tomizawa K. Lithium inhibits neurite growth and tau protein
kinase I/glycogen synthase kinase-3beta-dependent phosphorylation of juvenile tau in
cultured hippocampal neurons. J Neurochem 1999;73(5):2073–83.
[62] Lew J, Huang QQ, Qi Z, et al. A brain-specific activator of cyclin-dependent kinase 5.
Nature 1994;371(6496):423–6.
[63] Tsai LH, Delalle I, Caviness VS Jr, et al. p35 is a neural-specific regulatory subunit of
cyclin-dependent kinase 5. Nature 1994;371(6496):419–23.
[64] Sengupta A, Wu Q, Grundke-Iqbal I, et al. Potentiation of GSK-3-catalyzed Alzheimer-
like phosphorylation of human tau by cdk5. Mol Cell Biochem 1997;167(1–2):99–105.
[65] Sobue K, Agarwal-Mawal A, Wei L, et al. Interaction of neuronal cdc2 like protein kinase
with microtubule associated protein tau. J Biol Chem 2000;275(22):16673–80.
[66] Yamaguchi H, Ishiguro K, Uchida T, et al. Preferential labeling of Alzheimer neuro-
fibrillary tangles with antisera for tau protein kinase (TPK) I/glycogen synthase kinase-3
beta and cyclin-dependent kinase 5, a component of TPK II. Acta Neuropathol (Berl) 1996;
92(3):232–41.
[67] Drewes G, Mandelkow EM, Baumann K, et al. Dephosphorylation of tau protein and
Alzheimer paired helical filaments by calcineurin and phosphatase-2A. FEBS Lett
1993;336(3):425–32.
[68] Goedert M, Jakes R, Qi Z, et al. Protein phosphatase 2A is the major enzyme in brain that
dephosphorylates tau protein phosphorylated by proline-directed protein kinases or cyclic
AMP-dependent protein kinase. J Neurochem 1995;65(6):2804–7.
[69] Matsuo ES, Shin RW, Billingsley ML, et al. Biopsy-derived adult human brain tau is
phosphorylated at many of the same sites as Alzheimer’s disease paired helical filament
tau. Neuron 1994;13(4):989–1002.
J.Q. Trojanowski, V.M.-Y. Lee / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 615–627 625
[70] Liao H, Li Y, Brautigan DL, Gundersen GG. Protein phosphatase 1 is targeted to micro-
tubules by the microtubule-associated protein tau. J Biol Chem 1998;273(34):21901–8.
[71] Sontag E, Nunbhakdi-Craig V, Bloom GS, et al. A novel pool of protein phosphatase 2A
is associated with microtubules and is regulated during the cell cycle. J Cell Biol
1995;128(6):1131–44.
[72] Sontag E, Nunbhakdi-Craig V, Lee G, et al. Molecular interactions among protein
phosphatase 2A, tau, and microtubules. Implications for the regulation of tau phos-
phorylation and the development of tauopathies. J Biol Chem 1999;274(36):25490–8.
[73] Merrick SE, Trojanowski JQ, Lee VM-Y. Selective destruction of stable microtubules and
axons by inhibitors of protein serine/threonine phosphatases in cultured human neurons.
J Neurosci 1997;17(15):5726–37.
[74] Brion JP, Passareiro H, Nunez J, et al. Mise en evidence immunologique de la proteine tau
au niveau des lesions de degenerescence neurofibrillaire de la maladie d’Alzheimer. Arch
Biol 1985;95:229–35.
[75] Grundke-Iqbal I, Iqbal K, Tung YC, et al. Abnormal phosphorylation of the microtubule-
associated protein tau (tau) in Alzheimer cytoskeletal pathology. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
1986;83(13):4913–7.
[76] Kidd M. Paired helical filaments in electron microscopy of Alzheimer’s disease. Nature
1963;197:192–4.
[77] Crowther RA. Straight and paired helical filaments in Alzheimer disease have a common
structural unit. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1991;88(6):2288–92.
[78] Greenberg SG, Davies P. A preparation of Alzheimer paired helical filaments that displays
distinct tau proteins by polyacrylamide gel electrophoresis. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
1990;87(15):5827–31.
[79] Greenberg SG, Davies P, Schein JD, et al. Hydrofluoric acid-treated tau PHF proteins
display the same biochemical properties as normal tau. J Biol Chem 1992;267(1):564–9.
[80] Kondo J, Honda T, Mori H, et al. The carboxyl third of tau is tightly bound to paired
helical filaments. Neuron 1988;1(9):827–34.
[81] Kosik KS, Orecchio LD, Binder L, et al. Epitopes that span the tau molecule are shared
with paired helical filaments. Neuron 1988;1(9):817–25.
[82] Lee VM-Y, Balin BJ, Otvos L Jr, et al. A68: a major subunit of paired helical filaments
and derivatized forms of normal tau. Science 1991;251(4994):675–8.
[83] Wischik CM, Novak M, Thogersen HC, et al. Isolation of a fragment of tau derived from
the core of the paired helical filament of Alzheimer disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
1988;85(12):4506–10.
[84] Goedert M, Spillantini MG, Jakes R, et al. Molecular dissection of the paired helical
filament. Neurobiol Aging 1995;16(3):325–34.
[85] Morishima-Kawashima M, Hasegawa M, Takio K, et al. Hyperphosphorylation of tau in
PHF. Neurobiol Aging 1995;16(3):365–71.
[86] Hasegawa M, Jakes R, Crowther RA, et al. Characterization of mAb AP422, a novel
phosphorylation-dependent monoclonal antibody against tau protein. FEBS Lett 1996;
384(1):25–30.
[87] Hoffmann R, Lee VM-Y, Leight S, et al. Unique Alzheimer’s disease paired helical
filament specific epitopes involve double phosphorylation at specific sites. Biochem 1997;
36(26):8114–24.
[88] Zheng-Fischhofer Q, Biernat J, Mandelkow EM, et al. Sequential phosphorylation of Tau
by glycogen synthase kinase-3beta and protein kinase A at Thr212 and Ser214 generates
the Alzheimer-specific epitope of antibody AT100 and requires a paired-helical-filament-
like conformation. Eur J Biochem 1998;252(3):542–52.
[89] Kusakawa G, Saito T, Onuki R, et al. Calpain-dependent proteolytic cleavage of the p35
cyclin-dependent kinase 5 activator to p25. J Biol Chem 2000;275(22):17166–72.
[90] Lee MS, Kwon YT, Li M, et al. Neurotoxicity induces cleavage of p35 to p25 by calpain.
Nature 2000;405(6784):360–4.
626 J.Q. Trojanowski, V.M.-Y. Lee / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 615–627
[91] Nath R, Davis M, Probert AW, et al. Processing of cdk5 activator p35 to its truncated
form (p25) by calpain in acutely injured neuronal cells. Biochem Biophys Res Commun
2000;274(1):16–21.
[92] Patrick GN, Zukerberg L, Nikolic M, et al. Conversion of p35 to p25 deregulates Cdk5
activity and promotes neurodegeneration. Nature 1999;402(6762):615–22.
[93] Lu PJ, Wulf G, Zhou XZ, et al. The prolyl isomerase Pin1 restores the function of
Alzheimer-associated phosphorylated tau protein. Nature 1999;399(6738):784–8.
[94] Schneider A, Biernat J, von Bergen M, et al. Phosphorylation that detaches tau protein
from microtubules (Ser262, Ser214) also protects it against aggregation into Alzheimer
paired helical filaments. Biochem 1999;38(12):3549–58.
[95] Schweers O, Schonbrunn-Hanebeck E, Marx A, et al. Structural studies of tau protein and
Alzheimer paired helical filaments show no evidence for beta-structure. J Biol Chem
1994;269(39):24290–7.
[96] Tseng HC, Graves DJ. Natural methylamine osmolytes, trimethylamine N-oxide and
betaine, increase tau-induced polymerization of microtubules. Biochem Biophys Res
Commun 1998;250(3):726–30.
[97] Tseng HC, Lu Q, Henderson E, et al. Phosphorylated tau can promote tubulin assembly.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1999;96(17):9503–8.
[98] Braak E, Braak H, Mandelkow EM. A sequence of cytoskeleton changes related to the
formation of neurofibrillary tangles and neuropil threads. Acta Neuropathol (Berl) 1994;
87(6):554–67.
[99] Crowther RA, Olesen OF, Jakes R, et al. The microtubule binding repeats of tau protein
assemble into filaments like those found in Alzheimer’s disease. FEBS Lett 1992;
309(2):199–202.
[100] Goedert M, Spillantini MG, Cairns NJ, et al. Tau proteins of Alzheimer paired
helical filaments: abnormal phosphorylation of all six brain isoforms. Neuron 1992;8(1):
159–68.
[101] Wille H, Drewes G, Biernat J, et al. Alzheimer-like paired helical filaments and anti-
parallel dimers formed from microtubule-associated protein tau in vitro. J Cell Biol
1992;118(3):573–84.
[102] Arrasate M, Perez M, Valpuesta JM, et al. Role of glycosaminoglycans in determining the
helicity of paired helical filaments. Am J Pathol 1997;151(4):1115–22.
[103] Goedert M, Jakes R, Spillantini MG, et al. Assembly of microtubule-associated protein
tau into Alzheimer-like filaments induced by sulphated glycosaminoglycans. Nature
1996;383(6600):550–3.
[104] Perez M, Valpuesta JM, Medina M, et al. Polymerization of tau into filaments in the
presence of heparin: the minimal sequence required for tau-tau interaction. J Neurochem
1996;67(3):1183–90.
[105] Von Bergen M, Friedhoff P, Biernat J, et al. Assembly of tau protein into Alzheimer
paired helical filaments depends on a local sequence motif ((306)VQIVYK(311)) forming
beta structure. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2000;97(10):5129–34.
[106] Friedhoff P, von Bergen M, Mandelkow EM, et al. A nucleated assembly mechanism of
Alzheimer paired helical filaments. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1998;95(26):15712–7.
[107] Hasegawa M, Crowther RA, Jakes R, et al. Alzheimer-like changes in microtubule-
associated protein Tau induced by sulfated glycosaminoglycans. Inhibition of microtubule
binding, stimulation of phosphorylation, and filament assembly depend on the degree of
sulfation. J Biol Chem 1997;272(52):33118–24.
[108] Kampers T, Friedhoff P, Biernat J, et al. RNA stimulates aggregation of microtubule-
associated protein tau into Alzheimer-like paired helical filaments. FEBS Lett 1996;
309(3):344–9.
[109] Wilson DM, Binder LI. Free fatty acids stimulate the polymerization of tau and amyloid
beta peptides. In vitro evidence for a common effector of pathogenesis in Alzheimer’s
disease. Am J Pathol 1997;150(6):2181–95.
J.Q. Trojanowski, V.M.-Y. Lee / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 615–627 627
[110] Ginsberg SD, Galvin JE, Chiu TS, et al. RNA sequestration to pathological lesions of
neurodegenerative diseases. Acta Neuropathol (Berl) 1998;96(5):487–94.
[111] Snow AD, Mar H, Nochlin D, et al. Early accumulation of heparan sulfate in neurons and
in the beta- amyloid protein-containing lesions of Alzheimer’s disease and Down’s
syndrome. Am J Pathol 1990;137(5):1253–70.
[112] Verbeek MM, Otte-Holler I, van den Heuvel LP, et al. Agrin is a major heparan sulfate
proteoglycan accumulating in Alzheimer’s disease brain. Am J Pathol 1999;155(6):2115–25.
[113] Gamblin TC, King ME, Dawson H, et al. In vitro polymerization of tau protein
monitored by laser light scattering: method and application to the study of FTDP-17
mutants. Biochem 2000;39(20):6136–44.
Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 629–639
0025-7125/02/$ - see front matter 2002, Elsevier Science (USA). All rights reserved.
PII: S 0 0 2 5 - 7 1 2 5 ( 0 2 ) 0 0 0 2 2 - 6
630 S. Sinha / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 629–639
from the time that the protein precursors to the Ab peptides, collectively
called APP, were cloned [3] and subsequently expressed in cell culture.
Studies of the metabolism of the APP from various laboratories have
conclusively established that following maturation of the protein in the
secretory pathway, proteolytic cleavages take place in the extracellular or
luminal domain of the protein, which leads to the release (secretion) of the
bulk of the N-terminal ectodomain of the protein as sAPP [4], which is
detected not only in the culture medium of cells expressing APP, but also
in physiologic fluids such as plasma and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF). It turns
out that most of the sAPP generation takes place as a consequence of cleav-
age of the membrane-bound protein approximately 12 amino acids N-termi-
nal to the membrane interface [5]. This ‘‘a-secretase’’ cleavage also leads to
the loss of the intact Ab peptide region, the site of endoproteolysis being 16
aa C-terminal to the start of the Ab peptide sequence. The early identifica-
tion of this pathway, and its recognition as the major, ubiquitous pathway
of mature APP processing, led to the dogma that this represents ‘‘normal’’
APP metabolism, and that alternate, pathologically triggered pathways
must be responsible for the aberrant generation of the Ab peptides, which
are then subsequently deposited as insoluble amyloid during the disease
process.
The recognition that the Ab peptides are actually normal metabolites of
APP came about as the consequence of the discovery that primary human
neuronal cells release Ab peptides, Ab1-40, as well as Ab11-40, in their cul-
ture medium [6], and that tissue culture cell lines, such as HEK293 that are
transfected with APP also release Ab peptides into their culture medium as
well. Shortly thereafter, it was shown that a subset of sAPP is generated by
an alternate ‘‘b-secretase’’ activity that cleaves the full-length membrane-
bound APP immediately N-terminal to the Ab peptide sequence. This
cleavage takes place between Met671-Asp672 (using the APP770 codon
numbering), releasing a b-sAPP [7] that is C-terminally truncated compared
with a-sAPP, and also generating a cell-associated 99 aa long C-terminal
fragment (CTF) which starts with Asp672 (¼Ab1) (Fig. 1).
This alternate secretory cleavage was found to be far more prevalent in
primary neuronal cultures than in peripherally derived tissue culture cells;
the b-sAPP made up as much as 40–50% of total sAPP derived from culture
medium of the neuronal cells, compared with 5% or less as found in culture
medium from HEK293 cells transfected with APP. It therefore appeared
that b-secretase cleavage of APP, enriched in neuronal cells, could initiate
the normal production of Ab by the cellular turnover of the 99 aa b-CTF.
to the familial inheritance of AD. These classified into two groups, one
in which Val717 was found to be mutated into either Ile [8], Phe or Gly
(‘‘London’’ mutations, APP717), and a second pedigree [9] with a double
mutation, changing the Lys670Met671 sequence of the b-secretase cleavage
site to a Asn670Leu671 motif (‘‘Swedish’’ mutation, APPNL). The identifi-
cation of such disease-causing mutations that framed the Ab peptide region
suggested that they somehow alter the metabolism of APP, leading to
increased Ab production.
This was demonstrated dramatically on transfection of the Swedish APP
forms into HEK293 cells, which led to a five to sixfold overproduction of
the Ab peptides compared with Wt APP [10]. The increased Ab peptide
release was accompanied by increased levels of b-sAPP as well, indicating
that the Swedish double mutation at the b-secretase site leads to an
increased cleavage of the full-length APP molecule at the altered sequence.
The higher levels of the cell-associated b-CTF generated as a consequence of
this gives rise to the higher levels of Ab peptide released.
Although missense mutations in APP account for a very small number of
total AD cases, epidemiologic data have long pointed to the strong contri-
bution of genetic factors to the overall population risk for AD. For example,
there is an increased age-dependent risk for AD among primary relatives of
patients with AD [11], consistent with either incompletely penetrant single-
gene defects, or with a more complex interaction among multiple hereditary
and extrahereditary factors, such as possible environmental modifiers.
Approximately 10% of AD cases, however, appear to be transmitted in
632 S. Sinha / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 629–639
that position. The observation that a single point mutation in APP can lead
to AD provided the first direct genetic evidence for Ab as a causative factor
in AD.
The pathologic observation of the preponderance of Ab42 plaques, in
both sporadic as well as in this first FAD disease, prompted the analysis
of the mechanism by which APP717 leads to disease. Analysis of Ab species
derived on transfection of the mutated APP717 form into 293 cells showed
that a small yet significant change in the composition of the Ab species
secreted into the conditioned medium. Compared with cells transfected with
Wt APP, an increased level of Ab42 peptides were secreted [20], relative to
the Ab40 species, resulting in an 50% increase in the Ab42/Ab40 ratio.
Analysis of plasma Ab from individuals with codon 717 mutations also
showed an approximately twofold increase in Ab42 levels. The in vivo and
in vitro data provided very strong evidence for the hypothesis that increased
levels of Ab42 are produced from the mutated APP, and that this was suffi-
cient to cause early onset AD.
The second FAD mutation identified was also in APP, the ‘‘Swedish’’ or
APPNL, although the mechanism via which it causes AD was established
chronologically earlier than that with the APP717 mutations. Transfection
of APPNL into 293 cells led to a very large increase in the total amount
of Ab secreted into the conditioned medium, accompanied by a large
increase in the b-secretase-cleaved secreted APP, b-sAPP [10]. A twofold
increase in total Ab was found in the plasma of individuals carrying the
APPNL mutation, confirming the cellular results [21]. Thus, increased levels
of total Ab (both Ab40 and Ab42) also cause early onset AD, providing a
very strong basis for the hypothesis that Ab is causative for AD.
Presenilins and AD
Although the APP mutations provided strong evidence for the amyloid-
centric view of AD, such mutations were very infrequent, and the locus on
Chromosome 14 (APP is on Chromosome 21) seemed to account for the
vast majority of FAD cases around the world. The identification of this
gene, PS-1, opened up a new field of investigation for AD researchers.
PS-1 is widely expressed in various tissues and cells and exists in cells as
a noncovalent yet stable complex [22], of a 30 kDa N-terminal fragment
(NTF) and a 15 kda CTF. The NTF and CTF are derived by intracellular
proteolytic cleavage of the intact PS-1 holoprotein [23], and the consequent
stable complex appears to be the functionally relevant form of PS-1. PS-2 is
processed in a similar manner as well. Most cells appear to have a saturable
capacity for forming the stable NTF:CTF complex [24], and transfection of
PS-1 in cell culture usually leads to only a relatively small (about twofold)
increase in the levels of the cleaved forms detected; the PS-1 holoprotein,
under these conditions, appears to be rapidly degraded, and not to be incor-
porated in a functionally relevant complex.
S. Sinha / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 629–639 635
compact amyloid plaques in the cortex and hippocampus, but not in other
regions spared in AD such as the cerebellum. The pathologic changes
observed in these transgenic mice lines also include microglial activation and
astrogliosis, as well as neuritic changes in the vicinity of plaques that are
remarkably similar to that observed in sporadic AD. Although it has been
difficult to measure frank cell loss in these transgenic lines, other neurofibril-
lary changes highly reminiscent of AD have been shown to occur in the
PDAPP mouse with age [32].
In addition to demonstrating that the APP FAD mutations do result in
the production of amyloid plaques, and ancillary pathologies seen in AD,
these animal models have also proven to be useful in identifying pharma-
cologic agents that modulate the production or clearance of the Ab peptide.
Immunization of the PDAPP transgenic mice with human Ab42 was shown
to result in the clearance of existing plaques by microglial cells by an anti-
body mediated mechanism [33]. Peripheral administration of antibodies
directed to the human Ab was also shown to result in a similar clearance
[34], along with a reduction of the neuritic pathology and glial pathology.
In a separate approach, an inhibitor of c-secretase, identified using cell cul-
ture models, was shown to inhibit the production of Ab in young PDAPP
animals, in a dose- and time-dependent manner [35]. The ability to test
potential drugs and therapeutic approaches directed toward the lowering
of Ab peptide levels and the associated aggregated amyloid deposits has
opened up the way in utilizing these animal models of AD pathology toward
identifying and testing strategies for treating AD.
the ability to generate Ab, either from endogenous APP [39] or from concur-
rently expressed human transgenic APP forms [40]. Thus, cleavage of APP
by BACE is absolutely required for the generation of the Ab peptide, and
the lack of biologic redundancy in this process suggests that strategies
directed toward inhibiting the activity of this enzyme in vivo should lead
to salutary effects on the production, and subsequent aggregation, of Ab
peptides.
Summary
The current understanding of the role of amyloid in AD has been estab-
lished by a remarkable congruence of multiple disciplines: neuropathology,
biochemistry, molecular biology, and epidemiologic genetics. Although the
precise etiology of AD still remains poorly understood, the identification of
the key biochemical players in the Ab generation (APP, PS-1 and -2, and
BACE), and the development of transgenic models exhibiting progressive
disease pathology, provide a strong framework on which to build a better
understanding of the molecular events that lead to progressive neurodegen-
eration in AD.
References
[1] Dickson DW. The pathogenesis of senile plaques. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 1997;56:
321–39.
[2] Grundke-Iqbal I, Iqbal K, Tung YC, Quinlan M, Wisnieswki HM, Binder LI. Abnormal
phosphorylation of the microtubule-associated protein tau (tau) in Alzheimer cytoskeletal
pathology. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1986;83:4913–7.
[3] Kang J, Lemaire HG, Unterbeck A, Salbaum JM, Masters CL, Grzeschik KH, et al. The
precursor of Alzheimer’s disease amyloid A4 protein resembles a cell-surface receptor.
Nature 1987;325:733–6.
[4] Oltersdorf T, Ward PJ, Heinriksson T, Beattie EC, Neve R, Lieberburg I, et al. The
Alzheimer amyloid precursor protein: Identification of a stable intermediate in the
biosynthetic/degradative pathway. J Biol Chem 1990;265:4492–7.
[5] Esch FS, Keim PS, Beattie EC, Blacher RW, Culwell AR, Oltersdorf T, et al. Cleavage of
amyloid beta peptide during constitutive processing of its precursor. Science 1990;
248:1122–4.
[6] Seubert P, et al. Isolation and quantification of soluble Alzheimer’s beta-peptide from
biological fluids. Nature 1992;359:325–7.
[7] Seubert P, et al. Secretion of beta-amyloid precursor protein cleaved at the amino terminus
of the beta-amyloid peptide. Nature 1993;361:260–3.
[8] Goate A, et al. Early-onset Alzheimer’s disease caused by mutations at codon 717 of the
beta-amyloid precursor protein gene. Nature 1991;349:704–6.
[9] Mullan M, Crawford F, Axelman K, Houlden H, Lilius L, Winblad B, et al. A pathogenic
mutation for probable Alzheimer’s disease in the APP gene at the N-terminus of beta-
amyloid. Nat Genet 1992;1:345–7.
[10] Citron M, Oltersdorf T, Haass C, McConlogue L, Hung AY, Seubert P, et al. Mutation of
the beta-amyloid precursor protein in familial Alzheimer’s disease increases beta-protein
production. Nature 1992;360:672–4.
638 S. Sinha / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 629–639
[11] Lautenschlager NT, et al. Risk of dementia among relatives of Alzheimer’s disease patients
in the MIRAGE study: What is in store for the oldest old? Neurology 1996;46:641–50.
[12] Sherrington R, et al. Cloning of a gene bearing missense mutations in early-onset familial
Alzheimer’s disease. Nature 1995;375:754–60.
[13] Levy-Lehad, et al. A familial Alzheimer’s disease locus on chromosome 1. Science 1995;
269:973–7.
[14] Strittmatter WJ, Saunders AM, Schmechel D, Pericak-Vance M, Enghild J, Salvesen GS,
et al. Apolipoprotein E: high-avidity binding to beta-amyloid and increased frequency of type
4 allele in late-onset familial Alzheimer disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1993;90:1977–81.
[15] Saunders AM, et al. Association of apolipoprotein E allele epsilon 4 with late-onset familial
and sporadic Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 1993;43:1467–72.
[16] Iwatsubo T, et al. Visualization of A beta 42(43) and A beta 40 in senile plaques with end-
specific A beta monoclonals: evidence that an initially deposited species is A beta 42(43).
Neuron 1994;13:45–52.
[17] Lemere CA, et al. The E280A presenilin 1 Alzheimer mutation produces increased A beta
42 deposition and severe cerebellar pathology. Nat Med 1996;2:1146–50.
[18] Tokuda T, Fukushima T, Ikeda S, Sekijima Y, Shoji S, Yanagisawa N, et al. Plasma levels
of amyloid beta proteins Abeta1-40 and Abeta1-42(43) are elevated in Down’s syndrome.
Ann Neurol 1997;41:271–3.
[19] Lemere CA, Blustzian JK, Yamaguchi H, Wisniewski T, Saido TC, Selkoe DJ. Sequence of
deposition of heterogeneous amyloid beta-peptides and APO E in Down syndrome:
implications for initial events in amyloid plaque formation. Neurobiol Dis 1996;3:16–32.
[20] Suzuki N, Cheung TT, Cai X-D, Odaka A, Otvos C, Eckman L, Golde TE, Younkin SG.
An increased percentage of long amyloid beta protein secreted by familial amyloid beta
protein precursor (beta APP717) mutants. Science 1994;264:1336–40.
[21] Scheuner D, et al. Secreted amyloid beta-protein similar to that in the senile plaques of
Alzheimer’s disease is increased in vivo by the presenilin 1 and 2 and APP mutations linked
to familial Alzheimer’s disease. Nat Med 1996;2:864–70.
[22] Thinakaran G, et al. Endoproteolysis of presenilin 1 and accumulation of processed
derivatives in vivo. Neuron 1996;17:181–90.
[23] Podlisny MB, et al. Presenilin proteins undergo heterogeneous endoproteolysis between
Thr291 and Ala299 and occur as stable N- and C-terminal fragments in normal and
Alzheimer brain tissue. Neurobiol Dis 1997;325–37.
[24] Thinakaran G, et al. Evidence that levels of presenilins (PS1 and PS2) are coordinately
regulated by competition for limiting cellular factors. J Biol Chm 1997;272:28415–22.
[25] Borchelt DR, et al. Familial Alzheimer’s disease-linked presenilin 1 variants elevate
Abeta1-42/1-40 ratio in vitro and in vivo. Neuron 1996;17:1005–13.
[26] Strooper D, et al. Deficiency of presenilin-1 inhibits the normal cleavage of amyloid
precursor protein. Nature 1998;391:387–90.
[27] Wolfe MS, Xia W, Ostaszewski BL, Diehl TS, Kimberly WT, Selkoe DJ. Two
transmembrane aspartates in presenilin-1 required for presenilin endoproteolysis and
gamma-secretase activity. Nature 1999;398:513–7.
[28] Li YM, et al. Photoactivated gamma-secretase inhibitors directed to the active site
covalently label presenilin 1. Nature 2000;405:689–94.
[29] Yu G, et al. Nicastrin modulates presenilin-mediated notch/glp-1 signal transduction and
betaAPP processing. Nature 2000;407:48–54.
[30] Hsiao K, et al. Age-related CNS disorder and early death in transgenic FVB/N mice
overexpressing Alzheimer amyloid precursor proteins. Neuron 1995;15:1203–18.
[31] Games D, et al. Alzheimer-type neuropathology in transgenic mice overexpressing V717F
beta-amyloid precursor protein. Nature 1995;373:523–7.
[32] Masliah E, Sisk A, Mallory M, Games D. Comparison of neurodegenerative pathology in
transgenic mice overexpressing V717F beta-amyloid precursor protein and Alzheimer’s
disease. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 2001;60:357–68.
S. Sinha / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 629–639 639
Nonpharmacologic treatment
of behavioral disturbance in dementia
Linda Teri, PhD*, Rebecca G. Logsdon, PhD,
Susan M. McCurry, PhD
Department of Psychosocial and Community Health, University of Washington,
9709 3rd Avenue NE, Suite 507, Seattle, WA 98115, USA
* Corresponding author.
E-mail address: [email protected] (L. Teri).
Preparation of this manuscript was supported in part by grants from the Alzheimer’s
Association (PIO-99-1800) and the National Institutes of Health (AG10483, AG10845,
MH01644).
0025-7125/02/$ - see front matter 2002, Elsevier Science (USA). All rights reserved.
PII: S 0 0 2 5 - 7 1 2 5 ( 0 2 ) 0 0 0 0 6 - 8
642 L. Teri et al / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 641–656
Assessment
The most common method for assessing behavioral disturbance in cogni-
tively impaired older adults is a detailed clinical interview with the patient
and an informant familiar with the patient’s day-to-day functioning, such
as the patient’s family or caregiver. In the interview, the patient and infor-
mant are typically asked about the occurrence, frequency, and severity of
typical problems, including wandering, anxiety, depression, agitation, ag-
gression, sleep disturbances, paranoia, suspiciousness, and verbal or physical
threats or abuse. To ensure that the full spectrum of problems is assessed in
an efficient and thorough manner, a standardized inquiry list is a valuable
tool. Such a list may be administered by a trained staff person (eg, a medical
assistant) or provided to an informant to complete while the patient is being
examined. This process is similar to the preliminary health screening forms
used in many clinics. By asking the caregiver about the presence or absence
of a variety of potential problems, the clinician can then focus on the ones
that are of concern. In addition, the caregiver is given an opportunity to
express concerns on paper and, later, to discuss these concerns privately
with the clinician.
A variety of mood and behavior assessment measures with good psycho-
metric properties have been developed. Table 1 shows a representative sam-
pling of instruments that have been used to measure depression, agitation,
or general psychiatric disturbance in clinical controlled trials with dementia
subjects. Some measures are based on paper-and-pencil informant report,
whereas others require structured interviews with trained clinicians or direct
behavioral observation. The instruments also vary in the data format they
yield. Some (eg, the Cornell Scale for Depression in Dementia [15]) provide
information on the severity of symptoms; others (eg, the Revised Memory
and Behavior Problem Checklist [3]) rate behaviors on the frequency of their
occurrence and provide information about caregiver reactions to symptoms
when they do occur. For some measures (eg, the Neuropsychiatric Inventory
[29]), summary subscale information is computed from a combination of fre-
quency and severity ratings for individual behavioral symptoms. There are
also a number of self-report instruments that have been shown to be valid
when completed by surrogate informants, such as dementia caregivers [24].
As can be seen from Table 1, there is no single instrument that has gained
universal use in clinical trials with dementia patients. The selection of appro-
priate measures varies according to the outcomes targeted by each study,
and behaviors are often included as part of a comprehensive assessment with
other cognitive, functional, and psychosocial measures. Both researchers
and health care providers interested in using screening instruments should
consider a number of factors, including the length of the measure, its
reported sensitivity to change, and specific behaviors of concern for their
clinical population. Providers caring for populations that include non-
English-speaking individuals from multicultural backgrounds or persons
L. Teri et al / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 641–656 643
Table 1
Common mood and dementia screening instruments for dementia
Informant Reliability/ Used in clinical
report Interviewer validity controlled
Title questionnaire administered data trials
Agitation
Agitated behavior X X [10] X [11]
in dementia scale
(ABID) [9]
Cohen-Mansfield X X [10,13] X [14]
agitation inventory
(CMAI) [12]
Depression
Cornell scale for X X [16,17] X [18,19]
depression in dementia
(CSDD) [15]
Dementia mood X X [17,21] X [22]
assessment scale
(DMAS) [20]
Geriatric depression X X [24] X [19]
scale (GDS) [23]
Hamilton depression X X [24] X [19,26]
rating scale (HDRS) [25]
General behavioral disturbance
Behavioral pathology X X [16] X [14,18]
in Alzheimer’s disease
(BEHAVE-AD) [27]
CERAD behavior rating X X [10,17] X [26]
scale for dementia
(CERAD-BRSD) [28]
Neuropsychiatric X X [17] X [30]
inventory (NPI) [29]
Revised memory X X [10] X [19,26]
and behavior problem
checklist (RMBPC) [3]
CERAD: Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease.
Treatment
Nonpharmacologic treatments are often recommended as the most ap-
propriate initial strategy for managing behavioral disturbances in persons
with dementia [7,36,37]. The risk of adverse side effects or interactions with
other medications is nonexistent, and the increased involvement of care-
givers often has a secondary benefit of providing overburdened caregivers
with an opportunity to receive support, information, and skills. Further-
more, environmental factors (eg, confusing or noisy surroundings) or inter-
personal factors (eg, arguing with the patient) are frequently the primary
triggers of behavior problems. Attention to these factors through nonphar-
macologic approaches (caregiver education, support, and training in beha-
vioral strategies for management) can be quite effective in alleviating or
preventing behavioral problems in patients with dementia.
Providing education, support, and advice to help families better under-
stand and cope with the disease process has a long clinical history and often
represents the core of any treatment whether it is nonpharmacologic or
pharmacologic. Practitioners often advise families to use strategies to adapt
the environment and modify their own behaviors so as to better care for
patients. The amount and popularity of this kind of information have grown
exponentially in recent years as is evident in the proliferation of books,
training materials, informational handouts, videotapes and worldwide web
technology. For example, The 36 Hour Day [38], long considered a staple
of information to families, is in its second printing with over one-half mil-
lion copies sold worldwide. A videotape training program entitled ‘‘Manag-
ing and Understanding Behavior Problems in Alzheimer’s Disease and
Related Disorders’’ [39] has been used in Alzheimer Association support
groups worldwide. It is also part of standard training in numerous long-term
care facilities; has been translated into Japanese, Korean, and Italian; and
has been used in three controlled trials thus far.
Published reports of nonpharmacologic treatment span two decades. The
earliest reports showed that caregivers were able to learn specific techniques
and successfully reduce problematic behaviors [40–43]. Later, case-con-
trolled and randomized controlled trials investigated the effectiveness of
nonpharmacologic treatment in reducing behavioral problems or delaying
institutionalization in patients with dementia. Three investigations included
behavior training as part of a larger psychoeducational and social support
program and examined general behavioral outcomes and delayed patient
institutionalization [44–46]. One provided a focus on behavioral training
to reduce patient depression [19], and another compared nonpharmacologic
L. Teri et al / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 641–656 645
after the problem begins. Many problems have more than one antecedent
and consequence, and the more caregivers understand how the A-B-C’s fit
together, the more likely it is that they can successfully intervene.
Set realistic goals and make plans to achieve those goals. For a plan to
work, it must be creative but realistic and tailored to the individual pa-
tient and caregiver. For example, whereas a caregiver may ‘‘want the
patient to be happy,’’ this goal is too broad and unstructured. Goals
of ‘‘increasing pleasant activity’’ and ‘‘not discussing upsetting topics’’
are more realistic and more achievable. Start with small achievable
goals, and proceed step by step.
Encourage rewards for the patient and the caregiver for small successes.
Changing behavior is hard work for everyone involved. It is important
for caregivers to reward themselves and their patients for their accom-
plishments no matter how small.
Continually evaluate and modify plans. It is important that caregivers be
consistent in carrying out plans but flexible in trying new approaches.
Strategies need to change as caregivers discover some that do and do
not work.
One goal of the A-B-C approach is to help teach caregivers to develop
strategies on their own so that once treatment is concluded, they can effec-
tively deal with new problems that may develop over the course of the dis-
ease. In addition, specific suggestions can be helpful. In the Seattle Protocol,
these suggestions are again framed in an A-B-C approach. We now provide
some suggestions for management of several of the most common and trou-
bling behavior problems, including depression, agitation/aggression, wan-
dering, and sleep disturbances. For additional suggestions (although not
framed within an A-B-C approach), the reader is referred to the books by
Robinson et al [52], self-help books (eg, The 36 Hour Day [38] and Alzhei-
mer’s Disease: A Guide for Families [53]), and materials available from the
Alzheimer’s Association (www.alz.org; 1-800-272-3900).
Depression
Depressive symptoms occur in 30% to 70% of individuals diagnosed with
Alzheimer’s disease, the most common form of dementia in older adults [54–
56]. Unhappiness, withdrawal, inactivity, fatigue, expressions of guilt and
worthlessness, tearfulness, and loss of interest can all signify depression.
Patients with dementia gradually lose the ability to engage in enjoyable hob-
bies or activities, and because of their cognitive impairment, they may be
unable to identify replacement activities. Consequently, they may spend
much of their time in activities that are not gratifying or meaningful, or they
may disengage from activity altogether.
Methods for modifying antecedents and altering consequences to depres-
sive behaviors include:
L. Teri et al / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 641–656 647
Agitation
Agitated behaviors, such as irritability, restlessness, physical and verbal
aggression, resisting needed assistance, pacing, and wandering, affect be-
tween 70% and 90% of patients with dementia at some point during the
course of the disease [3,61]. As with depression, agitation may have a variety
of causes, including physiologic, environmental, and interpersonal triggers.
Furthermore, these causes often interact. For example, patients may have
decreased tolerance for stimulation or decreased inhibition of inappropriate
behaviors as a result of neurologic changes. They may experience agitation
or catastrophic reactions in response to environmental triggers, such as
overstimulation or activities they do not understand (eg, being bathed by
a caregiver they do not know or remember). The key to nonpharmacologic
intervention for episodes of agitation or aggression is to identify and avoid
their triggers, thus reducing their frequency or impact.
648 L. Teri et al / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 641–656
Wandering
Cognitively impaired individuals who wander pose a danger to themselves
and are of considerable concern to their care providers and family [62,63].
Most research on wandering has focused on nursing home– or dementia
clinic–based samples. In these samples, wandering occurs in up to 65% of
patients at some point in the disease process and is related to severity of cog-
nitive impairment [63–65], rate of cognitive decline [66], and decreased length
of survival [67,68]. In an investigation of disruptive wandering behavior in a
population-based sample of 193 individuals with Alzheimer’s disease, 24% of
subjects had wandered at least once, and 12% had wandered within the past
week. Wandering occurred in subjects at all levels of cognitive impairment,
but greater frequency of wandering was associated with significantly more
impairment in cognition, day-to-day functioning, and behavior. Caregiver
distress increased significantly with increased frequency of wandering [69].
There have been no published randomized controlled trials of interven-
tions for wandering in dementia [70]. Research on strategies for preventing
dangerous wandering has been conducted primarily in long-term care
settings, and promising approaches include environmental modification
[71,72], activity programming [73], electronic alarm systems [74], and rein-
forcement of nonwandering behaviors [75].
Methods for modifying antecedents and altering consequences of wan-
dering include:
• Environmental modification. Visual cues and labels can be used to direct
wanderers in a particular direction or away from potential dangers. For
example, clear and unambiguous labels on bathrooms and bedrooms may
L. Teri et al / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 641–656 649
Sleep disturbances
Wandering at night, bedtime agitation, disturbed sleep, and night-day
reversal are common problems for persons with dementia [76]. When pre-
sent, such problems are quite stressful for family members. Caregivers who
are awakened frequently during the night to reassure or assist the dementia
patient eventually become exhausted. This exhaustion, and its associated
impact on physical and emotional health, is one of the most common rea-
sons why caregivers are forced to institutionalize their loved ones [77,78].
There is general agreement that hypnotic treatments for sleep problems
should be used sparingly in the elderly so as to minimize falls, impaired
memory, drug-related sleep disorders, or aggravation of a preexisting sleep
apnea syndrome [79,80]. Cognitive-behavioral and psychoeducational strat-
egies are readily available and known to be effective in normal older adults
[81,82], but they have not been systematically studied in persons with
dementia. There is evidence that environmental and behavioral factors, such
as time spent in bed during the day, environmental noise and light, and
nighttime incontinence care practices, are associated with sleep disturbances
in institutionalized older adults [83–86]. One recent study in 29 nursing
650 L. Teri et al / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 641–656
home residents found that a combination of light physical exercise and other
behavioral strategies (eg, keeping patients out of bed in the late afternoon
and evening or providing quiet nighttime incontinence care) produced sig-
nificant improvements in measures of percentage of total sleep and duration
of sleep episodes [87]. There is also a growing amount of literature suggest-
ing that timed bright light exposure may be helpful in reducing the sleep dis-
turbances found in Alzheimer’s disease patients [88,89].
Methods for modifying antecedents and altering consequences associated
with sleep disturbances in persons with dementia include:
• Strive for consistent bedtime and rising times. Regular sleep time habits
and routines help to reinforce time cues for demented individuals and
may also promote circadian rhythmicity by maximizing cues to the
brain’s time-keeping system [90]. Although there have been no clinical
trials demonstrating the value of consistent sleep habits in dementia pa-
tients, sleep regularity is considered an important aspect of the treat-
ment and prevention of sleep disturbances in all older adults [80].
• Limit daytime napping. In the later stages of disease, many persons with
dementia spend a significant proportion of their daytime hours asleep
[91,92]. Reductions in daytime napping, particularly in combination
with an increase in physical activity, can increase the likelihood that
the dementia patient has longer and more consolidated nighttime sleep.
• Restrict use of alcohol and caffeinated beverages (including chocolate
products). When consumed close to bedtime, these can disrupt sleep
and exacerbate the preexisting tendency of dementia patients to awaken
more often and spend less time in deep (slow wave) sleep [93,94]. A light
bedtime snack comprising foods that promote sleep, such as milk, bana-
nas, or cheese, may reduce patient awakenings that are caused by night-
time hunger.
• Eliminate or reduce environmental nuisance factors that may interrupt
sleep. Reductions in light levels and nighttime noise have been shown to
be critical for the alleviation of sleep disturbances for nursing home res-
idents [83]. In community home settings, potential environmental
causes of disturbed patient sleep that should also be evaluated include
the impact of household pets, live-in adult children or grandchildren,
caregiver snoring, and outside traffic noise.
• Be aware that changes in daily routine, such as family get-togethers or
holidays, may produce a worsening in nighttime sleep. Try to plan
ahead, and build in some quiet ‘‘catch-up’’ time after any out-of-the-
ordinary planned activity.
Conclusion
Most if not all patients with dementia experience behavioral problems.
Some forms of dementia seem particularly prone to behavioral disturbances.
For example, individuals who have dementia with Lewy bodies may present
with psychotic symptoms early in the course of dementia [95,96]. Persons
with either frontotemporal or subcortical dementia often exhibit prominent
impairment in executive functioning (eg, problem solving, judgment, plan-
ning) that affects patient care and safety; mood and personality changes are
also common [97,98]. Whatever the cause, behavioral problems are preva-
lent, pervasive, and disruptive. They adversely affect patient and caregiver
alike, often leading to patient institutionalization and caregiver burnout.
It is important for health care providers to assess the occurrence and
impact of these behaviors so as to best care for their patients with dementia.
Measurement tools can augment the clinical interview to help the practi-
tioner obtain a thorough assessment of behavioral problems. Once identi-
fied, these problems may be amenable to nonpharmacologic treatments
designed to prevent or alleviate such problems. Although such strategies
have been subjected to few rigorous controlled clinical trials, extensive clin-
ical experience exists indicating their utility, and results thus far support the
use of these strategies in patients with dementia. Caregivers, whether they
652 L. Teri et al / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 641–656
are family members, long-term care staff, or health care professionals, are
critical to the assessment and treatment of behavioral problems. Oftentimes,
they are the sole providers of care for patients with dementia and must learn
how to manage the variety of problems that are experienced, including
depression, agitation, wandering, and sleep disturbance.
We have provided an overview of the literature in this area as well as
detailing one method using the A-B-C approach to patient care. We have
also provided specific suggestions as to how to begin a treatment plan
for common problems, such as depression, agitation/aggression, wandering,
and sleep disturbance. As more and more patients become elderly and more
and more elderly persons become demented, health care providers must
become familiar with nonpharmacologic treatment of behavioral problems
in dementia. Furthermore, they must be able to impart this knowledge to
those caregivers providing essential daily care of patients with dementia.
Acknowledgements
The authors thank Elizabeth ‘‘Buzzy’’ Mounce for her help in prepara-
tion of this article.
References
[1] Burns A. The Institute of Psychiatry Alzheimer’s disease cohort 1986–1992: part 1—clinical
observations. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 1996;11:309–20.
[2] Cummings JL, Kaufer D. Neuropsychiatric aspects of Alzheimer’s disease: the cholinergic
hypothesis revisited. Neurology 1996;47:876–83.
[3] Teri L, Truax P, Logsdon RG, Uomoto J, Zarit S, Vitaliano PP. Assessment of behavioral
problems in dementia: the Revised Memory and Behavior Problems Checklist. Psychol
Aging 1992;7:622–31.
[4] Fitz AG, Teri L. Depression, cognition, and functional ability in patients with Alzheimer’s
disease. J Am Geriatr Soc 1994;42:186–91.
[5] Vitaliano PP, Russo J, Young HM, Vitaliano PP, Russo J, Young HM, Teri L, Maiuro
RD. Predictors of burden in spouse caregivers of individuals with Alzheimer’s disease.
Psychol Aging 1991;6:392–402.
[6] Wagner AW, Logsdon RG, Pearson JL, Teri L. Caregiver expressed emotion and
depression in Alzheimer’s disease. Aging Ment Health 1997;1:132–9.
[7] Teri L, Rabins P, Whitehouse P, Berg L, Reisberg B, Sunderland T, et al. Management of
behavior disturbance in Alzheimer’s disease: current knowledge and future directions.
Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 1992;6:77–88.
[8] Small GN, Rabins PV, Barry PP, Buckholtz NS, DeKosky ST, Ferris SH, et al. Diagnosis
and treatment of Alzheimer disease and related disorders: consensus statement of the
American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry, the Alzheimer’s Association, and the
American Geriatrics Society. JAMA 1997;278:1363–71.
[9] Logsdon RG, Teri L, Weiner MF, Gibbons LE, Raskind M, Peskind E, et al. Assessment
of agitation in Alzheimer’s disease: the Agitated Behavior in Dementia Scale. J Am Geriatr
Soc 1999;47:1354–8.
[10] Weiner MF, Tractenberg R, Teri L, Logsdon R, Thomas RG, Gamst A, et al. Quantifying
behavioral disturbance in Alzheimer’s disease patients. J Psychiatr Res 2000;34:163–7.
L. Teri et al / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 641–656 653
[11] Teri L, Logsdon RG, Peskind E, Raskind M, Weiner MF, Tractenberg RE, et al. Treat-
ment of agitation in Alzheimer’s disease: a randomized placebo controlled clinical trial.
Neurology 2000;55:1271–8.
[12] Cohen-Mansfield J, Marx MS, Rosenthal AS. A description of agitation in a nursing home.
J Gerontol 1989;44:77–84.
[13] Koss E, Weiner M, Ernesto C, Cohen-Mansfield J, Ferris SH, Grundman M, et al. The
ADCS. Assessing patterns of agitation in Alzheimer’s disease patients with the Cohen-
Mansfield Agitation Inventory. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 1997;11(Suppl 2):S45–50.
[14] de Deyn P, Rabheru K, Rasmussen A, Bocksberger JP, Dautzenbert PL, Ericksson S, et al.
A randomized trial of risperidone, placebo, and haloperidol for behavioral symptoms of
dementia. Neurology 1999;53:946–55.
[15] Alexopoulos GS, Abrams RC, Young RC, Shamoian CA. Cornell scale for depression in
dementia. Biol Psychiatry 1988;23:271–84.
[16] Mack JL, Patterson MB. The evaluation of behavioral disturbances in Alzheimer’s disease:
the utility of three rating scales. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 1994;7:99–115.
[17] Perrault A, Oremus M, Demers L, Vida S, Wolfson C. Review of outcome measurement
instruments in Alzheimer’s disease drug trials: psychometric properties of behavior and
mood scales. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 2000;13:181–96.
[18] Lyketsos CG, Lindell Veiel L, Baker A, Steele C. A randomized, controlled trial of bright
light therapy for agitated behaviors in dementia patients residing in long-term care. Int J
Geriatr Psychiatry 1999;14:520–5.
[19] Teri L, Logsdon RG, Uomoto J, McCurry S. Behavioral treatment of depression in demen-
tia patients: a controlled clinical trial. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 1997;52:P159–66.
[20] Sunderland T, Hill JL, Lawlor BA, Molchan SE. NIMH Dementia Mood Assessment
Scale (DMAS). Psychopharmacol Bull 1988;24:747–51.
[21] Onega LL, Abraham IL. Factor structure of the Dementia Mood Assessment Scale in a
cohort of community-dwelling elderly. Int Psychogeriatr 1997;9:449–57.
[22] Lawlor BA, Aisen PS, Green C, Fine E, Schmeidler J. Selegiline in the treatment of
behavioural disturbance in Alzheimer’s disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 1997;12:319–22.
[23] Yesavage JA, Brink TL, Rose TL, Lum O, Huang V, Adey MB, et al. Development and
validation of a Geriatric Depression Screening Scale: a preliminary report. J Psychiatr Res
1983;17:37–49.
[24] Logsdon RG, Teri L. Depression in Alzheimer’s disease patients: caregivers as surrogate
reporters. J Am Geriatr Soc 1995;43:150–5.
[25] Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1960;23:
56–62.
[26] Teri L, McCurry SM, Buchner D, Logsdon RG, LaCroix A, Kukull WA, et al. Exercise
and activity level in Alzheimer’s disease: a potential treatment focus. J Rehabil Res Dev
1998;35:411–9.
[27] Reisberg B, Borenstein J, Salob SP, Ferris SH, Franssen E, Georgotas A. Behavioral
symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease: phenomenology and treatment. J Clin Psychiatry 1987;
48(Suppl 5):S9–15.
[28] Tariot P, Mack J, Patterson M, Edland SD, Weiner MF, Fillenbaum G, et al. The CERAD
Behavior Rating Scale for Dementia (BRSD). Am J Psychiatry 1995;152:1349–57.
[29] Cummings JL, Mega M, Gray K, Rosenberg-Thompson S, Carusi DA, Gornbein J. The
Neuropsychiatric Inventory: comprehensive assessment of psychopathology in dementia.
Neurology 1994;44:2308–14.
[30] Olin J, Schneider L. Galantamine for Alzheimer’s disease. (Cochrane Review). In: The
Cochrane Library. Oxford: Update Software; 2002.
[31] Katz IR. Diagnosis and treatment of depression in patients with Alzheimer’s disease and
other dementias. J Clin Psychiatry 1998;59(Suppl 9):S38–44.
[32] Kluger A, Ferris SH. Scales for the assessment of Alzheimer’s disease. Psychiatr Clin North
Am 1991;14:309–26.
654 L. Teri et al / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 641–656
[33] Sky AJ, Grossberg GT. The use of psychotropic medication in the management of problem
behaviors in the patient with Alzheimer’s disease. Med Clin North Am 1994;78:811–22.
[34] Teri L, Logsdon RG. Methodologic issues regarding outcome measures for clinical drug trials of
psychiatric complications in dementia. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurolm 1995;8(Suppl 1):S8–S17.
[35] Weiner MF, Schneider LS, Gray KF, Stern RG. Pharmacologic management and
treatment of dementia and secondary symptoms. In: Weiner MF, editor. The dementias:
diagnosis, management, and research. 2nd edition. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric
Press; 1996. p. 175–210.
[36] Rabins PV. Non-cognitive symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease. In: Terry RD, Katzman R,
Bick KL, editors. Alzheimer’s disease. New York: Raven Press; 1994. p. 419–29.
[37] Smith DA, Perry PJ. Nonneuroleptic treatment of disruptive behavior in organic mental
syndromes. Ann Pharmacother 1992;Vol 26(11):1400–08.
[38] Mace NL, Rabins PV. The 36 hour day. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press; 1991.
p. 89–110.
[39] Teri L. Managing and understanding behavior problems in Alzheimer’s disease and related
disorders. Video tape training program [training program with video tapes and written
manual]. Seattle: University of Washington; 1990.
[40] Aronson MK, Levin G, Lipkowitz R. A community based family/patient group program
for Alzheimer’s disease. Gerontologist 1984;24:339–42.
[41] Haley WE. A family-behavioral approach to the treatment of the cognitively impaired
elderly. Gerontologist 1983;23:18–20.
[42] Pinkston EM, Linsk N. Behavioral family intervention with the impaired elderly. Geron-
tologist 1984;24:576–83.
[43] Zarit S, Anthony C, Boutselis M. Interventions with care givers of dementia patients:
comparison of two approaches. Psychol Aging 1987;2:225–32.
[44] Carlson DL, Fleming KC, Smith GE, Evans JM. Management of dementia-related
behavioral disturbances: a nonpharmacological approach. Mayo Clin Proc 1995;70:1108–15.
[45] Mittelman MS, Ferris SH, Shulman E, Steinberg G, Levin B. A family intervention to
delay nursing home placement of patients with Alzheimer’s disease: a randomized con-
trolled trial. JAMA 1996;276:1725–31.
[46] Mohide EA, Pringle DM, Streiner DL, Gilbert JR, Muir G, Tew M. A randomized trial of
family caregiver support in the home management of dementia. J Am Geriatr Soc 1990;38:
446–54.
[47] Teri L, Logsdon RG, Whall AL, Weiner MF, Trimmer C, Peskind E, et al. Treatment for
agitation in dementia patients: a behavior management approach. Psychotherapy 1998;35:
436–43.
[48] Mittelman MS, Ferris SH, Steinberg G, Shulman E, Mackell JA, Ambinder A, et al. An
intervention that delays institutionalization of Alzheimer’s disease patients: treatment of
spouse-caregivers. Gerontologist 1993;33:730–40.
[49] Bourgeois MS, Schulz R, Burgio L. Interventions for caregivers of patients with
Alzheimer’s disease: a review and analysis of content, process, and outcomes. Int J Aging
Hum Dev 1996;43:35–92.
[50] Teri L, Logsdon R, Schindler R. Treatment of behavioral and mood disturbances in dementia.
Generations 1999;23:50–6.
[51] Teri L. Behavioral treatment of dementia: an overview. Semin Clin Neuropsychiatry 1997;
2:100–1.
[52] Robinson A, Spencer B, White L. Understanding difficult behaviors. Ypsilanti, MI: Eastern
Michigan University; 1991.
[53] Powell LS, Courtice K. Alzheimer’s disease: a guide for families. Reading, MA: Addison-
Wesley; 1983.
[54] Lazarus LW, Newton N, Cohler B, Lesser J, Schweon C. Frequency and presentation of
depressive symptoms in patients with primary degenerative dementia. Am J Psychiatry
1987;144:41–5.
L. Teri et al / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 641–656 655
[78] Pollak CP, Perlick D. Sleep problems and institutionalization of the elderly. J Geriatr
Psychiatry Neurol 1991;4:204–10.
[79] Kripke DF. Chronic hypnotic use: deadly risks, doubtful benefit. Sleep Med Rev 2000;
4:5–20.
[80] NIH. Consensus Conference. Treatment of sleep disorders of older people. Washington,
DC: National Institutes of Health; 1990.
[81] Chesson AL Jr, Anderson WM, Littner M, Davila D, Hartse K, Johnson S, et al. Practice
parameters for the nonpharmacologic treatment of chronic insomnia. Sleep 1999;22:1128–33.
[82] Edinger JD, Wohlgemuth WK. The significance and management of persistent primary
insomnia: the past, present and future of behavioral insomnia therapies. Sleep Med Rev
1999;3:101–18.
[83] Alessi CA, Schnelle JF. Approach to sleep disorders in nursing home setting. Sleep Med
Rev 2000;4:45–56.
[84] Gentili A, Weiner DK, Kuchibhatil M, Edinger JD. Factors that disturb sleep in nursing
home residents. Aging (Milano) 1997;9:207–13.
[85] Middlelkoop HA, Kerkhof GA, Smilde-van den Doel DA, Ligthart GJ, Kamphuisen HA.
Sleep and ageing: the effect of institutionalization on subjective and objective characteristics
of sleep. Age Aging 1994;23:411–7.
[86] Schnelle JF, Cruise PA, Alessi CA, Ludlow K, Al-Samarrai NR, Ouslander JG. Sleep
hygiene in physically dependent nursing home residents: behavioral and environmental
intervention implications. Sleep 1998;21:515–23.
[87] Alessi CA, Yoon EJ, Schnelle JF, Al-Samarrai NR, Cruise PA. A randomized trial of a
combined physical activity and environmental intervention in nursing home residents: do
sleep and agitation improve? J Am Geriatr Soc 1999;47:784–91.
[88] McCurry SM, Reynolds CF, Ancoli-Israel S, Teri L, Vitiello MV. Treatment of sleep
disturbance in Alzheimer’s disease. Sleep Med Rev 2000;4:603–8.
[89] van Someren EJW, Kessler A, Mirmiran M, Swaab DF. Indirect bright light improves
circadian rest-activity rhythm disturbances in demented patients. Biol Psychiatry 1997;41:
955–63.
[90] Taylor JO, Friedman L, Sheikh J, Yesavage JA. Assessment and management of
‘‘sundowning’’ phenomena. Semin Clin Neuropsychiatry 1997;2:113–22.
[91] Ancoli-Israel S, Klauber MR, Gillin JC, Campbell SS, Hofstetter CR. Sleep in non-
institutionalized Alzheimer’s disease patients. Aging Clin Exp Res 1994;6:451–8.
[92] Dowling GA, Wiener CL. Roadblocks encountered in recruiting patients for a study of
sleep disruption in Alzheimer’s disease. Image J Nurs Sch 1997;29:59–64.
[93] Vitiello MV, Prinz PN, Williams DE, Frommlet MS, Ries RK. Sleep disturbances in
patients with mild-stage Alzheimer’s disease. J Gerontol: Med Sci 1990;45:M131–8.
[94] Wooten V. Sleep disorders in geriatric patients. Clin Geriatr Med 1992;8:427–39.
[95] Ballard C, Holmes C, McKeith I, Neill D, O’Brien J, Cairns N, et al. Psychiatric morbidity
in dementia with Lewy bodies: a prospective clinical and neuropathological comparative
study with Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 1999;156:1039–45.
[96] Raskind M, Peskind E. Alzheimer’s disease and related disorders. Med Clin North Am
2001;85:803–17.
[97] Levy ML, Miller BL, Cummings JL, Fairbanks LA, Craig A. Alzheimer disease and
frontotemporal dementias: behavioral distinctions. Arch Neurol 1996;53:687–90.
[98] Perry RJ, Miller BL. Behavior and treatment in frontotemporal dementia. Neurology
2001;56(Suppl 4):S46–51.
Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 657–674
* Corresponding author.
E-mail address: [email protected] (L.T. Bonner).
This work was supported by the Department of Veterans Affairs and NIA AG05136 and
AG18644.
0025-7125/02/$ - see front matter Ó 2002, Elsevier Science (USA). All rights reserved.
PII: S 0 0 2 5 - 7 1 2 5 ( 0 2 ) 0 0 0 0 7 - X
658 L.T. Bonner, E.R. Peskind / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 657–674
Table 1
Cholinesterase inhibitor titration schedule
Titration period/maximum
Cholinesterase Starting dose increase per titration
inhibitor dose period Maximum dose
Donepezil 5 mg qhs 4–6 weeks/5 mg 10 mg qhs
Rivastigmine 1.5 mg bid 4–6 weeks/1.5 mg bid 6 mg bid (total of 12 mg)
Galantamine 4 mg bid 4–6 weeks/4 mg bid 12 mg bid (total of 24 mg)
Abbreviations: qhs, at bedtime; bid, twice daily.
Tacrine
Tacrine, the ‘‘first generation’’ cholinesterase inhibitor, was shown to
have modest efficacy in improving cognition in several large-scale placebo-
controlled trials in AD [7,8,10]. After 6 months of treatment, the drug-
placebo differences on the ADAS-Cog obtained from intent-to-treat analysis
(ITT) were approximately four points, interpreted as delay of cognitive
decline by approximately 6 months [7,8,10].
A retrospective analysis of the tacrine trial data suggests that tacrine
doses of greater than 80 mg/d may delay time to nursing home placement
and provides evidence for long-term cost-effective use of tacrine [42]. In this
study, 102 patients on low-dose or no tacrine (80 mg or less/d) were com-
pared with 198 patients on high-dose tacrine (>80 mg/d). Patients were fol-
lowed for approximately 800 days. At the end of this period, 45% of patients
in the low-dose or no-tacrine group had undergone nursing home placement
as compared to approximately 21% of patients in the higher dose tacrine
L.T. Bonner, E.R. Peskind / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 657–674 661
Donepezil
The piperidine derivative donepezil is a noncompetitive reversible acetyl-
cholinesterase inhibitor with specificity for the brain [45]. Donepezil has
been demonstrated to be modestly effective in stabilizing the cognitive func-
tion of AD patients in three large, randomized, placebo-controlled trials
[5,12,13]. These trials all used ITT analyses.
Donepezil is metabolized in the liver; its half-life is approximately 70
hours. Donepezil has no significant effect on the metabolism of other drugs,
including cimetidine, theophylline, warfarin, furosemide, and digoxin; done-
pezil metabolism is inhibited by ketoconazole and quinidine [44,46].
Donepezil at doses of 5 and 10 mg was compared with placebo in a
30-week treatment trial [13]. Four hundred seventy-three patients with
AD were randomly assigned to three treatment groups: placebo or 5 or 10
mg of donepezil. Significant and equivalent drug-versus-placebo group dif-
ferences were found for both the 5- and 10-mg donepezil groups on the
ADAS-Cog. The mean difference between active treatment and placebo
groups was approximately three points. There was no difference between the
5- and 10-mg doses. Donepezil was well tolerated, with no significant differ-
ence in occurrence of adverse events between the 5-mg and placebo groups.
An international study of 818 patients with the same study design demon-
strated similar results [5]. There were significant drug-versus-placebo differ-
ences on the ADAS-Cog. The mean drug-versus-placebo differences for the
5- and 10-mg donepezil groups were 1.5 and 2.9 points, respectively. A trial
in 468 patients consisting of a 12-week treatment phase followed by a
3-week washout phase compared 5 and 10 mg of donepezil with placebo and
662 L.T. Bonner, E.R. Peskind / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 657–674
Rivastigmine
Like donepezil and tacrine, rivastigmine is a noncompetitive reversible
inhibitor of acetylcholinesterase [47,48]. ‘‘Pseudoirreversible’’ enzyme inhibi-
tion has also been used to describe its mechanism of action, because the dura-
tion of inhibition of acetylcholinesterase induced by rivastigmine is longer
than its half-life [49,50]. Rivastigmine has relatively more inhibitory activity
for butyrylcholinesterase than acetylcholinesterase compared with other cho-
linesterase inhibitors [51]. The clinical relevance of this differential property
remains unclear. Compared with other cholinesterase inhibitors, rivastigmine
may have increased selectivity for the hippocampus and neocortex, which are
the areas of the brain preferentially affected by AD [47,51,52].
Rivastigmine is metabolized by the cholinesterases in brain, liver, and
intestine and is renally excreted [50,51]. Rivastigmine has been shown to
have no effect on the metabolism of other drugs, including warfarin, diaze-
pam, fluoxetine, and digoxin [50,51]. It has not been shown to affect hepatic
enzymes or other laboratory parameters [6,14,50,51].
Clinical trials indicate that rivastigmine is modestly effective in the treat-
ment of the cognitive impairments of mild to moderate AD [6,14]. Rivastig-
mine was compared with placebo in a 26-week international treatment trial
[14]. Seven hundred twenty-five patients with mild to moderate AD were ran-
domly assigned to three treatment groups of 1 to 4 mg of rivastigmine, 6 to 12
mg of rivastigmine, or placebo. Dose increases occurred weekly, with a max-
imum dose increase of 1.5 mg/d allowed during the 12-week dose escalation
phase. During weeks 8 through 12, no dose changes were permitted. There was
a 13-week maintenance phase during which the dose could be adjusted with the
goal of administering the highest tolerated dose. Mean drug-versus-placebo
differences on the ADAS-Cog were 0.17 for low-dose rivastigmine and 2.58 for
high-dose rivastigmine. Only the high dose differed significantly from placebo.
In the United States, a 26-week, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial of
699 patients compared low-dose rivastigmine (1–4 mg), high-dose rivastigmine
L.T. Bonner, E.R. Peskind / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 657–674 663
(6–12 mg), and placebo [6]. Patients in the 6- to 12-mg rivastigmine group
demonstrated significantly better cognitive function than patients treated with
placebo. The mean drug-versus-placebo difference in the ADAS-Cog between
the 6- to 12-mg group and the placebo group was 4.94 points. In addition,
patients receiving high-dose rivastigmine demonstrated better preservation
of functional ability as measured by the Progressive Deterioration Scale [53]
when compared with the placebo group.
There was an additional 6-month extension phase of the US trial in which
all patients who completed the double-blind phase were eligible for treatment
with a flexible maintenance dose of rivastigmine, ranging from a minimum of 1
mg administered twice daily to 6 mg administered twice daily [41]. Initially, all
patients were treated with open-label rivastigmine at a rate of 1 mg adminis-
tered twice daily for the first week, with maximum allowed dose increases of
1 mg administered twice daily per week for a maximum allowed total dose
of 6 mg administered twice daily. Decreases in dose were allowed per patient
tolerability, with a minimum required dose of 1 mg administered twice daily.
The change in ADAS-Cog score at week 52 was compared in four groups: the
three original treatment groups given placebo, 1 to 4 mg of rivastigmine, or 6
to 12 mg of rivastigmine, and a fourth group consisting of a theoretic 52-week
placebo group based on a modeling procedure of the projected decline if pla-
cebo had been continued for the full 52 weeks. Mean treatment differences on
the ADAS-Cog at week 52 for the original placebo group were 1.4 points ver-
sus the original 6- to 12-mg/d rivastigmine group, 0.3 points versus the original
1- to 4-mg/d rivastigmine group, and 4.3 points versus the theoretic 52-week
placebo group. The theoretic 52-week placebo group versus the original 6- to
12-mg/d rivastigmine group mean difference in ADAS-Cog scores was 5.7
points. These data suggest that rivastigmine may modify the course of AD and
are consistent with data for other cholinesterase inhibitors [11,39,40,42].
The recommended starting dose of rivastigmine is 1.5 mg administered
twice daily given with breakfast and dinner. Administration of rivastigmine
at mealtime helps to minimize nausea. Although the rivastigmine package
insert recommends titration increases at 2-week intervals, gastrointestinal
adverse effects may be minimized by using a slower 4- to 6-week titration
schedule. The dose should be increased to 3 mg administered twice daily,
then to 4.5 mg administered twice daily, and finally to 6 mg administered
twice daily at 4- to 6-week intervals as tolerated. As with all cholinesterase
inhibitors, the dose should be titrated slowly upward to the maximum toler-
ated dose. If rivastigmine therapy is interrupted for more than a few days, it
is recommended that treatment be restarted at 1.5 mg administered twice
daily (the lowest starting dose) and then titrated up again every 4 to 6 weeks
to the highest tolerated dose. Nausea, the most common side effect, can be
minimized by adequate hydration. If necessary, an antiemetic may be used.
As with other cholinesterase inhibitors, the incidence of gastrointestinal side
effects is dose dependent. Rivastigmine is the only cholinesterase inhibitor
with which weight loss is a clinically meaningful problem.
664 L.T. Bonner, E.R. Peskind / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 657–674
Galantamine
Galantamine hydrobromide, a derivative of the snowdrop plant, is the
most recently approved cholinesterase inhibitor. Unlike tacrine, donepezil,
and rivastigmine, it is a competitive and reversible acetylcholinesterase inhib-
itor. It has little effect on butyrylcholinesterase, being more selective for acet-
ylcholinesterase [54].
Galantamine is unique among cholinesterase inhibitors in having a poten-
tial second mechanism of action as an allosteric modulator at the nicotinic
cholinergic receptor [55,56]; this effect is separate from cholinesterase inhibi-
tion [55]. In vitro, galantamine acts as an ‘‘allosterically potentiating ligand,’’
increasing the likelihood of acetylcholine-induced channel opening and de-
creasing the rate of receptor desensitization [55]. This additional mechanism
of action is a possible explanation for the finding that the amount of cholines-
terase inhibition induced by galantamine and other cholinesterase inhibitors
does not correlate well with their clinical ‘‘potency’’ [55]. The clinical relevance
of this property is unknown, however.
Galantamine is metabolized by the liver [44,57] and has no effects on the
metabolism of other drugs. It is renally excreted. The half-life of galanta-
mine is 7.5 hours. Absorption is not affected by food.
The results of three published large-scale placebo-controlled trials indi-
cate that galantamine is modestly effective in the treatment of the cognitive
impairments of mild to moderate AD [11,15,16]. These trials also demon-
strate that galantamine preserves the activities of daily living. All trials
demonstrated that doses of 16, 24, and 32 mg were significantly statistically
superior to placebo.
Galantamine at doses of 8, 16, and 24 mg administered in divided doses
twice daily was compared with placebo in a 21-week treatment trial in 978
patients with AD [15]. Doses were started at 8 mg/d (4 mg twice-daily dosing)
and were increased by 8 mg/d every 4 weeks. Significant drug-versus-placebo
group differences were found for both the 16- and 24-mg galantamine groups
on the ADAS-Cog. The mean differences between the 16- and 24-mg–versus-
placebo groups were 3.3 and 3.6 points, respectively, on the ADAS-Cog,
which is equivalent to approximately 6 months of cognitive function. There
was not a statistically significant difference between the 16-mg versus 24-mg
dose groups. Both doses were well tolerated. The number of discontinua-
tions secondary to adverse events in the placebo and active treatment groups
was similar (7% in the placebo group and 6%–10% in the active treatment
groups) and increased in a dose-dependent fashion. In addition, galanta-
mine had similar beneficial effects on global function and behavior.
Galantamine at doses of 24 and 32 mg was compared with placebo in two
26-week treatment trials—one in the United States and the other interna-
tional [11,16]. Both studies demonstrated that galantamine significantly
improved cognitive function relative to placebo. Mean differences between
the 24- and 32- mg–versus-placebo groups on the ADAS-Cog were 3.9 and
L.T. Bonner, E.R. Peskind / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 657–674 665
3.8 points in the US trial and 3.1 and 4.1 points in the international trial,
a difference equivalent to approximately 6 months of cognitive function.
There was no statistically significant difference between the 24-mg versus
32-mg groups.
In the US trial, there was an additional 6-month extension phase during
which all patients who completed the double-blind phase were eligible for
open-label treatment with 24 mg of galantamine [11]. Patients taking
24 mg of galantamine for 12 months demonstrated preservation in activities
of daily living compared to patients who received placebo for six months
followed by 24 mg open-label galantamine for six months. Activities of daily
living were evaluated by using the Disability Assessment for Dementia [58].
Those patients who were randomized to placebo in the 26-week parallel-
group design who then went on to open-label active galantamine in the
6-month extension formed, in effect, a delayed-start design group. Interest-
ingly, this group of patients’ cognitive function then stabilized but never
reached the level of the patients who had received 12 months of continuous
active drug. Thus, at the 12-month time point, there remained a statisti-
cally significant difference in the ADAS-Cog scores between patients on
12-month continuous galantamine versus 6 months of placebo followed
by 6 months of galantamine (2 points on the ADAS-Cog). These data and
similar data from other cholinesterase inhibitor studies suggest these agents
may have disease-modifying effects that may slow disease progression in AD.
All trials demonstrated that galantamine had no effect on liver transami-
nases or other laboratory parameters. As with other cholinesterase inhibi-
tors, gastrointestinal adverse events were the most common side effects
and increased with increasing dose. Mild weight loss was also common.
The recommended starting dose of galantamine is 4 mg administered twice
daily given with breakfast and dinner. Administration of galantamine at
mealtime helps to minimize nausea associated with its use. The dose is
increased by 4 mg administered twice daily every 4 to 6 weeks as the patient
tolerates it. If galantamine therapy is interrupted for more than a few days, it
is recommended that treatment be restarted at 4 mg administered twice daily,
with the patient then titrated up every 4 to 6 weeks to the highest tolerated
dose. Nausea, the most common adverse event, usually resolves within a
week and can be further minimized by adequate hydration. If necessary, an
antiemetic may be used.
Summary
In summary, the cholinesterase inhibitors have provided the first known
effective treatments for the cognitive deficits of AD. Symptomatic efficacy is
modest and may include positive effects on memory, language, praxis, and
problematic noncognitive behaviors, such as pacing, delusions, and unco-
operativeness. It should be noted, however, that only approximately one
third of patients have clinically meaningful improvement in cognitive func-
tion. It is much more common for patients to have stabilization of their cog-
nitive, behavioral, and functional status. This is the message that should be
conveyed to patients and their families: the goal of treatment is stabilization.
Currently available data suggest that this period of stabilization at or near
baseline function lasts at least a year [11,29,39–42].
receive one of three active treatments or placebo for a 2-year period. The
active treatments consisted of vitamin E at a dose of 1000 IU administered
twice a day, selegiline (1-deprenyl) at a dose of 10 mg (given as 5 mg twice a
day), or a combination of vitamin E and selegiline. Primary outcome mea-
sures were indices of functional decline: progression from moderate to
severe dementia; loss of two of three basic activities of daily living, ie, feed-
ing, grooming, and toileting; nursing home placement; or death.
Vitamin E and selegiline both delayed progression to the outcome end-
points. The average delay for vitamin E was approximately 230 days com-
pared with placebo. Because of its low cost and relative safety, vitamin E
is now recommended in addition to a cholinesterase inhibitor for slowing
progression in AD. Vitamin E is started at a dosage of 400 IU/d and is
increased by 400 IU as tolerated each week until the maximum dose of
2000 IU in divided doses administered twice daily is reached. Because vita-
min E inhibits platelet adhesion [63,64] and may alter vitamin K–dependent
coagulation factors [65,66], its use is contraindicated in patients with vita-
min K deficiency, and cautious use is recommended in patients receiving
anticoagulant therapy.
Ginkgo biloba
Ginkgo biloba is a traditional Chinese herbal medicine with reputed
mood-elevating, cognitive-enhancing, and behavior-altering properties. Its
theoretic properties include anti-inflammatory and antioxidant effects
[67,68]. Although its safety profile has not been fully characterized, ginkgo
biloba is a modest anticoagulant [68].
EGB 761, a standardized extract of the Ginkgo biloba tree, was evaluated in
a 52-week, double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled trial to assess effi-
cacy and safety in patients with AD and VaD [69]. Primary outcome measures
included the ADAS-Cog and the Clinical Global Impression of Change. In
addition, the nonvalidated Geriatric Evaluation by Relative Rating Instru-
ment (GERRI) [70], a caregiver-rated measure of patient functioning, was
used as an outcome measure. The GERRI is a 49-item inventory that includes
a range of scores from 1 to 5 in which an increasing score indicates poorer
patient functioning. An ITT analysis was used to compare data.
When compared with placebo, EGB 761 at a dose of 40 mg three times
a day demonstrated a small but statistically significant difference in the
ADAS-Cog with a drug-versus-placebo difference of 1.4 points. There was
also a statistically significant drug-versus-placebo group difference in the
GERRI of 0.14 points. There was no difference demonstrated in the Clinical
Global Impression of Change. The number of adverse events was similar in
the placebo and active treatment groups. There was a high attrition rate dur-
ing the 52-week trial; only 137 of the 309 subjects included in the ITT anal-
ysis completed the trial. The effects of EGB 761 on agitation and mood were
assessed but not reported.
668 L.T. Bonner, E.R. Peskind / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 657–674
The results of this study must be interpreted with caution. There are a
number of methodologic flaws that render interpretation of the data some-
what difficult. Until the results of this study are replicated and the safety
profile of ginkgo biloba is further clarified with regard to potential for caus-
ing significant bleeding events, the use of ginkgo biloba in AD is not sup-
ported by the available data. In addition, the manufacture of ginkgo
biloba and other herbal supplements is unregulated and lacks FDA-
mandated standards for potency and purity.
Summary
The management of dementia patients encompasses pharmacologic,
behavioral, and psychosocial intervention strategies. Before pharmacologic
intervention is instituted, it is important that sources of excess disability and
comorbidity be eliminated or reduced. Identification of comorbid medical
and psychiatric conditions, such as depression and delirium, should be iden-
tified and appropriately treated. Providing caregivers with education, sup-
port, and practical advice is a critical component of the management of
the demented patient.
The current standard of care for pharmacologic management of the cog-
nitive and functional disabilities of AD consists of the combination of a cho-
linesterase inhibitor and high-dose vitamin E. This standard is based on the
results of large-scale, double-blind, placebo-controlled trials.
Cholinesterase inhibitors are the only FDA-approved pharmacologic
treatments for AD. Cholinesterase inhibitors have been shown to be effec-
tive in the treatment of the cognitive, behavioral, and functional deficits
of AD. Large-scale placebo-controlled trials of tacrine, donepezil, rivastig-
mine, and galantamine have demonstrated moderate benefits in patients
with mild to moderate AD. Donepezil, rivastigmine, and galantamine are
the first-line choices in the treatment of AD because of their lack of hepato-
toxicity, ease of administration, few significant drug-drug interactions, and
mild to moderate side effects.
670 L.T. Bonner, E.R. Peskind / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 657–674
References
[1] Small GW, Rabins PV, Barry PP, et al. Diagnosis and treatment of Alzheimer disease
and related disorders. Consensus statement of the American Association for Geriatric Psy-
chiatry, the Alzheimer’s Association, and the American Geriatrics Society. JAMA 1997;
278:1363–71.
[2] McKeith IG, Galasko D, Kosaka K, et al. Consensus guidelines for the clinical and patho-
logic diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB): report of the Consortium on DLB
International Workshop. Neurology 1996;47:1113–24.
[3] Costa P, Williams T, Summerfield M, et al. Recognition and initial assessment of
Alzheimer’s disease and related dementias. Rockville, MD: US Department of Health and
Human Services, Public Health Service, Agency for Health Care Policy and Research,
AHCPR publication 97–0702, 1996.
[4] Evans DA. Estimated prevalence of Alzheimer’s disease in the United States. Milbank Q
1990;68:267–89.
[5] Burns A, Rossor M, Hecker J, et al. The effects of donepezil in Alzheimer’s disease—results
from a multinational trial. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 1999;10:237–44.
[6] Corey-Bloom J, Anand R, Veach J, et al. A randomized trial evaluating the efficacy and
safety of ENA 713 (rivastigmine tartrate), a new acetylcholinesterase inhibitor, in patients
with mild to moderately severe Alzheimer’s disease. Int J Geriatr Psychopharmacol 1998;
1:55–65.
L.T. Bonner, E.R. Peskind / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 657–674 671
[7] Davis KL, Thal LJ, Gamzu ER, et al. A double-blind, placebo-controlled multicenter study
of tacrine for Alzheimer’s disease. The Tacrine Collaborative Study Group. N Engl J Med
1992;327:1253–9.
[8] Farlow M, Gracon SI, Hershey LA, et al. A controlled trial of tacrine in Alzheimer’s
disease. The Tacrine Study Group. JAMA 1992;268:2523–9.
[9] Feldman H, Gauthier S, Hecker J, et al. A 24-week, randomized, double-blind study of
donepezil in moderate to severe Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 2001;57:613–20.
[10] Knapp MJ, Knopman DS, Solomon PR, et al. A 30-week randomized controlled trial of
high-dose tacrine in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. The Tacrine Study Group. JAMA
1994;271:985–91.
[11] Raskind MA, Peskind ER, Wessel T, et al. Galantamine in AD: a 6-month randomized,
placebo-controlled trial with a 6-month extension. The Galantamine USA-1 Study Group.
Neurology 2000;54:2261–8.
[12] Rogers SL, Doody RS, Mohs RC, et al. Donepezil improves cognition and global function
in Alzheimer disease: a 15-week, double-blind, placebo-controlled study. Donepezil Study
Group. Arch Intern Med 1998;158:1021–31.
[13] Rogers SL, Farlow MR, Doody RS, et al. A 24-week, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial
of donepezil in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Donepezil Study Group. Neurology 1998;
50:136–45.
[14] Rosler M, Anand R, Cicin-Sain A, et al. Efficacy and safety of rivastigmine in patients with
Alzheimer’s disease: international randomised controlled trial. BMJ 1999;318:633–8.
[15] Tariot PN, Solomon PR, Morris JC, et al. A 5-month randomized placebo-controlled trial of
galantamine in AD. The Galantamine USA-10 Study Group. Neurology 2000;54:2269–76.
[16] Wilcock GK, Lilienfeld S, Gaens E. Efficacy and safety of galantamine in patients with
mild to moderate Alzheimer’s disease: multicentre randomised controlled trial. Galanta-
mine International-1 Study Group. BMJ 2000;321:1445–9.
[17] Sano M, Ernesto C, Thomas RG, et al. A controlled trial of selegiline, alpha-tocopherol, or
both as treatment for Alzheimer’s diseaseThe Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study.
N Engl J Med 1997;336:1216–22.
[18] McGeer PL, McGeer EG, Suzuki J, et al. Aging, Alzheimer’s disease, and the cholinergic
system of the basal forebrain. Neurology 1984;34:741–5.
[19] Perry E, Perry R, Gibson P, et al. A cholinergic connection between normal aging and
senile dementia in the human hippocampus. Neurosci Lett 1977;6:85–9.
[20] Rossor MN, Iversen LL, Reynolds GP, et al. Neurochemical characteristics of early and
late onset types of Alzheimer’s disease. BMJ 1984;288:961–4.
[21] Whitehouse PJ, Price DL, Struble RG, et al. Alzheimer’s disease and senile dementia: loss
of neurons in the basal forebrain. Science 1982;215:1237–9.
[22] Davies P, Maloney AJ. Selective loss of central cholinergic neurons in Alzheimer’s disease.
Lancet 1976;2:1403.
[23] Arai H, Ichimiya Y, Kosaka K, et al. Neurotransmitter changes in early- and late-onset
Alzheimer-type dementia. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 1992;16:883–90.
[24] Hansen LA, DeTeresa R, Davies P, et al. Neocortical morphometry, lesion counts, and
choline acetyltransferase levels in the age spectrum of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 1988;
38:48–54.
[25] Perry EK, Perry RH, Smith CJ, et al. Nicotinic receptor abnormalities in Alzheimer’s and
Parkinson’s diseases. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1987;50:806–9.
[26] Whitehouse PJ, Martino AM, Antuono PG, et al. Nicotinic acetylcholine binding sites in
Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Res 1986;371:146–51.
[27] Etienne P, Gauthier S, Dastoor D, et al. Alzheimer’s disease: clinical effects of lecithin
treatment. In: Barbeau A, Growden J, Wurtman R, editors. Nutrition and the brain, vol. 5.
New York: Raven Press; 1969. p. 389–90.
[28] Etienne P, Gauthier S, Johnson G, et al. Clinical effects of choline in Alzheimer’s disease.
Lancet 1978;1:508–9.
672 L.T. Bonner, E.R. Peskind / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 657–674
[29] Mohs RC, Davis KL, Tinklenberg JR, et al. Choline chloride treatment of memory deficits
in the elderly. Am J Psychiatry 1979;136:1275–7.
[30] Peters BH, Levin HS. Effects of physostigmine and lecithin on memory in Alzheimer disease.
Ann Neurol 1979;6:219–21.
[31] Sitaram N, Weingartner H, Gillin JC. Human serial learning: enhancement with arecoline
and choline impairment with scopolamine. Science 1978;201:274–6.
[32] Sunderland T, Tariot PN, Newhouse PA. Differential responsivity of mood, behavior, and
cognition to cholinergic agents in elderly neuropsychiatric populations. Brain Res 1988;
472:371–89.
[33] Summers WK, Majovski L, Marsh G, et al. Oral tetrahydroaminoacridine in long-term
treatment of senile dementia, Alzheimer type. N Engl J Med 1986;315:1241–5.
[34] Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. ‘‘Mini-mental state.’’ A practical method for
grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975;12:189–98.
[35] American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders.
3rd edition (revised). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1987.
[36] American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders.
4th edition. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994.
[37] McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, et al. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease:
report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health
and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology 1984;34:939–44.
[38] Rosen WG, Mohs RC, Davis KL. A new rating scale for Alzheimer’s disease. Am J
Psychiatry 1984;141:1356–64.
[39] Doody RS, Dunn JK, Clark CM, et al. Chronic donepezil treatment is associated with
slowed cognitive decline in Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2001;12:
295–300.
[40] Doody RS, Geldmacher DS, Gordon B, et al. Open-label, multicenter, phase 3 extension
study of the safety and efficacy of donepezil in patients with Alzheimer disease. Arch
Neurol 2001;58:427–33.
[41] Farlow M, Anand R, Messina J Jr, et al. A 52-week study of the efficacy of rivastigmine in
patients with mild to moderately severe Alzheimer’s disease. Eur Neurol 2000;44:236–41.
[42] Knopman D, Schneider L, Davis K, et al. Long-term tacrine (Cognex) treatment: effects on
nursing home placement and mortality. Tacrine Study Group. Neurology 1996;47:166–77.
[43] Watkins PB, Zimmerman HJ, Knapp MJ, et al. Hepatotoxic effects of tacrine adminis-
tration in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. JAMA 1994;271:992–8.
[44] Nordberg A, Svensson AL. Cholinesterase inhibitors in the treatment of Alzheimer’s
disease: a comparison of tolerability and pharmacology. Drug Saf 1998;19:465–80.
[45] Taylor P. Development of acetylcholinesterase inhibitors in the therapy of Alzheimer’s
disease. Neurology 1998;51(Suppl):S30–7.
[46] Shintani EY, Uchida KM. Donepezil: an anticholinesterase inhibitor for Alzheimer’s
disease. Am J Health Syst Pharm 1997;54:2805–10.
[47] Cutler NR, Polinsky RJ, Sramek JJ, et al. Dose-dependent CSF acetylcholinesterase inhi-
bition by SDZ ENA 713 in Alzheimer’s disease. Acta Neurol Scand 1998;97:244–50.
[48] Enz A, Boddeke H, Gray J, et al. Pharmacologic and clinicopharmacologic properties
of SDZ ENA 713, a centrally selective acetylcholinesterase inhibitor. Ann NY Acad Sci
1991;640:272–5.
[49] Kennedy JS, Polinsky RJ, Johnson B, et al. Preferential cerebrospinal fluid acetylcholi-
nesterase inhibition by rivastigmine in humans. J Clin Psychopharmacol 1999;19:513–21.
[50] Polinsky RJ. Clinical pharmacology of rivastigmine: a new-generation acetylcholinesterase
inhibitor for the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. Clin Ther 1998;20:634–47.
[51] Spencer CM, Noble S. Rivastigmine. A review of its use in Alzheimer’s disease. Drugs
Aging 1998;13:391–411.
[52] Enz A, Amstutz R, Boddeke H, et al. Brain selective inhibition of acetylcholinesterase:
a novel approach to therapy for Alzheimer’s disease. Prog Brain Res 1993;98:431–8.
L.T. Bonner, E.R. Peskind / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 657–674 673
[53] DeJong R, Osterlund OW, Roy GW. Measurement of quality-of-life changes in patients
with Alzheimer’s disease. Clin Ther 1989;11:545–54.
[54] Thomsen T, Kewitz H. Selective inhibition of human acetylcholinesterase by galanthamine
in vitro and in vivo. Life Sci 1990;46:1553–8.
[55] Maelicke A, Samochocki M, Jostock R, et al. Allosteric sensitization of nicotinic receptors by
galantamine, a new treatment strategy for Alzheimer’s disease. Biol Psychiatry 2001;49:279–88.
[56] Samochocki M, Zerlin M, Jostock R, et al. Galantamine is an allosterically potentiating
ligand of the human alpha4/beta2 nAChR. Acta Neurol Scand 2000;176:68–73.
[57] Bachus R, Bickel U, Thomsen T, et al. The O-demethylation of the antidementia drug
galanthamine is catalysed by cytochrome P450 2D6. Pharmacogenetics 1999;9:661–8.
[58] Gelinas I, Gauthier L, McIntyre M, et al. Development of a functional measure for persons
with Alzheimer’s disease: the disability assessment for dementia. Am J Occup Ther 1999;53:
471–81.
[59] Knopman DS, Knapp MJ, Gracon SI, et al. The Clinician Interview-Based Impression (CIBI):
a clinician’s global change rating scale in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 1994;44:2315–21.
[60] Hallliwell B, Gutteridge J. Oxygen radicals in the nervous system. Trends Neurosci 1985;
8:22–6.
[61] Hara H, Kato H, Kogure K. Protective effect of alpha-tocopherol on ischemic neuronal
damage in the gerbil hippocampus. Brain Res 1990;510:335–8.
[62] Volicer L, Crino PB. Involvement of free radicals in dementia of the Alzheimer type:
a hypothesis. Neurobiol Aging 1990;11:567–71.
[63] Jandak J, Steiner M, Richardson PD. Alpha-tocopherol, an effective inhibitor of platelet
adhesion. Blood 1989;73:141–9.
[64] Steiner M. Vitamin E, a modifier of platelet function: rationale and use in cardiovascular
and cerebrovascular disease. Nutr Rev 1999;57:306–9.
[65] Corrigan JJ Jr, Marcus FI. Coagulopathy associated with vitamin E ingestion. JAMA
1974;230:1300–1.
[66] Corrigan JJ Jr, Ulfers LL. Effect of vitamin E on prothrombin levels in warfarin-induced
vitamin K deficiency. Am J Clin Nutr 1981;34:1701–5.
[67] Oyama Y, Chikahisa L, Ueha T, et al. Ginkgo biloba extract protects brain neurons
against oxidative stress induced by hydrogen peroxide. Brain Res 1996;712:349–52.
[68] Yoshikawa T, Naito Y, Kondo M. Ginkgo biloba leaf extract: review of biological actions
and clinical applications. Antioxid Redox Signal 1999;1:469–80.
[69] Le Bars PL, Katz MM, Berman N, et al. A placebo-controlled, double-blind, randomized
trial of an extract of Ginkgo biloba for dementia. North American EGb Study Group.
JAMA 1997;278:1327–32.
[70] Schwartz GE. Development and validation of the geriatric evaluation by relatives rating
instrument (GERRI). Psychol Rep 1983;53:479–88.
[71] Hansen L, Salmon D, Galasko D, et al. The Lewy body variant of Alzheimer’s disease:
a clinical and pathologic entity. Neurology 1990;40:1–8.
[72] Perry RH, Irving D, Blessed G, et al. Senile dementia of Lewy body type. A clinically and
neuropathologically distinct form of Lewy body dementia in the elderly. J Neurol Sci
1990;95:119–39.
[73] Perry EK, Haroutunian V, Davis KL, et al. Neocortical cholinergic activities differentiate
Lewy body dementia from classical Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroreport 1994;5:747–9.
[74] Perry EK, McKeith I, Thompson P, et al. Topography, extent, and clinical relevance
of neurochemical deficits in dementia of Lewy body type, Parkinson’s disease, and
Alzheimer’s disease. Ann NY Acad Sci 1991;640:197–202.
[75] Lebert F, Pasquier F, Souliez L, et al. Tacrine efficacy in Lewy body dementia. Int J Geriatr
Psychiatry 1998;13:516–9.
[76] McKeith I, Del Ser T, Spano P, et al. Efficacy of rivastigmine in dementia with Lewy bodies:
a randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled international study. Lancet 2000;356:
2031–6.
674 L.T. Bonner, E.R. Peskind / Med Clin N Am 86 (2002) 657–674
[77] Shea C, MacKnight C, Rockwood K. Donepezil for treatment of dementia with Lewy
bodies: a case series of nine patients. Int Psychogeriatr 1998;10:229–38.
[78] Cummings JL, Mega M, Gray K, et al. The Neuropsychiatric Inventory: comprehensive
assessment of psychopathology in dementia. Neurology 1994;44:2308–14.
[79] Gorelick PB, Erkinjuntti T, Hofman A, et al. Prevention of vascular dementia. Alzheimer
Dis Assoc Disord 1999;13(Suppl 3):S131–9.
[80] Nyenhuis DL, Gorelick PB. Vascular dementia: a contemporary review of epidemiology,
diagnosis, prevention, and treatment. J Am Geriatr Soc 1998;46:1437–48.