Handwriting Styles
Handwriting Styles
Handwriting Styles
50(6), 2013
C 2013 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
View this article online at wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/pits DOI: 10.1002/pits.21691
MARIE-FRANCE MORIN
University of Sherbrooke
An important issue relating to the teaching of handwriting concerns the style that should be
learned at school (manuscript or cursive). Whereas some countries choose to teach both styles
(e.g., Canada), other countries choose to teach only one (e.g., France). Our research had three main
underlying goals, namely (1) to observe and describe the handwriting styles spontaneously used by
fourth and fifth graders according to the first style learned at school; (2) to describe the evolution
of handwriting between the fourth and fifth grades; and (3) to examine the relationship between
speed, legibility, and handwriting style. The results revealed that the effects of country, grade level,
handwriting style, and handwriting instruction were significant. Quebec children wrote faster than
French children did, but their handwriting was less legible. Cursive handwriting was the slower
style, whereas mixed handwriting seemed to be more efficient. Handwriting speed and legibility
improved from fourth to fifth grade. C 2013 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Despite advances in computer technology and the availability of computers both at school
and at home, handwriting continues to take up a large proportion of time in the course of daily
school activities and, for this reason at least, handwriting is an important issue (Christensen, 2009).
Handwriting is a complex task that requires attention and memory, as well as linguistic and motor
skills (Bara & Gentaz, 2010a; Graham & Weintraub, 1996). It requires access to the motor program
for a selected letter, a decision to be made regarding the spatial arrangement of the letters on the
page, and the setting of the necessary parameters for executing the motor program (Graham, Struck,
Santoro, & Berninger, 2006). Children have to learn the specific shapes that are used in writing
(letter shape and allographic particularity) and the way these shapes are produced (direction and
trajectory of movement).
As shown by Berninger et al. (2006), handwriting is not a purely motor or visual activity; it
is “language by hand,” which shares common processes with other kinds of language (listening,
speaking and reading), but also some distinct processes that are unique to writing. Mastering
handwriting is very important for children, as it places the earliest constraints on writing development.
If children cannot form letters with a minimum of speed and legibility, they cannot translate their
ideas into written texts. In fact, given the limited capacity of working memory and the number
and complexity of writing processes, novice writers are likely to experience difficulty during the
learning process (Berninger, Whitaker, Feng, Swanson, & Abbott, 1996; Bourdin & Fayol, 2000;
Christensen, 2009; Graham, Berninger, Abbott, Abbott, & Whitaker, 1997; Graham, Harris, & Fink,
2000; Jones & Christensen, 1999; Olive, Favart, Beauvais, & Beauvais, 2009; Olive & Kellogg,
2002; Torrance & Galbraith, 2005). The need to switch attention from the composition process to
the mechanical demands of handwriting may result in a writer forgetting his or her ideas for the text.
It is therefore not surprising that handwriting quality affects academic performance and that there is
a strong relationship between transcription and text generation.
Correspondence to: Florence Bara, CREAD, IUFM Bretagne, 8 rue d’Avranches, 29200 Brest, France. E-mail:
[email protected]
601
602 Bara and Morin
More precisely, individual differences in handwriting skills predict how much and how well
children write (Berninger et al., 1997; Jones & Christensen, 1999). Basic measures of handwriting
correlate with and predict compositional fluency (Berninger et al., 1996; Berninger & Fuller, 1992;
Berninger & Swanson, 1994) but also compositional quality (Berninger & Swanson, 1994) for both
primary and middle school students (Berninger et al., 1997). Automaticity, as defined by the ability
to recall information from memory quickly, accurately, and effortlessly, is really important in order
not to exceed the limited resources in terms of working memory. Thus, it is essential that producing
letters becomes automatic and that information about letter shape and trajectory of movement can be
retrieved without consuming attention. Research conducted by Medwell, Strand, and Wray (2009)
at the end of primary school in the United Kingdom suggests that handwriting, specifically the
ability to generate letters automatically, has an important role in text composition. As handwriting
skills become more automatic, attention and cognitive resources for carrying out other learning
processes become available (McCutchen, 2011). Throughout the school day, children have to take
notes, copy, produce written texts, and perform written exams; all these tasks rely on handwriting
ability. Therefore, handwriting strongly impacts academic performance. In Graham et al.’s (2008)
study, only 46% of teachers reported that their students’ handwriting was fast enough to keep up with
classroom demands. Thus, handwriting teaching has to focus on better ways to improve handwriting
automaticity, and this is linked with the choice of handwriting style that should be taught.
H ANDWRITING D EVELOPMENT
The acquisition of the motor programs for producing letters is gradual and develops throughout
elementary school. Handwriting quality improves greatly during the first year of learning and much
more slowly thereafter (Karsldotir & Stefansson, 2003). Mojet (1991) showed that children made a
dramatic improvement from Grades 2 to 3, followed by a stagnation in Grade 4 and then by a steady
improvement from Grades 5 to 6. In two longitudinal studies, Blöte and Hamstra-Bletz (1991) and
Hamstra-Bletz and Blöte (1990) described the different stages of handwriting development from
Grades 2 to 6. From Grades 2 to 3, they observed improved skills in making the fine movements
required to write; letter size decreased, word and letter alignment improved, the written line became
steady, and links were smoother. Handwriting changed after Grade 4, with the shapes of letters
deteriorating, making for ambiguous letter shapes. Letter writing speed increases in a linear manner
over the primary school years (Graham et al., 1994; Hamstra-Bletz & Blöte, 1990; Karlsdottir &
Stefansson, 2003; Sassoon, 1986; Ziviani & Elkins, 1984). The development of handwriting is based
on a gradual switch from a feedback to a feed-forward control of movement (Meulenbroek & van
Galen, 1988). This change may occur at around the age of 10. However, the production of movement
is not yet automatic at this age, and the lack of visual feedback leads to many modifications to the
movement (Chartrel & Vinter, 2006).
L ETTER P RODUCTION
Letter production can be achieved in different ways, using a pen or a keyboard, and using
different styles of handwriting. Even though typing on a keyboard has become a widely used
mode of transcription, handwriting is often commonly used and results in faster word production
in elementary school children (Berninger, Abbott, Augsburger, & Garcia, 2009; Connelly, Gee, &
Walsh, 2007). With regard to handwriting, different styles can be taught and used (most commonly
manuscript and cursive). The motor output process is influenced by allographic variability when
children have to choose which letter is going to be produced. Motor planning and motor execution
are different for manuscript and for cursive handwriting (Meulenbroek & van Galen, 1990). In
manuscript writing, letters are disconnected and correspond to letter shapes classically encountered
in printed books. Most of the manuscript letters are formed by a single continuous stroke or by two
or more basic strokes (lines, circles, parts of circles, etc.).
On the contrary, cursive style is characterized by joined letters, continuous movement, and few
pen lifts. Although this is considered to improve writing fluency, it is more demanding in terms of
fine motor coordination (Paoletti, 1999). One of the most fundamental issues in teaching handwriting
involves the style that should be taught at school. This point is a contested issue, and the style used
depends on the country (Ediger, 2002). For example, in the United States and Canada, manuscript
writing is generally learned first and cursive is taught in second or third grade. Despite the general
practice of teaching both forms of writing, some educators recommend that only manuscript be
taught or that cursive be taught from the start.
A common variation in the United States is to teach slanted manuscript letters that more
closely resemble their cursive counterparts than the traditional manuscript alphabet. In France,
cursive writing is learned as of the first year of formal education. Before entering elementary school,
children learn to master the movements required to write, learn the strokes that form the letters, and
begin to write cursive letters and words. In the first years of elementary school, specific handwriting
activities are introduced, and children practice cursive handwriting. Letter shapes, both printed and
cursive, are introduced simultaneously, but only cursive letters have to be written. After Grade 4, they
stop performing specific handwriting exercises, but an emphasis is already placed on handwriting
quality (French primary school syllabus, 2008).
In Quebec, the official specifications regarding the teaching of handwriting are less specific
(Ministry of Education Quebec, 2001). In kindergarten, no specific indications are given regarding
the nature of the graphic activities that can develop handwriting skills. In primary school, legibility,
spacing between letters and words, and fluency of movement are identified as important components
of handwriting. No clear recommendations exist regarding the use of the manuscript or cursive
handwriting, and official texts state that “depending on the situation, pupils write in manuscript or
in cursive style so that their texts can be read easily” (Ministry of Education Quebec, 2001). A
clear tendency can be observed in schools, where pupils learn manuscript writing in first grade, both
in reading and writing, and are taught cursive handwriting the following year. Some studies have
tried to present arguments in favor of one handwriting instruction method or the other (Karlsdottir,
1996a). These arguments have focused on the complexity of the motor act, the speed of handwriting,
and the link between reading and writing.
rotational direction for producing loops and circles (Goodnow, Friedman, Bernbaum, & Lehman,
1973; Meulenbroek, Vinter, & Mounoud, 1993). The counterclockwise direction of cursive letters is
not spontaneously used by young children because it imposes strong motor constraints. As a result,
the changes in sequencing strategy could lead to difficulty in handwriting acquisition.
In manuscript writing, the direction of movement is less important than in cursive because
letters are not joined. Production rules in manuscript writing are therefore closer to drawing, and
children can more easily master the trajectory of these letters. However, even though, manuscript
seems to be easier in terms of motor execution, this is not the case for the perceptual task of letter
recognition. In manuscript writing, the simpler shapes and the high number of mirror letters make
the differentiation between letters more difficult than in cursive (Paoletti, 1999).
With regard to the speed of writing, it has been reported that children wrote manuscript
faster than cursive (Gray, 1956; Houston, 1938; Turner, 1930). These results are congruent with
Meulenbroek and Van Galen’s (1986) study, which showed that children copied patterns with
clockwise and counterclockwise movements faster than patterns alternating the rotational direction
of movements (which is the case in cursive writing because of the joined letters). However, these
results were not reproduced in all studies, and it was shown that practice, more than handwriting
style, influences handwriting speed (Graham, Berninger, & Weintraub, 1998; Laszlo & Broderick,
1991). Learning manuscript writing prior to cursive is considered to improve the development of
quality and speed in subsequent cursive writing (Leung, Treblas, Cooper, & Porter, 1982; Otto
& Rarick, 1969). However, Armitage and Ratzlaff (1985) did not find any significant correlation
between the quality of manuscript and later cursive writing and concluded that teaching manuscript
as the first style did not influence the quality of cursive handwriting. Poor printers do not necessarily
become poor cursive writers, and the changeover from manuscript to cursive writing might slow
down the development of subsequent cursive handwriting (Herrick, 1960).
Another argument in favor of the teaching of manuscript writing in first grade concerns the
link between reading and writing. Manuscript letters should be better produced and recognized than
cursive letters because they look more like the typeset letters found in books. Using the same letters
in reading and writing should make these acquisitions easier (Myers, 1983). The deviation between
the allographic shape of letters in books (manuscript) and the shape of letters taught at school in
writing (cursive style) would lead to a cognitive overload that would damage the effective treatment
of the written characters. However, the results of Bara and Morin’s (2009) study did not bring to
light any difficulties in reading caused by the learning of cursive handwriting.
The debate over the type of writing style to be taught in school has led scholars to suggest
alternative styles of writing, such as disjointed cursive letters, slanted alphabet, and italic or modern
cursive. In Australia, for example, modern cursive was introduced to facilitate the transition from
manuscript to cursive. The style was oval, not circular; the lines were slanted, not vertical; and
links were introduced in order for it to more closely resemble cursive handwriting (Ziviani, 1998).
According to Karlsdottir (1996b, 1996c), the difficulty of cursive handwriting seems to stem more
from the links between the letters than from the complexity of letter shapes. She compared two
ways of teaching handwriting to first-grade children, namely, manuscript and disjointed cursive. In
second grade, both groups of children learned to write in cursive. The results showed no differences
between the two groups at any grade level with regard to handwriting quality. For 75% of letters,
manuscript and cursive letter shapes were of comparable difficulty.
In the same way, the results of a study in Grade 2 that observed the effects of different styles
on writing performances (Morin, Lavoie, & Montésinos-Gelet, 2012) showed this same non-effect
for quality in three different groups (cursive, manuscript, and manuscript-cursive), even if this study
reported results that suggest an effect of writing style on speed. As Graham et al. (1998) underlined,
one issue that was not often addressed in the debate on the relative efficiency of manuscript and cursive
relates to the fact that many children used mixed handwriting. They showed that the handwriting of
pupils who used a mixed style was faster than the handwriting of pupils who used either manuscript
or cursive exclusively. It was assumed that children who mixed manuscript and cursive selected the
allographic shape that they were able to retrieve and execute more efficiently.
The study by Berninger and colleagues (2006) investigated the developmental trajectories in
producing manuscript, cursive, and keyboard letters and the predictors of these alternative modes
of letter production. The results showed that cursive writing was less accurate and slower than
manuscript writing and keyboarding. Manuscript writing, cursive writing, and keyboarding were only
moderately correlated and each had a different set of predictors. In first and third grades, orthographic
coding, phonemic skills, rapid naming, finger succession, inhibition, and inhibition/switching cor-
related and contributed to various factors in manuscript handwriting (automaticity, accuracy, speed,
etc.). In fifth grade, orthographic coding appeared to be the only contributor. For cursive handwriting,
both finger succession and phonemic skills contributed in third grade. In fifth grade, inhibition and
orthographic coding were the only contributing factors.
assume that for French children, cursive should be more legible than mixed handwriting because
children were trained to produce cursive letters and not manuscript letters in first grade. For Quebec
children, there should not be any differences between the different styles of handwriting because the
tracing of letters was taught in the different styles of handwriting.
S TUDY 1
Method
Participants. The participants of the study were 236 pupils. About half of them were attending
schools in Quebec (58 in Grade 4 and 69 in Grade 5), and the other half were attending schools
in France (50 in Grade 4 and 59 in Grade 5). Among the pupils in Grade 4, there were 57 boys
and 51 girls, and among the pupils in Grade 5, there were 59 boys and 65 girls. All pupils were
right-handed and had developed normally without any learning difficulties. Some information about
how teachers view and teach handwriting in each school was collected in a previous study (Bara,
Morin, Montésinos-Gelet, & Lavoie, 2011). First and second grade teachers in each school were
interviewed on the way they see handwriting instruction and on the way they teach it. The questions
asked were about the importance of teaching handwriting; the style of handwriting that they feel
should be taught; the time devoted each day to handwriting teaching and practice; the teaching
aids and mediums used to teach handwriting; and their demands concerning speed and legibility of
handwriting in writing productions.
Materials and Procedure. The children were given 5 minutes to copy the text presented in
the concise evaluation scale for handwriting (BHK) developed by Hamstra-Bletz, DeBie, and Den
Brinker (1987) and adapted in France by Charles, Soppelsa, and Albaret (2003). They were asked
to copy the text in their “usual handwriting” as quickly as possible while avoiding mistakes and in
legible handwriting. The text was printed on the top of a white sheet of paper, and the pupils had to
copy it below. Data were collected simultaneously in the classrooms. Handwriting style, speed, and
legibility were measured and analyzed.
Handwriting Style. Each text was examined to determine the style of handwriting. Samples
were classified as manuscript (all letters manuscript), cursive (all letters cursive), mixed–mostly
manuscript (50% or more letters manuscript), and mixed–mostly cursive (50% or more letters
cursive).
Handwriting Speed. The number of letters handwritten was counted for each writing sample.
Handwriting speed was calculated based on the mean number of letters written per minute.
Handwriting Legibility. Handwriting quality was assessed by means of the BHK analytical
scale. The first five sentences of the copied text were used to assess the quality of the handwriting on
a 13-point scale. The points assessed were related to the spatial characteristics of the writing (spacing
between letters and words, alignment of letters, etc.) and letter formation (size, shape, consistency
of letter size, relative height of the various kinds of letters, etc.). The higher the BHK score was, the
less legible was the writing.
Results
Handwriting Style. The percentage of pupils using each style of handwriting is reported in
Table 1. In Grade 4, 86% of French pupils used cursive handwriting. Only 14% used mixed–mostly
cursive handwriting. None of them wrote exclusively in manuscript. About half of Quebec pupils
used manuscript handwriting only (55.2%). It is interesting to note that only 10.4% of Quebec
children chose to use cursive handwriting (even though this style was explicitly taught in second
Table 1
Percentage of Children in Grade 4 and Grade 5 Using Each Style of Handwriting According to the Style Learned
in First Grade
Grade Handwriting %
4
Manuscript as the First Style Learned (Quebec)
Manuscript 55.2
Cursive 10.4
Mixed, mostly cursive 15.5
Mixed, mostly manuscript 18.9
Cursive as the First Style Learned (France)
Manuscript 0
Cursive 86
Mixed, mostly cursive 14
Mixed, mostly manuscript 0
5
Manuscript as the First Style Learned (Quebec)
Manuscript 62.3
Cursive 7.2
Mixed, mostly cursive 1.5
Mixed, mostly manuscript 29
Cursive as the First Style Learned (France)
Manuscript 0
Cursive 59.3
Mixed, mostly cursive 35.6
Mixed, mostly manuscript 5.1
and third grade). In Grade 5, about 40% of French pupils used mixed handwriting (mainly mixed–
mostly cursive). The majority of Quebec pupils used manuscript only (62.3%), mixed handwriting
was essentially mixed–mostly manuscript, and cursive accounted for only 7.2%. Quantitative and
qualitative analyses were carried out to better understand mixed handwriting. A ratio was established
to determine the proportion of cursive to manuscript letters in mixed handwriting. For mixed–mostly
cursive writing, the ratio was the number of letters written in manuscript to the total number
of letters handwritten. For mixed–mostly manuscript writing, the ratio was the number of letters
written in cursive to the total number of letters. Student t tests revealed that there were no significant
differences between French and Quebec children and there were no differences between fourth
and fifth graders. We noticed that mixed–mostly cursive and mixed–mostly manuscript handwriting
styles were qualitatively different. In mixed–mostly manuscript, whole words and letter clusters
(mostly syllables) were written in cursive style. In mixed–mostly cursive, some letters were in
manuscript at the beginning of words or inside words written in cursive style. This concerned a few
letters that were frequently written in manuscript (mostly p, t, d, v, and i for French children and b,
i and l for Quebec children).
For the analyses relating to handwriting speed and legibility, we took into account the following
three between-subjects variables:
1. The country: Quebec and France, which differed in terms of the instructional methods, used
a single style being practiced during the primary school years in France and two styles
(manuscript and cursive) being formally taught in Quebec.
Table 2
Mean Number of Letters Copied per Minute in Each Handwriting Style by Fourth and Fifth Graders
Grade 4 Grade 5
Table 3
Mean Scores in the BHK (Text Legibility) in Fourth and Fifth Graders and According to the Style Used
Grade 4 Grade 5
Handwriting Speed. The mean number and standard deviation of letters copied per minute
are presented in Table 2. An analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed with Country
(France/Quebec), Grade level (fourth grade/fifth grade) and Handwriting style (style learned in first
grade/mixed handwriting) as the between-subjects variables. The main effect of Country, F(1,217) =
12.11 p < .01, and the main effect of Grade level, F(1,217) = 42.67, p < .01, were significant. Que-
bec pupils wrote faster (M = 52.86) than French pupils (M = 46.85), and handwriting was faster in
fifth grade (M = 54.02) than in fourth grade (M = 44.81). The Grade level × Handwriting style was
significant, F(1,217) = 7.64, p < .01. Post-hoc analyses (with HSD [honestly significant difference]
test for N different) were performed. In fourth grade, there were no significant differences between
the use of the style learned in first grade and mixed handwriting. In fifth grade, mixed handwriting
was faster than the style learned in first grade (cursive for French pupils and manuscript for Quebec
pupils).
Handwriting Legibility. Mean overall scores and (standard deviation) in the BHK are reported
in Table 3. A 2 (Country) × 2 (Grade level) × 2 (Handwriting style) ANOVA was performed on the
overall quality of handwriting (overall BHK scores). The main effect of Country, F(1,217) = 53.46,
p < .01, and the main effect of Grade level, F(1,217) = 5.85, p < .05, were significant. The overall
legibility was higher for French pupils (M = 7.84) than for Quebec pupils (M = 13.46). The overall
legibility of handwriting improved with grade level (Mfourth grade = 12.13; Mfifth grade = 9.93). The
interactions were not significant.
We performed ANOVAs on each of the BHK items (which determine handwriting legibility).
The texts copied by French pupils consisted of smaller letters, F(1,217) = 26.38, p < .01; the
alignment of letters was better, F(1,217) = 18.39, p < .01; the trace was more regular, F(1,217) =
2.49, p < .05; and the consistency of letter size, F(1,217) = 10.70, p < .01, the relative height of
the various kind of letters, F(1,217) = 31.15, p < .01, and the quality of letter forms, F(1,217) =
22.95 p < .01, were higher than in the texts copied by Quebec pupils. The main effect of Grade
level was significant and showed improvement from Grade 4 to Grade 5 in terms of the letter size,
F(1,217) = 8.76, p < .01, the consistency of letter size, F(1,217) = 10.70, p < .01, and the quality
of letter shapes, F(1,217) = 12.07, p < .01. An interaction between Country and Handwriting style
was found in terms of the consistency of letter size, F(1,217) = 9.58, p < .01, the relative height
of letters, F(1,217) = 5.59, p < .05, and the steadiness of the trace, F(1,217) = 5.18, p < .05. The
difference between Quebec and France with regard to handwriting quality was more significant for
the first style learned (cursive in France and manuscript in Quebec) than for mixed handwriting.
Discussion
This study assessed the effect of the handwriting style learned in first grade on the style used
in fourth and fifth grades and on the speed and legibility of handwriting. We compared handwriting
in French pupils, who learned cursive in first grade, with the handwriting of Quebec pupils, who
learned manuscript in first grade and cursive in second grade. Generally speaking, the first style
learned at school was used by the majority of pupils in fourth grade. In French children, handwriting
was not really personalized, and the majority of children used cursive handwriting. In fifth grade,
mixed handwriting was more commonly used. However, there was a large prevalence of the first style
learned at school. French children primarily used mixed–mostly cursive writing, whereas Quebec
children primarily used mixed–mostly manuscript writing. Cursive handwriting was not used much
by Quebec children, even though it was explicitly taught in the second and third grades. This finding
addresses the question of the relevance of this supplementary learning at primary school.
We assumed that because French children were not taught manuscript, the proportion of
manuscript letters in mixed handwriting should be smaller than the proportion of cursive letters
in mixed handwriting for Quebec children. This was not the case; the proportion was not different
between French and Quebec children. However, there was a difference between writing strategies
with regard to mixed–mostly cursive and mixed–mostly manuscript writing. When the largest part
of the text was written in cursive style (which was always the case for French children), only a few
isolated letters were written in manuscript. This concerned a small set of letters (p, d, t, v, and i),
which were produced several times in manuscript in the texts. When the largest part of the text was
written in manuscript, whole syllables or words were written in cursive style. It could be assumed
that the links between letters that characterize cursive style forced the children to write several letters
in cursive at a time. We can also assume that because French children have not learned to write in
manuscript, they were just able to memorize a small set of letters they had encountered in books.
We can assume, too, that they have just selected some letters that are easier to produce with regard
to the motor act. In fact, the letters p and v are less complex in manuscript than in cursive, which
justifies a move from cursive to manuscript. However, the letters t, d, and i seem to be of the same
difficulty in both cursive and manuscript. The reasons that prompted these changes in handwriting
style should be the subject of further in-depth investigation.
The aim of this study was also to assess the changes in handwriting between the fourth and
fifth grades and the handwriting speed and legibility in French and Quebec pupils. Handwriting
speed and quality improved from fourth to fifth grade. This improvement concerned the letter size,
the consistency of the letters, and the quality of letter shapes. Whatever the style used (mixed
handwriting or manuscript), Quebec children wrote faster than French children. This seems to relate
more to a cultural difference in instructional methods than to a particularity of the style used. With
regard to the comparison between mixed handwriting and the use of cursive style or manuscript
exclusively, the results were significant only in Grade 5. In Grade 4, pupils who wrote in mixed
style did not write faster than pupils who used the first style they learned at school (cursive for
French pupils and manuscript for Quebec pupils). In Grade 5, mixed handwriting became faster
than cursive or manuscript. Previous research, which was conducted with children who were taught
both styles of handwriting during primary school, showed that mixed handwriting was faster than
the exclusive use of cursive or manuscript (Graham et al., 1998). If we consider that children wrote
faster in mixed style because they selected the allograph that was the most available when they
were writing, the pattern of results should be a little different for French children, who were not
taught both handwriting styles. For French children, mixed style should correspond to a search
for personalization of handwriting and originality, but not necessarily to a search for efficiency.
However, even for French children, mixed handwriting was faster than cursive.
With regard to handwriting quality, country and grade level also had an effect. Texts written
by French children were more legible than were texts written by Quebec children. The elements
that made the difference were the size and the alignment of the letters, the regularity of the trace,
the consistency of letter size, the relative height of the various kinds of letters, and the quality of
letter shapes. These differences may be due to differences in instructional methods in France and
Quebec. The study by Bara, Morin, Montésinos-Gelet, and Lavoie (2011) highlighted differences in
handwriting teaching and demands between French and Quebec teachers. It is important to note that
the differences between Quebec and French children essentially concerned texts written in the style
initially learned at school. With regard to mixed handwriting, there were fewer differences between
the handwriting quality of French and Quebec children.
One issue that could not be addressed in our study pertained to the relative merits of cursive
and manuscript styles. Indeed, one of the main limitations relates to the fact that two conditions
(learning of cursive or manuscript first at school) were intertwined with the country. We have to
take into account that there are cultural differences in the way handwriting is taught in France and
Quebec. To carry on this research and address this question, we assessed handwriting style, speed,
and legibility in Quebec pupils who had learned to write in cursive from the first grade. We found
two classrooms in Quebec where cursive was taught as of the first grade. The aim of the second
study was to compare manuscript and cursive handwriting independently of the culture.
S TUDY 2
Method
Participants. The participants in this study were 96 pupils schooled in the fifth grade in
Quebec. Of these, 48 (25 girls and 23 boys) were taught cursive in first grade and 48 (22 girls and
26 boys) were taught manuscript in first grade and cursive in second grade. We tried to control the
instructional methods between the classes by interviewing teachers in first and second grade (Bara
et al., 2011). We chose classes where practices were as similar as possible. In each school, when
teaching handwriting, first- and second-grade teachers reported spending the same amount of time
in handwriting teaching, using the same teaching aids, and having the same demands on legibility
in writing production.
Table 4
Mean Number of Letters Copied per Minute According to Handwriting Style and Instruction
Handwriting Style
Handwriting Instruction Manuscript Mean (SD) Cursive Mean (SD) Mixed Mean (SD)
Table 5
Mean Legibility Scores (BHK) According to Handwriting Style and Instruction
Handwriting Style
Handwriting Instruction Manuscript Mean (SD) Cursive Mean (SD) Mixed Mean (SD)
Results
Handwriting Style. Among Quebec pupils who were taught cursive style in first grade, 19
pupils wrote in cursive, 16 pupils wrote in manuscript, and 13 pupils used mixed handwriting.
Among Quebec pupils who were taught manuscript in first grade and cursive in second grade, 24
wrote in manuscript, 19 wrote in mixed handwriting, and 5 wrote in cursive.
Handwriting Speed. The mean number (and standard deviation) of letters copied per minute
is reported in Table 4. An ANOVA with Handwriting instruction (cursive in first grade/manuscript
in first grade) and Handwriting style (cursive, manuscript, and mixed handwriting) as the between-
subjects variables was carried out. The main effect of Handwriting style, F(2.90) = 8.37, p < .01,
was significant. Post hoc analyses (with HSD test for N different) showed that mixed and manuscript
handwriting were faster than cursive handwriting. There were no significant differences between
manuscript and mixed handwriting.
Handwriting Legibility. The mean overall scores (with standard deviations) in the BHK are
reported in Table 5. A 2 (Handwriting instruction) × 3 (Handwriting style) ANOVA on the overall
score in the BHK was carried out. There were no significant effects.
ANOVAs on each of the BHK items showed that there was a main effect of Handwriting style
on the size of the letters, F(2.90) = 5.66, p < .01, the steadiness of the trace, F(2.90) = 7.4, p < .01,
and the consistency of letter size, F(1.90) = 6.36, p < .01. In cursive and mixed handwriting, letters
were smaller and the consistency of letter size was greater than in manuscript. However, the trace
was less unsteady in manuscript and mixed handwriting than in cursive. One of the main effects
of Handwriting instruction was on the relative height of the letters, F(1.90) = 6.23, p < .05, the
alignment of the letters, F(1.90) = 9.37, p < .01, and the quality of letter shapes, F(1.90) = 4.52,
p < 05. Pupils who learned to write in cursive in first grade produced letters of a higher quality with
a less unsteady trace and with a better alignment on the page, regardless of the style used.
G ENERAL D ISCUSSION
Handwriting teaching is more important than was initially assumed, and this component of
writing is more than a matter of presentation. It has an important impact on academic performance
because a large number of school activities rely on handwriting ability (Sassoon, 1990). In fact,
children spend 31% to 60% of their school day performing handwriting and other fine motor tasks
(McHale & Cermak, 1992). If the motor production of letters is not automatic, activities that rely
on handwriting cannot be performed with ease and quality because of cognitive overload. Thus, an
important issue in educational research is how to help children to automate their handwriting and
which style of handwriting should be taught to improve handwriting speed and legibility.
The aims of these studies were to (1) describe the styles spontaneously used by pupils according
to the handwriting style learned in first grade at school, (2) examine the changes in handwriting
between fourth and fifth grade, and (3) assess the relative merits of cursive, manuscript, and mixed
handwriting in terms of speed and legibility. We investigated the relationship between handwriting
style, speed, and legibility in French and Quebec pupils who were taught either cursive or manuscript
style in first grade. The differences in handwriting instruction between Quebec and France are
interesting and can provide answers to the educational issue of which style of handwriting should
be taught. Handwriting was examined in fourth- and fifth-grade pupils because it was shown that
handwriting tends to be personalized after Grade 4 and that children’s style often deviates from the
one they were initially taught (Blöte & Hamstra-Bletz, 1991; Hamstra-Bletz & Blöte, 1990).
of these particular letters. The reasons may reside in the complexity of the motor act for producing
letters and in the frequency of these letters in printed books.
In Study 2, it is surprising that a large proportion of Quebec children used manuscript exclu-
sively, even though they were not taught this style. It could be that this is a cultural particularity. In
fact, even though they were taught cursive at school, this is an unusual practice in Quebec, and these
children may have been used to writing in manuscript at home and seeing their parents and friends
writing in manuscript. Thus, we can assume that they were more exposed to manuscript in writing
than were French children.
improves (Hamstra-Bletz & Blöte, 1990). Manuscript handwriting should bypass the problem of
the changes in sequencing strategy involved in cursive style because it is produced by using the
drawing production rules and because the direction of movement is less significant than in cursive
(Meulenbroek et al., 1993). Finally, the simplicity of letter shapes and the numerous pen lifts should
help to improve handwriting speed (Meulenbroek & van Galen, 1986).
Manuscript was superior to cursive in terms of handwriting speed but not overall legibility. In
fifth grade, handwriting quality was higher in pupils who first learned cursive than in pupils who
first learned manuscript. Moreover, the quality of letter shapes was higher in cursive and mixed
handwriting than in manuscript. Cursive was superior to manuscript in terms of the size of the
letters, the consistency of letter size and the relative height of the various kinds of letters. This
result challenges the observation that manuscript is more compatible with the motor skills of young
pupils and that the simpler, more stable shapes of the letters facilitate handwriting in terms of either
speed or legibility. However, the quality of letter shapes in handwriting relies as much on visual
perceptual abilities as on motor skills (Bara & Gentaz, 2010b; Berninger et al., 1997; Jongmans,
Linthorst-Bakker, Westenberg, Smits-Engelsman, & Bouwien, 2003; Karlsdottir, 1996a; Karlsdottir
& Stefansson, 2003; Vinter & Chartrel, 2010; Volman, van Schendel, & Jongmans, 2006; Weintraub
& Graham, 2000).
In manuscript, even though the motor task is easier, this is not the case for the perceptual task.
The high number of mirror letters and the simpler shapes might impair writing quality by creating
interference between closely related shapes. The search for speed in writing might also reduce
writing legibility. However, in some cases, mixed handwriting was as fast as it was legible. The
results showed the effect of handwriting instruction. Handwriting quality in pupils who were taught
both styles was poorer than handwriting quality in pupils who were taught a single style (French and
Quebec). There is a need to investigate whether there are differences in instruction when a single
rather than two styles are taught in school.
C ONCLUSION
The question about the handwriting style that should be taught at school is an important
educational issue that has been poorly investigated in recent research. Our results revealed that
country, instructional method, and style of handwriting all had an effect on handwriting speed
and legibility. Cursive handwriting was the slowest style but was more legible than manuscript.
The more efficient style of writing seems to be mixed handwriting because it was as fast, if not
faster, than manuscript and was similar to cursive handwriting in terms of legibility. Examining the
development of fluency in handwriting is of particular importance because it is strongly linked to
academic achievement because it is necessary for most daily school activities. It is also important
in pupils’ ability to produce long and coherent texts (Berninger et al., 1996; Berninger et al., 1997;
Berninger & Fuller, 1992; Berninger & Swanson, 1994), and measures of handwriting fluency
accounted for 25% to 42% of the variance in the writing quality (Graham et al., 1997).
Whatever the handwriting style taught initially, children grow more and more distant from this
style and use more and more mixed handwriting. These changes in handwriting style might reflect
a search for more efficiency and fluency. Making this choice, we can suppose that children tend
to reduce the cognitive load generated by the motor production of letters by accessing the easiest
allographic shape in memory. If this is the case, children who use mixed handwriting should produce
texts of better quality. This link between the style of handwriting and compositional quality needs to
be investigated in future research. Children inevitably develop their own style and combine letters
from different scripts. They are able to learn letters either from motor or from visual experience,
through print exposure. Thus, it is difficult to provide advice concerning the handwriting style that
should be taught at school. Maybe we can suggest to teachers not to insist on a strict adherence to a
particular model so as not to reduce the automaticity in motor production necessary for written text
production.
R EFERENCES
Armitage, D., & Ratzlaff, H. (1985). The non-correlation of printing and writing skills. Journal of Educational Research, 78,
174 – 177.
Bara, F., & Gentaz, E. (2010a). Apprendre à tracer les lettres: Une revue critique. Psychologie Française, 55, 129 – 144.
Bara, F., & Gentaz, E. (2010b). Haptics in teaching handwriting: the role of perceptual and visuo-motor skills. Human
Movement Science, 30, 745 – 759.
Bara, F., & Morin, M.-F. (2009). Est-il nécessaire d’enseigner l’écriture script en première année? Les effets du style d’écriture
sur le lien lecture/écriture. Nouveaux Cahiers de la Recherche en Education, 12, 149 – 160.
Bara, F., Morin, M.-F., Montésinos-Gelet, I., & Lavoie, N. (2011). Conceptions et pratiques en graphomotricité chez des
enseignants de primaire en France et au Québec. Revue Française de Pédagogie, 176, 41 – 56.
Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R. D., Augsburger, A., & Garcia, N. (2009). Comparison of pen and keyboard transcription modes
in children with and without learning disabilities. Learning Disabilities Quarterly, 32, 123 – 141.
Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R. D., Jones, J., Wolf, B. J., Gould, L., Anderson, M., Shimada, S., & Apel, K. (2006). Early
development of language by hand: Composing, reading, listening, and speaking connections; three letter-writing modes;
and fast mapping in spelling. Developmental Neuropsychology, 29, 61 – 92.
Berninger, V. W., & Fuller, F. (1992). Gender differences in orthographic, verbal, and compositional fluency: Implications
for assessing writing disabilities in primary grade children. Journal of School Psychology, 30, 363 – 382.
Berninger, V. W., Graham, S., Vaughan, K. B., Abbott, R. D., Abbott, S. P., Woodruff Rogan, L. W., Brooks, A., Reed, E., &
Graham, S. (1997). Treatment of handwriting problems in beginning writers: Transfer from handwriting to composition.
Journal of Educational Research, 89, 652 – 666.
Berninger, V. W., Richards, T., Stock, P., Abbott, R. D., Trivedi, P., Altemeier, L., & Hayes, J. R. (2009). fMRI activation
related to nature of ideas generated and differences between good and poor writers during idea generation. British
Journal of Educational Psychology Monograph, Series II, 6, 77 – 93.
Berninger, V. W., & Swanson, H. L. (1994). Modification of the Hayes and Flower model to explain beginning and developing
writing. In E. Butterfield (Ed.), Advances in cognition and educational practice. Children’s writing: Toward a process
theory of development of skilled writing: Vol. 2. (pp. 57 – 82). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Berninger, V. W., Whitaker, D., Feng, Y., Swanson, H. L., & Abbott, R. D. (1996). Assessment of planning, translating and
revising in juniors high writers. Journal of School Psychology, 34, 23 – 52.
Blöte, A. W., & Hamstra-Bletz, L. (1991). A longitudinal study on the structure of handwriting. Perceptual and Motor Skills,
72, 983 – 994.
Bourdin, B., & Fayol, M. (2000). Is graphic activity cognitively costly? A developmental approach. Reading and Writing:
An interdisciplinary Journal, 13, 183 – 196.
Chao, L. L., & Martin, A. (2000). Representation of manipulable man-made objects in the dorsal stream. Neuroimage, 12,
478 – 484.
Charles, M., Soppelsa, R., & Albaret, J.-M. (2003). BHK – Echelle d’évaluation rapide de l’écriture chez l’enfant. Paris,
France: Editions et Applications Psychologiques.
Chartrel, E., & Vinter, A. (2006). Rôle des informations visuelles dans la production de lettres cursives chez l’enfant et
l’adulte. L’Année Psychologique, 106, 45 – 66.
Christensen, C. A. (2009). The critical role handwriting plays in the ability to produce high-quality written text. In R. Beard,
D. Myhill, J. Riley, & M. Nystrand (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of writing development. London, UK: Sage.
Connelly, V., Gee, D., & Walsh, E. (2007). A comparison of keyboarded and written compositions and the relationship with
transcription speed. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 479 – 492.
Duval, B. (1985). Evaluating the difficulty of four handwriting styles used for instruction. Spectrum, 3, 13 – 20.
Ediger, M. (2002). Assessing handwriting achievement. Reading Improvement, 39, 103 – 110.
French primary school syllabus (2008). Horaires et programmes d’enseignement de l’école primaire. Bulletin officiel hors-
série n◦ 3 du 19 juin. [Ministry of National Education. Schedules and programs of primary school, Bulletin, 3, 19 June.]
Retrieved from http://www.education.gouv.fr/bo/2008/hs3/default.htm
Gates, A. I., & Brown, H. (1929). Experimental comparisons of print-script and cursive writing. Journal of Educational
Research, 20, 1 – 14.
Goodnow, J. J., Friedman, S., Bernbaum, M., & Lehman, E. B. (1973). Direction and sequence in copying : The effect of
learning to write in English and Hebrew. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 4, 263 – 282.
Graham, S., Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R. D., Abbott, S. P., & Whitaker, D. (1997). Role of mechanics in composing of
elementary school students: A new methodological approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 170 – 182.
Graham, S., Berninger, V. W., & Weintraub, N. (1998). The relationships between handwriting style and speed and legibility.
Journal of Educational Research, 5, 290 – 296.
Graham, S., Harris, K. R., & Fink, B. (2000). Is handwriting causally related to learning to write? Treatment of handwriting
problems in beginning writers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 620 – 633.
Graham, S., Harris, K. S., Mason, L., Fink-Chorzempa, B., Moran, S., & Saddler, B. (2008). How do primary grade teachers
teach handwriting? A national survey. Reading and Writing, 21, 49 – 69.
Graham, S., Struck, M., Santoro, J., & Berninger, V. W. (2006). Dimensions of good and poor handwriting legibility in first
and second graders: Motor programs, visual–spatial arrangement, and letter formation parameter setting. Developmental
Neuropsychology, 29, 43 – 60.
Graham, S., & Weintraub, N. (1996). A review of handwriting research: Progress and prospects from 1980 to 1994. Educational
Psychology Review, 8, 7 – 87.
Gray, W. S. (1956). The teaching of reading and writing. Chicago, IL: Scott Foresman.
Hamstra-Bletz, L., & Blöte, A. W. (1990). Development of handwriting in primary school: A longitudinal study. Perceptual
and Motor Skills, 70, 759 – 770.
Hamstra-Bletz, L., DeBie, J., & Den Brinker, B. (1987). Concise Evaluation Scale for children’s handwriting. Lisse, Nether-
lands: Swets & Zeitlinger.
Herrick, V. E. (1960). Handwriting and children’s writing. Elementary English, 37, 248 – 258.
Houston, H. (1938). Manuscript writing and progress in reading. Elementary School Journal, 39, 116 – 118.
Jones, D., & Christensen, C. A. (1999). Relationship between automaticity in handwriting and student’s ability to generate
written text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 44 – 49.
Jongmans, M. J., Linthorst-Bakker, E., Westenberg, Y., Smits-Engelsman, B. C., & Bouwien, C. (2003). Use of a task-
oriented self-instruction method to support children in primary school with poor handwriting quality and speed. Human
Movement Science, 22, 549 – 566.
Karlsdottir, R. (1996a). Development of cursive handwriting. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 82, 659 – 673.
Karlsdottir, R. (1996b). Print-script as initial handwriting style I: Effects on the development of handwriting. Scandinavian
Journal of Educational Research, 40, 161 – 174.
Karlsdottir, R. (1996c). Print-script as initial handwriting style II: Effects on the development of reading and spelling.
Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 40, 255 – 262.
Karlsdottir, R., & Stefansson, T. (2003). Predicting performance in primary school subjects. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 97,
1058 – 1060.
Laszlo, J. I., & Broderick, P. (1991). Drawing and handwriting difficulties: Reasons for and remediation of dysfunction. In
J. Wann, A. M. Wing, & N. Sovik (Eds.), The development of graphic skills (pp. 259 – 280). London, UK: Academic
Press.
Leung, E. K., Treblas, P. V., Cooper, J. O., & Porter, J. T. (1982). Effects of training for manuscript handwriting on
generalization to cursive handwriting. Behavioural Psychotherapy, 10, 311 – 323.
McCutchen, D. (2011). From novice to expert: Implications of language skills and writing-relevant knowledge for memory
during the development of writing skill. Journal of Writing Research, 3, 51 – 68.
McHale, K., & Cermak, S. A. (1992). Fine motor activities in elementary school: Preliminary findings and provisional
implications for children with fine motor problems. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 46, 898 – 903.
Medwell, J., Strand, S., & Wray, D. (2009). The links between handwriting and composing for Y6 children. Cambridge
Journal of Education, 39, 329 – 344.
Meulenbroek, R. G., & van Galen, G. (1986). Movement analysis of repetitive writing behavior of first, second and third
grade primary school children. In H. S. R. Kao, G. van Galen, & R. Hoosain (Eds.), Graphonomics: Contemporary
research in handwriting (pp. 71 – 92). Amsterdam, Netherlands: North-Holland.
Meulenbroek, R. G., & van Galen, G. (1988). The acquisition of skilled handwriting: Discontinuous trends in kinematic
variables. In A. M. Cooley & J. R. Beech (Eds.), Cognition and action in skilled behavior (pp. 273 – 281). Amsterdam:
North-Holland.
Meulenbroek, R. G., & van Galen, G. (1990). Perceptual-motor complexity of printed and cursive letters. Journal of
Experimental Education, 58, 95 – 110.
Meulenbroek, R. G., Vinter, A., & Mounoud, P. (1993). Development of the start-rotation principle in circle production.
British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 11, 307 – 320.
Ministry of Education, Quebec (2001). Programme de formation de l’école québécoise: Éducation préscolaire, enseignement
primaire. [School programs for kindergarten and primary school] Québec: National library.
Mojet, J. W. (1991). Characteristics of the developing handwriting skill in elementary education. In J. Wann, A. M. Wing, &
N. Sovik (Eds.), Development of graphic skills (pp. 53 – 75). London, UK: Academic Press.
Morin, M.-F., Lavoie, N., & Montésinos-Gelet, I. (2012). Graphomotor skills, spelling and writing in Grade 2: The effects of
teaching practices. Language and Literacy, 14, 110 – 124.