Gojman de Millan S Millan S Et Al 2013
Gojman de Millan S Millan S Et Al 2013
Gojman de Millan S Millan S Et Al 2013
Citation or publication of
material prohibited without express written permission of the copyright holder.
Eigentum des Erich Fromm Dokumentationszentrums. Nutzung nur für persönliche Zwecke. Ver-
öffentlichungen – auch von Teilen – bedürfen der schriftlichen Erlaubnis des Rechteinhabers.
Gojman_de_Millan_S_Millan_S_et_al_2013
„Care Giving and Social Character. Towards a Systematization of the Clinical Assessment
of Social Character Traits and Their Relation to Mothers’ Care Giving Quality in Ur-
ban/Rural Mexican Samples,“ in: Fromm Forum (English Edition – ISBN 1437-1189), 17 /
2013, Tuebingen (Selbstverlag), pp. 35-46.
Copyright © 2013 by Sonia Gojman, Salvador Millán, Guadalupe Sanchez, Patricia Gon-
zález and Angélica Rodarte. Seminario de Sociopsicoanálisis A.C. SEMSOAC; E-Mail:
[email protected]; [email protected]. - The authors thank Teresa Villarreal
who has with her enthusiasm promoted the development of the Attachment Research Pro-
ject of SEMSOAC and Mary Main for her continual teachings and encouragement.
Abstract: The theory of social character developed by Erich Fromm asserts that the
significance of experience is intimately linked to the material conditions under which
persons, located in a specific economic system, live. The shared character traits thus
correspond to the social role that these persons play. In the case of women, existing
literature does not focus on their function as reproducers of the work force within the
family, although this is a generalized function and additional to out-of-home work. This
function has a transcendent importance to the development and emotional health of
children, as has been established in longitudinal research of attachment theory.
This study explores Mexican mothers’ shared character traits, a systematized appraisal
of their attitudes towards their lives and their children, their states of mind, and the
quality of care they provide as well as their relationship to attachment patterns of their
infants; some come from urban Middle, Middle-upper and Upper-class families and
others from Poor indigenous peasant families.
The fundamental focus is on the woman’s participation inside the family, although it
does not exclude the possibility that some of these mothers handle not only the care
giving and housework but also work outside of the home (the "double shift”). It con-
cludes that the daily material conditions of existence are closely related to shared
character traits and that these are in turn related to patterns of attachment in their in-
fants, their sensitivity in the treatment of their babies, and the results of their Adult At-
tachment Interviews. Women have been the support and pillar of subsistence for urban
populations as well as the most poor and disadvantaged communities. This role played
by women is as widespread as undocumented in literature.
page/Seite 1 of/von 16
Gojman de Millan, S., Millan, S., et al., 2013
Care Giving and Social Character
Propriety of the Erich Fromm Document Center. For personal use only. Citation or publication of
material prohibited without express written permission of the copyright holder.
Eigentum des Erich Fromm Dokumentationszentrums. Nutzung nur für persönliche Zwecke. Ver-
öffentlichungen – auch von Teilen – bedürfen der schriftlichen Erlaubnis des Rechteinhabers.
human need to relate to others and the person’s position within the economic social
system in which he/she develops and performs (cf. Fromm, 1962; 1970).
In Social Character in a Mexican village. A Socio Psychoanalytic Study, Fromm and
Maccoby (1970) tested their approach to the theory of character and essentially con-
firmed it through their observation of peasant labor and the type of demands placed
upon them by the nascent industrialization of the sugar industry in the state of Morelos,
Mexico. The role of the woman was analyzed as subrogated to that of the man, the
provider. The peasant women are described as "fundamentally dominant hoarding” and
with "masochistic tendencies” (ibid., pp. 144-145), while the demands of household du-
ties such as maintaining order and cleanliness were central to their day-to-day activi-
ties. The feudal or traditional mindset made them ideologically "property of the man”
and "they were expected to treat themselves as property” (ibid., p. 146).
The peasant village in Morelos, that Fromm and Maccoby studied, was composed of
homes that, albeit humble, had some urban services, like streets, electricity, water and
a health center, as part of the incipient modernization and industrialization of rural Mex-
ico. The authors mention the problem of alcoholism and its negative effects, particularly
among receptive-unproductive peasants. Later on, the inhabitants of the village experi-
enced an admiration and yearning for the advancement opportunities that existed in the
US, the prelude to a mass emigration that would take place decades later due to the
abandoned rural support policies.
The Method
The research project was based on the social character study conducted in Mexico by
the Seminario de Sociopsicoanálisis (SEMSOAC) (Gojman, 2000; 2003; Gojman &
Millán, 2007; 2008; Gojman et al., 2012; Gojman et al., in press a and b) among 70
mother-infant dyads, 39 of which belonged to urban Middle- and Upper-middle- or Up-
per-class families and 31 belonged to Poor, rural Nahua families like so many others
that exist in Mexico and are common throughout Latin America.
Because the questionnaire did not pose specific questions relating to the socio-
economic status of respondents, we reviewed the answers to the questions and de-
tected responses that might provide indicators of that status. The social character
questionnaires were sorted into rural and urban based on the place the interviews were
conducted. Based on the responses to the questionnaires, we were able to detect 10
aspects considered pertinent for determining the socioeconomic status of the women
interviewed: type of home, means of transport, husband’s occupation, woman’s occu-
pation, schooling, health services, use of free time, resources for services, parents’ oc-
cupation, and type of toys. A series of indicative elements were selected for each as-
pect and an economic scale assigned. This information enabled us to classify the re-
spondents into 5 socioeconomic brackets: Middle-upper/upper, Middle-middle, Middle-
lower, Poor and Very poor.
Mothers responded to the Social Interpretative Questionnaire (Fromm & Maccoby,
1970) and the Adult Attachment Interview (Main & Goldwyn, 1984; Main Goldwyn &
Hesse, 1998, Gojman, 2004); mother-infant dyads were videotaped twice in their
homes (Ainsworth et al.,1978), interacting with their infants (at least two weeks apart),
and infant-mother dyads participated in the videotaped Strange Situation laboratory
page/Seite 2 of/von 16
Gojman de Millan, S., Millan, S., et al., 2013
Care Giving and Social Character
Propriety of the Erich Fromm Document Center. For personal use only. Citation or publication of
material prohibited without express written permission of the copyright holder.
Eigentum des Erich Fromm Dokumentationszentrums. Nutzung nur für persönliche Zwecke. Ver-
öffentlichungen – auch von Teilen – bedürfen der schriftlichen Erlaubnis des Rechteinhabers.
Participants
The participants were 70 mothers (ranging from 15 to 44 years of age at the time of as-
sessment, M: 28.4) and their infants (ranging from 8 to 26 months, M: 13.5--40 boys
and 30 girls). 11.4 % were illiterate, 8.6% had not finished elementary school, 4.3%
had finished elementary school, 10% had studied something else than elementary
school or finished high school, 12.9% had finished preparatoria (equivalent to college),
28.6% professional studies, and 5.7% graduate studies (18.6% were missing the in-
formation). 18.6% had full time jobs, 45.7% combined housekeeping with part time jobs
and 35.7% were not working outside the home. 24.7% were Very poor, 13.7% were
Poor, 9.6% were Medium low, 20.5% were Medium-medium, 30.1% were Medium-
upper and 1.4% were Upper-income. These mothers were drawn from two different
groups:
I. The urban group with 39 native Spanish speaking mothers plus one fluent Span-
ish speaking mother whose native language was Portuguese (ages 21-44, M: 31.8)
and their infants (10-15 months, M: 13.1, 25 boys, 14 girls). Two were Poor, two were
Middle-lower income and the rest were middle to Middle-upper and upper income, ur-
ban mestizo families who live in Mexico City: 11% had studied something else than
elementary school or finished high school, 15% had finished preparatoria (equivalent to
college), 64% professional studies, and 10% graduate studies; 14 % had full time jobs,
42% combined housekeeping with part time jobs and 44% were not working outside
the home.
They were invited and voluntarily agreed to participate through a psycho-prophylactic
training center, through the breast feeding league or a kindergarten that was starting an
early stimulation project with infants.
II. The rural group with 31 mothers (ranging from 16 to 38 years, M: 26.1) and their
infants (ranging from 9 to 26 month,s M: 14, 15 boys and 16 girls) from poor to very
poor, peasant Nahua families who inhabit in a Nahua, coffee farming village of the Si-
page/Seite 3 of/von 16
Gojman de Millan, S., Millan, S., et al., 2013
Care Giving and Social Character
Propriety of the Erich Fromm Document Center. For personal use only. Citation or publication of
material prohibited without express written permission of the copyright holder.
Eigentum des Erich Fromm Dokumentationszentrums. Nutzung nur für persönliche Zwecke. Ver-
öffentlichungen – auch von Teilen – bedürfen der schriftlichen Erlaubnis des Rechteinhabers.
erra Norte region in the State of Puebla. 38% were illiterate, 24% had not finished ele-
mentary school, 9% had finished elementary school, 5% had studied something else
than elementary school or finished high school, 24% had finished preparatoria (an
equivalent to college). 19% had full time jobs, 39% combined housekeeping with part
time jobs and 42% were not working outside the home. They were classified as:
1) Very poor (peasant families without land, living in houses without basic services
such as water supply or electric power, floors without cement, just leveled earth
and/or straw ceilings);
2) Poor (peasants with their own land or working trades such as bus driver, construc-
tion or carpenter workers, their families counting with at least some of the elemen-
tary services of water and electric power) or
3) Medium-lower income (elementary school teachers, nurses or small grocery mer-
chants whose families live in houses with the main services, cement floors and
ceilings, as well as some domestic appliances).
Most of them were bilingual–although some are fluent and others have diverse levels of
Spanish speaking abilities–since their native ancestral language is Nahua (two or three
of them answered the Social Interpretative questionnaire with the help of Francisco
Sanchez as a Nahua translator, who facilitated their understanding of the queries and
our understanding of their Nahua answers or their rudimentary "Spanish”). These fami-
lies were contacted by the SEMSOAC who had participated previously in community
action projects with Prade, a group that has developed a 30 years participant observer
research project (See Sanchez & Almeida 2005) to enhance the community’s living
conditions and preserve their cultural practices. The mothers voluntarily agreed to par-
ticipate and were later on paid a symbolic sum for the time they spent coming from out-
lying areas to the village, in order to respond to the assessments.
Procedure
The 70 Participant mothers were assessed through the Social Interpretative Question-
naire and classified according to the central character orientations proposed by Fromm
(Fromm, 1947; see also Maccoby, 1995).
Mothers’ responses to the interview were written down and typed out by interviewers.
The text interviews were then assessed on the 6 scales specifically developed by
SEMSOAC (Gojman, Millán, Sanchez & Gonzalez, 2008) for appreciating mothers’ atti-
tudes towards their lives and towards their children: (a) Sensitivity to emotional needs,
(b) Lovingness, (c) Joy of life (d) Attention to everyday survival needs of their children,
(e) Passive hopelessness and (f) Active-violent hopelessness.
The basic tendency identified from the questionnaire responses of each participant, in-
dependently of their character orientation and only to observe whether the tendency
was Productive or Unproductive was then assessed and compared between the two
groups and within each group according to socio-economic level.
Materials
A. The Social Interpretative Questionnaire
The Social Interpretative Questionnaire is an instrument developed by Fromm (1932,
page/Seite 4 of/von 16
Gojman de Millan, S., Millan, S., et al., 2013
Care Giving and Social Character
Propriety of the Erich Fromm Document Center. For personal use only. Citation or publication of
material prohibited without express written permission of the copyright holder.
Eigentum des Erich Fromm Dokumentationszentrums. Nutzung nur für persönliche Zwecke. Ver-
öffentlichungen – auch von Teilen – bedürfen der schriftlichen Erlaubnis des Rechteinhabers.
1970, 1984, Fromm & Maccoby 1970, 1996) to assess shared character traits in a
group, based on the common material conditions of daily existence. The research "is
based on the premise that not only is the energy of the individual structured in terms of
Freud`s dynamic character concept, but that there is a character structure common to
most members of groups or classes within a given society” (ibid, p. 16). It is this com-
mon character structure which Fromm has called social character. "The concept of so-
cial character does not refer to the complete or highly individualized, in fact, unique
character structure as it exists in an individual, but to a "character matrix," a syndrome
of character traits which has developed as an adaptation to the economic, social, and
cultural conditions common to that group.” (Ibid.)
"The interpretative questionnaire is a method that permits the application of psycho-
analytic categories to the study of social groups, by the minute examination of the per-
sonality of each member of the group, by the simultaneous and equally minute obser-
vation of all social economic and cultural data, and eventually, by the attempt to use re-
fined statistic methods for the analysis of the data.” (Ibid., p. 8.) It is "an analogy be-
tween a social and a personal psychoanalytic interview….in which the psychoanalyst”,
and the researcher that interviews a person, "tries to understand the unconscious
meaning of certain phrases and statements used by the patient” or the respondent of
the interview, "a meaning which he did not intend to express or is not aware of express-
ing” (p. 25).
By analyzing each answer and the totality of the answers to the questionnaire (p. 26),
Fromm’s proposed method "attempts to arrive at the knowledge of the dynamic ten-
dencies of a respondent’s character most relevant to his social and political attitudes
…a character structure found in each questionnaire with all others and with objective
data such as age, income, sex, and education”.
The main difference between this type of questionnaire, called interpretative question-
naire, and most other questionnaires used in social research is not that one is open
and one is pre-formulated, but that the answers are used in different ways. The main
effort is not to choose an adequate sample of relevant questions and toward the most
fruitful statistical elaboration of them, but to interpret the answers with regards to the
unconscious or unintended meaning. "The task of interpretation is, like any other psy-
choanalytic interpretation, difficult, and takes a great deal of time. It requires knowledge
of psychoanalytic theory and therapy (including the experience of one’s own analysis),
a clinical psychoanalytic experience, and, as in everything else, skill and talent.” (Ibid.,
p. 26.) "Uncertainty is the price the psychoanalytic researcher pays for the attempt to
arrive at a deeper understanding of the most relevant data.” (Ibid., p. 28.)
page/Seite 5 of/von 16
Gojman de Millan, S., Millan, S., et al., 2013
Care Giving and Social Character
Propriety of the Erich Fromm Document Center. For personal use only. Citation or publication of
material prohibited without express written permission of the copyright holder.
Eigentum des Erich Fromm Dokumentationszentrums. Nutzung nur für persönliche Zwecke. Ver-
öffentlichungen – auch von Teilen – bedürfen der schriftlichen Erlaubnis des Rechteinhabers.
page/Seite 6 of/von 16
Gojman de Millan, S., Millan, S., et al., 2013
Care Giving and Social Character
Propriety of the Erich Fromm Document Center. For personal use only. Citation or publication of
material prohibited without express written permission of the copyright holder.
Eigentum des Erich Fromm Dokumentationszentrums. Nutzung nur für persönliche Zwecke. Ver-
öffentlichungen – auch von Teilen – bedürfen der schriftlichen Erlaubnis des Rechteinhabers.
The elements expressed on each scale, sometimes in subtle but meaningful ways, are
identified and marked in order to arrive at point score or grade indicating the extent to
which each characteristic seems to be present in the interviewee’s treatment of her
children.
The scales have to do fundamentally with the mother, in relation to the emotional
needs of her children. The traits manifested throughout all the questionnaire responses
are identified by marking the respective passages.
With the ratings from these 5-point scales, we then attempt to determine which charac-
teristics are outstanding, how prominent or reticent they are, since many times a num-
ber of tendencies can be present simultaneously, although in different proportions and
degrees.
Each scale is rated globally on the basis of what appears throughout the questionnaire,
and on what can be detected from the responses when they are viewed as a whole.
page/Seite 7 of/von 16
Gojman de Millan, S., Millan, S., et al., 2013
Care Giving and Social Character
Propriety of the Erich Fromm Document Center. For personal use only. Citation or publication of
material prohibited without express written permission of the copyright holder.
Eigentum des Erich Fromm Dokumentationszentrums. Nutzung nur für persönliche Zwecke. Ver-
öffentlichungen – auch von Teilen – bedürfen der schriftlichen Erlaubnis des Rechteinhabers.
which results in mixed results in the scales of sensitivity and lovingness: lower points
on the lovingness scale and higher on the sensitivity scale. She tends to intellectualize.
Hoarding-unproductive. Implies a distancing from other people; any intimacy with the
outside world represents a threat this type of autarkic security system. This orientation
makes people have little faith in anything new they might get from the outside world;
their security is based upon hoarding and saving, while spending is felt to be a threat.
They surround themselves as it were by a protective wall and their main aim is to bring
as much as possible into this fortified position. Love is essentially a possession; they
do not give love but try to get it by possessing the beloved.
Exploitative Authoritarian Narcissistic. Enterprising, courageous, dynamic: this is a per-
son whose basic premise is the sense that the source of all good comes from the out;
that anything that one wants must be pursued outside of oneself, the individual can
produce nothing for him or herself. She does not, however, expect to receive goods
from others as gestures of charity, but rather by taking them away by cunning or vio-
lence. Her activities are oven colored by a mix of hostility and calculation. Traits such
as suspicion and cynicism, envy and jealousy, are foremost; which they often compen-
sate through their belief that they are always right, and must impose their will on others.
Self-developing mothers might have a productive motivation for learning, for team
work, and solving problems (Maccoby, 1995). They usually value independence, net-
working, and maintaining employability. She tends to pursue her own intellectual or
professional development, even if it causes problems for her family members or co-
workers. Narcissistic tendencies may, on the other hand be pronounced, and may
override the authentic interests of personal development.
page/Seite 8 of/von 16
Gojman de Millan, S., Millan, S., et al., 2013
Care Giving and Social Character
Propriety of the Erich Fromm Document Center. For personal use only. Citation or publication of
material prohibited without express written permission of the copyright holder.
Eigentum des Erich Fromm Dokumentationszentrums. Nutzung nur für persönliche Zwecke. Ver-
öffentlichungen – auch von Teilen – bedürfen der schriftlichen Erlaubnis des Rechteinhabers.
tions of life, by external forces. They express what they do in self-effacing terms, ab-
sent of emotional life, acting as a robot or automaton. The general character orientation
is given by the function or prioritizing the material, appearance, objects, money and
power over relationships and people.
Results
(1) Character Orientation
As can be seen in Table I, we found that the Receptive orientation was the predomi-
nant trait in both groups; in the urban group the Receptive-productive orientation pre-
vailed, and in the rural group, Receptive-unproductive.
page/Seite 9 of/von 16
Gojman de Millan, S., Millan, S., et al., 2013
Care Giving and Social Character
Propriety of the Erich Fromm Document Center. For personal use only. Citation or publication of
material prohibited without express written permission of the copyright holder.
Eigentum des Erich Fromm Dokumentationszentrums. Nutzung nur für persönliche Zwecke. Ver-
öffentlichungen – auch von Teilen – bedürfen der schriftlichen Erlaubnis des Rechteinhabers.
A. Urban Sample
Middle- Middle- Middle-
Character orientations Poor Total
lower middle upper
Receptive-productive 28% 2+ 6+ 3+ 11+
Receptive-unproductive 23% 2- 3- 4- 9-
Hoarding-productive 15% 1+ 5+ 6+
Hoarding-unproductive 10% 1- 3- 4-
Exploitative-authoritarian-narcissistic
1- 6- 7-
18%
Self-developing 5% 1+ 1+ 2+
39
Total 2 (2-) 2 (2+) 13 (8+ & 5-) 22 (9+& 13-) (19+&
20-)
Note: +Productive -Unproductive.
Chi square (Productivity-Unproductivity) not arriving to a statistical significance
In the urban group, see Table IA (N=39) the Receptive orientation was more prevalent
(in 20 cases) than the others; the most frequent variant was Receptive-productive (11
cases) followed by Receptive-unproductive (9 cases). The next most prevalent orienta-
tions were Exploitative- authoritarian-narcissistic- (7 cases), Hoarding-productive (6
cases), Hoarding-unproductive (4 cases) and Self-developing (2 cases).
Grouping them on the basis of their productivity tendencies, we found 20 were Unpro-
ductive (9 Receptive-unproductive, 4 Hoarding-unproductive and 7 Exploitative-
authoritarian); 19 Productive (11 Receptive-productive, 6 Hoarding-productive and 2
Self-developing/Self-affirming);
By socioeconomic level, we found that: the 2 Poor mothers were Unproductive, the 2
Middle-lower were Productive, the Middle-middle were more Productive than Unpro-
ductive (8-5), and the Middle-upper were more Unproductive than Productive (13-9).
The distribution did not attain the level of statistical significance.
B. Rural Sample
Middle-
Character orientation Very poor Poor Total
lower
Receptive-productive 23% 1+ 6+ 7+
Receptive-unproductive 45% 13- 1- 14-
Hoarding-productive 16% 1+ 4+ 5+
Hoarding-unproductive 6% 2- 2-
Exploitative-authoritarian-narcissistic 6% 2- 2-
page/Seite 10 of/von 16
Gojman de Millan, S., Millan, S., et al., 2013
Care Giving and Social Character
Propriety of the Erich Fromm Document Center. For personal use only. Citation or publication of
material prohibited without express written permission of the copyright holder.
Eigentum des Erich Fromm Dokumentationszentrums. Nutzung nur für persönliche Zwecke. Ver-
öffentlichungen – auch von Teilen – bedürfen der schriftlichen Erlaubnis des Rechteinhabers.
Self-developing 3% 1+ 1+
18 (1+& 8 (7+& 31 (13+&
Total 5 (5+)
17-) 1-) 18-)
Chi square =28.887 df=10 p <.001 - Note: +Productive –Unproductive
In the rural group, see Table IB (N=31) the Receptive orientation was also more preva-
lent (in 21 cases): 14 were Receptive-unproductive and 7 were Receptive-productive; 5
were Hoarding-productive, 2 were Hoarding-unproductive, 2 were Exploitative-
authoritarian and 1 was Self-developing.
Grouping them on the basis of their productivity tendencies, we found 18 Unproductive
(14 Receptive-unproductive, 2 Hoarding-unproductive and 2 Exploitative-authoritarian);
and 13 Productive (7 Receptive-productive, 5 Hoarding-productive and 1 Self-
developing/Self-affirming).
With regard to the impact of poverty on character orientation in this group, we found
that there was a difference in character orientation among the Very poor, and the Poor
and Middle-lower income mothers (see Table 1B). The first group (the Very poor), were
mostly Unproductive, and the second group mostly Productive. These differences
reached a very high level of statistical significance (Chi square =28.887 df =10 p <
.001).
Among the Very poor group the tendency toward Unproductivity was the most preva-
lent (17 cases), and there was only 1 case of Productive; among the Poor, most were
Productive (7 cases) and only 1 was Unproductive; among the Middle-lower group, all
were Productive (5 cases).
We found that mothers in the Very poor group often underwent the presence of family
or inter-generational violence and alcoholism.
(2) Scales of mothers’ attitudes toward their lives and their children and their re-
lationships to the basic Productive or Unproductive tendency.
As shown in Table II, the statistical tests exploring the relationship between the differ-
ent scales to appreciate mothers’ attitudes toward their life and toward their children—
Sensitivity toward their children’s emotional needs, Lovingness, Joy of living, Instru-
mental attention toward their children’s survival needs, Passive hopelessness and Vio-
lent hopelessness—and the basic tendency of character orientation—Productive or
Unproductive—in the urban and rural samples (N=70) were significant (Chi square var-
ied between 45.316 and 4.015 with a probability between .000 and .048). In all cases,
the observed frequencies percentage that match what is to be expected, called Ob-
served Match, (meaning that Productive mothers present high Sensitivity, Lovingness,
Joy of living and Instrumental attention to their children`s survival and low levels of
Passive and Violent hopelessness) were significantly higher than chance. This indi-
cates that the scale ratings and the basic tendency of character orientation ratings fol-
lowed the same basic lines, having been rated independently. In this sense, we can
consider them to be reliable.
page/Seite 11 of/von 16
Gojman de Millan, S., Millan, S., et al., 2013
Care Giving and Social Character
Propriety of the Erich Fromm Document Center. For personal use only. Citation or publication of
material prohibited without express written permission of the copyright holder.
Eigentum des Erich Fromm Dokumentationszentrums. Nutzung nur für persönliche Zwecke. Ver-
öffentlichungen – auch von Teilen – bedürfen der schriftlichen Erlaubnis des Rechteinhabers.
Table II. Relations between scales of mothers’ attitudes and basic tendency toward
Productivity-Unproductivity.
Lovingness Chi
Low 13 (22.9) 37 (27.1)
Observed Match=80% Square =27.407 df=1 p
Chance Expected <.000
Match=51% High 19 (9.1) 1 (10.9)
page/Seite 12 of/von 16
Gojman de Millan, S., Millan, S., et al., 2013
Care Giving and Social Character
Propriety of the Erich Fromm Document Center. For personal use only. Citation or publication of
material prohibited without express written permission of the copyright holder.
Eigentum des Erich Fromm Dokumentationszentrums. Nutzung nur für persönliche Zwecke. Ver-
öffentlichungen – auch von Teilen – bedürfen der schriftlichen Erlaubnis des Rechteinhabers.
Discussion
This study may be considered as an attempt to facilitate the appraisal of mothers’ emo-
tional states which seem to be clinically transcendent to the care giving quality they of-
fer to their infants. The assessment of the above mentioned 6, 5–point scales were
very helpful for becoming aware of when and where the diverse attitudes considered in
each of the scales were manifested throughout the responses to the questionnaires;
the scores on the scales appeared, as we have mentioned before, significantly related
to both the basic character tendency shown by mothers in the combined group and the
attachment instruments.
The Receptive character orientation was prevalent among both groups and seems to
be related to their everyday activities, dedicated basically to the care of the family and
household. Their attitudes towards these may be vital and meaningful but may also be
in other cases passive and with a pervading resignation quality.
The results show that the daily material conditions of existence were closely related to
the shared character traits, especially the basic tendency toward Productivity-
page/Seite 13 of/von 16
Gojman de Millan, S., Millan, S., et al., 2013
Care Giving and Social Character
Propriety of the Erich Fromm Document Center. For personal use only. Citation or publication of
material prohibited without express written permission of the copyright holder.
Eigentum des Erich Fromm Dokumentationszentrums. Nutzung nur für persönliche Zwecke. Ver-
öffentlichungen – auch von Teilen – bedürfen der schriftlichen Erlaubnis des Rechteinhabers.
Unproductivity in the mothers. The Very poor mothers, in the rural group, were almost
exclusively Unproductive (a finding that was proven through statistically significant re-
sults) even compared to the Poor mothers -the vast majority of whom were Productive,
and in greater contrast to the other socio-economic levels studied in this rural group. A
further careful analysis of what allowed the development of the exceptionally productive
cases under these conditions (one in this study) should follow.
On the other hand Upper-middle and Upper class mothers in the urban group were
more likely to be rated Unproductive than Middle-lower and Middle-middle income
groups, although in this case the results did not reach statistical significance. Counting
with economical resources beyond the required for fulfilling everyday life needs, seems
to us to signal here to the possibility that these may not conduct by themselves to a
better quality of life or care giving.
In fact the mothers studied in the urban group –as described in method- may have
been filtered because most of them were coming from support groups like the Breast
Feeding League or natural childbirth and early stimulation classes for their babies, thus
perhaps being women especially interested in their children’s development and thus
having specific qualities that can make them not representative of the overall urban
population.
These findings may be especially significant to us, as we found that basic tendencies
toward Productivity or Unproductivity were in turn related with the babies’ attachment
patterns, the mothers’ sensitivity in the treatment of their babies and the Adult Attach-
ment Interviews.
Although we worked with a small group and further research is necessary among dif-
ferent and broader samples, we have found that recognizing and trying to decipher the
specific characteristics of different economic everyday life conditions in diverse cul-
tures, can allow to understand the effects of them on the mothers, and how they com-
ply their central role in the emotional development of the children (see also extreme
conditions in Gojman & Millán, 2008).
Our findings speak of the suffering of the poor excluded families. Also of the non pro-
ductive tendency in Upper and Upper-middle class mothers.
The desire or conscious intention of mothers to favor their children’s development may
be generalized, but may ultimately not be achieved when raising them and thereby fac-
ing their own childhood experiences, which they may often involuntarily repeat in their
care giving, and also importantly by being affected by the social conditions of a system
that can bent them all to impulsive consumption, which divert and alienate them; some-
thing that—we believe—can only be countered to some extent by a genuine attitude in
favor of life, joy and hope.
Let us end this preliminary report by still agreeing with the 1970 outlook of Fromm and
Maccoby at the end of their study (p. 237):
"The process of industrialization, increasing alienation and hunger for commodities,
and the new values of industrial society, profoundly influence the mentality of the
peasant in spite of the fact that economically he hardly participates in the new
structure. What we find in the village, as in many peasant societies all over the
world, is the victorious march of the spirit of technological industrialism destroying
page/Seite 14 of/von 16
Gojman de Millan, S., Millan, S., et al., 2013
Care Giving and Social Character
Propriety of the Erich Fromm Document Center. For personal use only. Citation or publication of
material prohibited without express written permission of the copyright holder.
Eigentum des Erich Fromm Dokumentationszentrums. Nutzung nur für persönliche Zwecke. Ver-
öffentlichungen – auch von Teilen – bedürfen der schriftlichen Erlaubnis des Rechteinhabers.
the traditional values and replacing them with nothing except a vague longing for
the good life represented by the (dream of the) City”.
References
Ainsworth, M. D., Bell, S. M., & Stayton D. J. (1974). Infant-mother attachment and social de-
velopment: "Socialization” as a product of reciprocal responsiveness to signals. In M.P.M
Richards (Ed.), The integration of a child into a social world (pp. 99-135). London: Cam-
bridge University Press.
Ainsworth, M.D., Blehar, M.C., Waters, E., Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment. Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Associates, Inc., Publishers, Hillsdale, New Jersey.
Bowlby, J. (1969/1982). Attachment and Loss: Vol. 1 Attachment. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and Loss: Vol. 2. Separation: Anxiety and Anger. New York, NY:
Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1979). On knowing what you are not supposed to know and feeling what you are not
supposed to feel. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, Vol. 24, 5.
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and Loss: Vol. 3. Loss: Sadness and Depression. New York, NY:
Basic Books.
Freud, S. (1908). Carácter y erotismo anal. En Freud S. Obras Completas, Amorrortu Editores
Libro IX. P 149-158.
Fromm, E. (1932). Die Psychoanalytische Charakterologie und ihre Bedeutung für die Sozial-
psychologie.
Fromm, E. (1941). Escape from freedom. New York: Farrar & Rinehart.
Fromm, E. (1947). Man for himself: An inquiry into the Psychology of Ethics. Greenwich, CT:
Fawcet, New York, Rinehart and Co.
Fromm, E. (1962). Beyond the chains of illusion. My encounter with Marx and Freud. Simon &
Schuster, New York.
Fromm, E. (1970) The Crisis of psychoanalysis, essays on Freud, Marx and social psychology,
Holt, Reinhart& Winston Inc. NY.
Fromm E. (1984). The Working Class in Weimar Germany. A Psychological and Sociological
Study. Ed by Bonss. Harvard University Press. Cambridge Massachusetts, 1984.
Fromm, E. and Maccoby, M. (1970) Social character in a Mexican village. A Socio psychoana-
lytic Study. Prentice-Hall, Inc, Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey.
Fromm, E. and Maccoby, M. (1996). Social Character in a Mexican Village. Transactions Pub-
lishers. New Brunswick USA and London UK
Funk, R. (1983). Erich Fromm. Rowohlt Taschenbuch. Hamburg.
George, C., Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1985/1996). Adult Attachment Interview. (3rd Ed.). Unpub-
lished manuscript, Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley.
Gojman, S. (2004/2005, versión 7.2, agosto 2010). Sistemas de Calificación y Clasificación de
la Entrevista de Apego Adulto. Manuscrito no publicado. Semsoac México. Traducción al
español de Main, M., Goldwyn, R., & Hesse, E. (2002, versión 7.2, julio 2003). [Transla-
tion of the Adult Attachment Scoring and Classification System. Unpublished manuscript,
Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, CA].
Gojman, S. & Millán, S. (2001). Attachment patterns and social character in a Nahuatl village.
Socialization processes through social character interviews and videotaped attachment
current methodology. Fromm Forum (English Edition – ISBN 1437-1189), 05 / 2001, Tue-
bingen (Selbstverlag), pp. 38-42.
Gojman, S & Millán, S. (2003). Integrating Attachment and Social Character Approaches to
Clinical Training: Case Studies from a Mexican Nahuatl Village. Ed Cortina, M. & Mar-
rone, M. Attachment Theory and the Psychoanalytic Process, Whurr Publishers LtD, Lon-
dres, Inglaterra y Philadelphia PA USA,
Gojman, S. y Millán S. (2007). Fundamentos Biológicos y Sociales del desarrollo Emocional.
page/Seite 15 of/von 16
Gojman de Millan, S., Millan, S., et al., 2013
Care Giving and Social Character
Propriety of the Erich Fromm Document Center. For personal use only. Citation or publication of
material prohibited without express written permission of the copyright holder.
Eigentum des Erich Fromm Dokumentationszentrums. Nutzung nur für persönliche Zwecke. Ver-
öffentlichungen – auch von Teilen – bedürfen der schriftlichen Erlaubnis des Rechteinhabers.
Una Contribución Empírica derivada de la Teoría del Apego y de la del Carácter Social.
Revista Norte de Salud Mental, Vol VII nº 29 Octubre, Revista en la Red, Bilbao España.
pp 8-17.
Gojman, S. & Millán, S. (2008). The AAI and its Contribution to a Therapeutic Intervention Pro-
ject for Violent, Traumatized and Suicidal Cases. In: Steele, H. & Steele, M. Clinical Ap-
plications of the Adult Attachment Interview. The Guilford Press. Capítulo 12, pp 297-319.
Gojman, S., Millán, S., Sánchez, G., González, P., (2008). Escalas para calificar a madres o
cuidadores principales en las respuestas al cuestionario de carácter social. Manuscrito no
publicado. Registro Público SEP_INDAUTOR # 032008-120511185300-01.
Gojman, S., Millán, S., Carlson, E., Sánchez, G., Rodarte, A., & González, P., Hernández, G.
(2012). Intergenerational Relations of Attachment, A Research Synthesis of Urban/Rural
Mexican Samples. Attachment and Human Development 14 (6) pp 553-566.
Gojman, S., Millán, S., Carlson, E., González, P., Guzmán, M.E., Hernández, G., Rodarte, L. A.,
Sánchez, F., y Sánchez, G. In: B. Torres, J. Causadias y G. Posadas (Eds), Investigación
y aplicaciones clínicas de la teoría del apego en Iberoamérica. Psimática Editores.
Capítulo 7 a. - in Press.
Gojman, S., Millán, S., Sánchez, G., González, P., Rodarte, A., & Espinosa, V. (2013) Entender
y Transformar desde la perspectiva del Sociopsicoanálisis. Revista Rubricas. Universidad
Iberoamericana. Puebla. México. b. - in Press.
Hesse E. (2008). The Adult Attachment Interview: Historical and current perspectives. In: Hand-
book of Attachment. Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications. Eds. J. Cassidy & P.
Shaver. The Guilford Press. London.pp. 552-559.
Maccoby M. (1995). Why Work? Motivating the new workforce. Miles River Press, Alexandría
Virginia USA.
Main, M. (1993). Discourse prediction and recent studies in attachment: Implications for psy-
choanalysis. Journal of the America Psychoanalytic Association 41, pp 209-244.
Main,M. (1994) A move to the level of representations in the study of attachment organization:
Implications for psychoanalysis. Annual Research Lecture of the British Psychoanalytic
Society. London. July 6, 1994.
Main, M., & Goldwyn, R. (1984/1985/1996/1998). Adult Attachment Scoring and Classification
System. Unpublished Manuscripts, University of California at Berkeley.
Main, M., Goldwyn, R., & Hesse, E. (2003/2008). Adult Attachment Scoring and Classification
System, Versions 6 and 7.2. Unpublished Manuscripts, University of California at Berke-
ley.
Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1990). Procedures for identifying infants as disorganized/disoriented
during the Ainsworth strange situation. In M. T. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti & E. M. Cum-
mings (Eds.). Attachment in the preschool years (pp. 121-160). Chicago: University of
Chicago Press.
Sanchez, M.E. and Almeida, E. 2005. Las Veredas de la Incertidumbre. Relaciones
Interculturales y Supervivencia Digna Universidad Iberoamericana Puebla, Lupus
Magíster, México.
Sroufe, A. (1990). An organizational perspective on the self. In: D. Cicchetti and M. Beeghly
(eds,).The Self in Transition: Infancy to Childhood.,. Chicago: University of Chicago
Press, pp 281-307.
Sroufe A. (1997). Psychopathology as an outcome of development. Development and Psycho-
pathology #9. Cambridge University Press NY, NY. USA, pp. 251-268.
Sroufe A & Waters, E. (1977). Attachment as an organizational construct. Child Development,
48, 1184-1199.
Sroufe A, Carlson E, Levy A.K, & Egeland B (1999). Implications of attachment theory for de-
velopmental psychopathology, Development and Psychopathology Vol. 11:1, Cambridge
University Press, NY, NY. USA. pp 1-14.
page/Seite 16 of/von 16
Gojman de Millan, S., Millan, S., et al., 2013
Care Giving and Social Character