The in Uence of Superhero Comic Books On Adult Altruism: December 2016
The in Uence of Superhero Comic Books On Adult Altruism: December 2016
The in Uence of Superhero Comic Books On Adult Altruism: December 2016
net/publication/311325127
CITATIONS READS
0 1,059
1 author:
Jonathan Macfarlane
Richmont Graduate University
1 PUBLICATION 0 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by Jonathan Macfarlane on 02 December 2016.
Jonathan MacFarlane
Abstract
There is a body of work on the influence of fictional characters on personal development, and
some research on the effects of superhero role modeling on children’s behavior and resilience.
We use fictional characters’ experiences to understand our own lives (Ress, 2010) and
internalize societal values (Singh & Lu, 2004). Fictional characters are often viewed as role
models (Melnick, 2002), and children frequently look to superheroes as role models (McCrary,
1999). Superhero role modeling can improve behavior (Karniol et al., 2011) and even health
resilience (Fradkin, Weschenfelder, & Yunes, 2016). However, there has been very little
research specifically about the influence comic books have on the development and/or exercise
of prosocial/altruistic values or behaviors. This project looked for, and did not find, a connection
between childhood comic book reading and elevated prosocial values/behaviors in adulthood. A
connection was found between childhood comic book reading and negative opinion of the
comics have a priming effect on altruistic behavior; a significant positive influence was shown.
THE INFLUENCE OF SUPERHERO COMICS ON ALTRUISM !3
Superheroes have been recognized as potential positive role models since the beginning
of the comic book as an art form (Ruben, 2012). Comic book characters are widely recognized
and engaged with due to mainstream prevalence of comic book-inspired media. For example, in
2016 alone eight theatrical movies, 24 television shows, and five video games based on comic
book characters were aired/released. These eight movies, which are as popular with adults as
with children (Berman, 2016), made approximately $4.97 billion in box office ticket sales as of
December 2, 2016 (Box Office Mojo, n.d.). DC Comics’ television shows, which account for ten
of 2016’s 24, bring in over $1 billion in revenue annually (Szalai, 2016). In 2013, the top four
properties from Marvel Comics and DC Comics sold approximately $2.4 billion worth of
merchandise1 (Block, 2014). This surge in comic book-based mass media, along with other
childhood favorites from the 1980s and 90s like Transformers, Power Rangers, My Little Pony,
and G.I. Joe, is largely due to the internet enabling easy nostalgic fulfillment (Harvey, 2011) and
Superhero stories are melodramas, stories designed to play on the emotions of the
audience and typically feature the protagonists overcoming seemingly insurmountable problems
through extraordinary bravery, resilience, and/or sacrifice (Argo, Zhu, & Dahl, 2008). At a basic
level, superheroes demonstrate ordinary people can be greater than their station. Superman is
a small town farm boy who learns he is the most powerful being on the planet; Batman
witnesses his parents’ senseless murder and transforms himself into the dread of criminals
everywhere; Spider-Man is poor and bullied but gains extraordinary abilities through a science
mishap (Morrison, 2012). “Supergirl’s shared secret with readers is not the specific identity of
1Spider-Man (Marvel): $1.3 billion; Batman (DC): $494 million; Avengers (Marvel): $325 million;
Superman (DC): $277 million
THE INFLUENCE OF SUPERHERO COMICS ON ALTRUISM !4
her mundane alter ego…but that any mundane girl might be more powerful than she
The characters of the Golden Age of comics (approximately 1930-1960) were mythical
figures dealing with lofty ideals and black-and-white morality (Genter, 2007; Ruben, 2012).
Superman is “the champion of the oppressed…sworn to devote his existence to helping those in
need” (Siegel & Shuster, 1938), and Batman and Robin are sworn to “fight together against
crime and corruption” and stay on “the path of righteousness” (Finger, 1940). In a 1948 issue of
But remember, this man’s world of yours will never be without pain and suffering until it
learns love and respect for human rights. Keep your hands extended to all in
These characters, though, were not directly relatable to most readers (Morrison, 2012),
who were not demigod-like aliens, incredibly wealthy, or Amazonian. This is not to say, however,
they were without morally educational value. Binary values are the underlying support structure
of most stories, especially fable-esque stories like superhero comics (Gover & Conway, 1999).
Accordingly, most heroes’ archnemeses are mirrors of the heroes to which they belong, sharing
in common a type of origin and/or set of abilities but pursuing villainy rather than heroism
(Morrison, 2012; Langley, 2012). Most of the stories ended with the evildoers being arrested
and/or imprisoned for their crimes. If nothing else, Golden Age comic storytelling served as a
warning of the moral (and literal) cost of choosing the wrong path.
Comic book stories and their heroes underwent a change in the early 1960s, when
Marvel Comics introduced an ever-growing cast of characters2 who, along with their heroics,
faced real-world problems such as bullying, discrimination and racism, poverty, and even
homework (Genter, 2007). The stories also took place in fictionalized real-world locations—
mainly New York City—and would often involve, directly or indirectly, real-world events and/or
people (Howe, 2012). This change, which gradually carried over to other publishers’ characters,
strengthened superheroes’ potential as role models and potential real world forces for good
(Calvert, Strouse, & Murray, 2006; Singh & Lu, 2004), as audiences better respond to stories
with high verisimilitude, even when they feature highly fantastical elements (Argo, Zhu, & Dahl,
2008).
The aim of this study was two-fold. First, to determine if reading superhero comic books
superhero comics have a priming effect on altruistic behavior. I hypothesized participants who
read superhero comics as children would be more likely to hold and exercise prosocial/altruistic
values than those who did not (H1). I further hypothesized childhood readers would have a lower
opinion of the general population’s level of altruism (H2). My third hypothesis (H3) was
participants who read a superhero comic before encountering an “accident” befalling someone
else would be more likely and quicker to help than those who read the control material.
Literature Review
To my knowledge there is no published research into the effects of childhood comic book
reading on moral values or behavior, or research into the priming effects of comics on prosocial
behaviors. There is, however, a growing body of work on the influence of fictional characters in
When members of an audience can relate to the lives of fictional characters, they are
able to use the character’s experiences to better understand their own lives (Ress, 2010), as
well as recognize and internalize societal values (Singh & Lu, 2004), which aids in the
development of personal identity (Gover & Conway, 1999). On this subject, Ress, speaking of
Little Orphan Annie, writes, “they were not reading the paper; they were gazing into a funhouse
THE INFLUENCE OF SUPERHERO COMICS ON ALTRUISM !6
mirror, watching a distorted image of their own becoming” (p. 791). This is an important benefit
beliefs established in childhood, both positive and negative, generally continue through
adulthood (Calvert, Strouse, & Murray, 2006; Fradkin, Weschenfelder, & Yunes, 2016).
By thinking about ourselves in the context of fictional stories, we are able to visualize
potential outcomes (Martin, 2007). We take the lessons characters learn and improve ourselves
(Martin, 2007; Ruben, 2012; Singh & Lu, 2004; Melnick, 2002). In this way, fictional characters
influence personal development, values, and identity. This type of inspiration is the core of many
I wouldn’t have it any other way. Dreams save us. Dreams lift us up and transform us.
And on my soul, I swear, until my dream of a world where dignity, honor, and justice
becomes a reality we all share, I’ll never stop fighting. Ever. (Kelly, Mahnke, & Bermejo,
2001)
There’s this ayah [verse] from the Quran that my dad always quotes when he sees
something bad on TV. A fire or a flood or a bombing. ‘Whoever kills one person, it is as if
he has killed all of mankind; and whoever saves one person, it is as if he has saved all of
mankind.’ When I was a little kid that always made me feel better. (Wilson & Alphona,
2014)
Consistent engagement with a real-world positive role model has been shown to
positively impact academic, personal, and social behaviors and interactions (Fradkin,
Weschenfelder, & Yunes, 2016). Fictional characters frequently play as strong a role, especially
when engaged with frequently (Calvert, Strouse, & Murray, 2006). Melnick (2002) found 66% of
her participants consider fictional characters to be role models, especially those with similar
possess, or wish to possess, that influence our attachment to fictional characters (Gover &
Conway, 1999). Johnson (2010) and Greitemeyer, Osswald, and Brauer (2010) showed
engagement with a fictional story, and/or witnessing prosocial behavior in fictional characters,
increased empathy and real-world prosocial behavior. Empathy, in turn, allows the audience to
better connect and identify with the characters—a cyclical connection resulting in greater
internalization of characters’ aspects (Argo, Zhu, & Dahl, 2008). The obvious commitment of
superheroes to prosocial ideals and behaviors can play a role in moral development (Martin
Engaging with positive role model superheroes can lead children to take on the
characters’ traits for themselves. McCrary (1999) found 64.7% of her participants selected a
superhero as their personal hero. When imagining ourselves to be the character, we are able to
embody their perceived values and characteristics (Gover & Conway, 1999). In both children
and adults being told one can emulate characters’ desirable behaviors frequently triggers
carrying out said behaviors in the real world (Farsides, Pettman, & Tourle, 2013). In a study
conducted by Karniol et al., one participant explained her ability to delay gratification by saying
she “said goodbye to Superman and took his powers for myself” (Karniol et al., 2011, p. 312).
Along these lines, internalizing Spider-Man’s motto, “With great power there must also come
great responsibility” (Lee & Ditko, 1962), could increase a sense of social responsibility, and so
on. Nelson and Norton (2005) report evidence adjacent to this idea in their study, which found
participants primed with superhero-linked words and concepts were both more likely to
volunteer (short-term effect) and follow-through on that commitment several months later (long-
term effect).
THE INFLUENCE OF SUPERHERO COMICS ON ALTRUISM !8
Seeing comic book characters experience similar real-world problems can, to an extent,
alleviate feelings of being alone in a negative circumstance and foster the will to overcome; for
those not experiencing these circumstances, it can increase tolerance and empathy (Fradkin,
Weschenfelder, & Yunes, 2016; Karniol et al., 2011; Norton, 2003). Witnessing superheroes
overcome enemies of both the villainous and life circumstance variety provides “a means of
vicariously healing” from real-world traumas (Ruben, 2012, p. 422). There is some evidence
from a program at the A.C. Camargo Cancer Center that “invincibility suggestion”3 increases
pediatric patients’ resilience (Fradkin, Weschenfelder, & Yunes, 2016, p. 409). Even
superheroes without superhuman abilities (for example, Batman or Iron Man) can serve as
Method
The Helping Attitudes Scale (Nickell, 1998) was used to evaluate participants’ personal
beliefs, feelings, and behaviors associated with altruism. This survey asks participants to use a
5 point scale to rate their level of agreement with 20 statements concerning helping others.
These include both positively- and negatively-phrased items, such as “Doing volunteer work
makes me very happy” and “Helping others is usually a waste of time.” The negatively-phrased
items are reverse scored, then the individual’s score totaled. This yields a score ranging from 20
The Philosophies of Human Nature Scale (Wrightsman, 1964) was used to evaluate
participants’ opinions of the general population’s altruism. This survey asks participants to use a
6-point scale to evaluate others’ prosocial values and altruistic behaviors. The 14 items are
3Medical personnel making comparisons between patients and superheroes, and providing
comics, videos, and superhero branded medical supplies
THE INFLUENCE OF SUPERHERO COMICS ON ALTRUISM !9
randomized, and include both positive- and negatively-phrased statements, such as “The typical
person is sincerely concerned about the problems of others” and “The average person is
corresponding point value, as shown in Table 2. The individual’s points are totaled, yielding a
total ranging from -42 to 42, which corresponds to their opinion of others’ altruism (see Table 3).
My survey, which evaluated H1 and H2, combined these two scales, and also requested
participants’ demographic information: age, gender, race and/or ethnicity, education level, and if
superhero comics as children or adults and, if so, asked for their favorite superhero. The survey
was conducted both in-person and online. The physical and digital versions were identical in
format and structure; see Appendix for the full survey. ANOVA tests were performed on the
results of both scales to determine if there was a significant difference between the childhood
reader group (CR) and the non-childhood reader group (NCR); p < .05.
An experimental procedure was used to evaluate H3. Participants were asked to come
complete a survey regarding comic books and personality. When they arrived, they were asked
to read either a superhero comic book (experimental group; EG) or a nature article (control
group; CG), under the pretense of giving the researcher time to set things up for the survey and
to clear their minds for the study. The reading material was assigned by arrival order, with odd-
numbered participants creating the CG and even-numbered participants the EG. The EG read
until they finished the issue; the CG until they finished the article, or for 10 minutes (the article
The comic, Ms. Marvel (Wilson & Alphona, 2015), was selected because of its non-
traditional lead character4 , who is a Muslim, Pakistani-American, teenage female. The particular
issue was selected because it contained strongly prosocial messaging in its story. The nature
article for the CG, an overview of various species of octopus (Judson, 2016), is from National
Geographic. It was selected because it lacks any obvious encouragement of prosocial values or
behaviors. It was printed with only the text, to avoid discomforting any participants who might be
After reading, then signing an informed consent agreement, an accident scenario was
manufactured, by knocking a paper cup filled with pens off the table while handing the
participant their copy of the consent agreement or survey. I reacted as one normally would,
saying “oops” or similar, then beginning to pick up the pens. Participants were timed to see how
long it took for them to start helping. Once the pens were picked up, the participants then
completed the previously-described survey, and their time to help (TTH) recorded, along with
which reading material they’d been given. Prior to beginning data collection an average time to
pick up the pens unaided was established by, in situ, knocking the cup off and timing myself
picking them up; this was repeated 5 times. The average time (12 seconds) was used when
participants did not help, and half (six seconds) used when participants offered help, but did not
help automatically. An independent samples T-test was performed to check for a significant
difference in the TTH of the control and experimental groups; p < .05.
Participants
Participants for the survey portion of the study were recruited from the Georgia Gwinnett
College student population through the psychology department’s research study participation
system (SONA). Participants were also recruited from outside the student population through
postings on Facebook and Twitter. 222 responses were received; 17 were discarded because
the participant was underage or answers were incomplete, yielding N = 205. Participant age
ranged from 18 to 71, with M = 29.92 (SD = 10.25). 53.66% of participants identify as female,
THE INFLUENCE OF SUPERHERO COMICS ON ALTRUISM !11
43.2% as male, and 2.93% as other5. CR n = 97; NCR n = 108. CR age M = 33.09 (SD =
Participants for the experiment were recruited from the Georgia Gwinnett College
student population using SONA. N = 43, five male (11.63%) and 38 female (88.37%). The ages
Results
Survey
There was a significant difference between the scores of the CR and NCR with both
scales (see Tables 4 and 5). On the Helping Attitudes Scale (p = .04), M = 83.06 (SD = 8.78) for
the NCR and M = 80.93 (SD = 8.629) for the CR; both scores fall in the “helpful” range of the
scale (see Table 1). There was no significant difference between the scores of adult comic
readers and adult non-readers; participants who read both in childhood and adulthood and all
others; or those who selected as their favorite superheroes characters whose identity largely
defined by their morality6 and all others. On the Philosophies of Human Nature Scale (p = .02),
NCR M = 2.84 (SD = 12.61), while CR M = -.77 (SD = 11.63). The CR score is considered
“slightly negative,” while the NCR score falls into the “slightly positive” range (see Table 3).
Experiment
There was significant difference (p = .01) in the TTH of the EG and CG (see Tables 6
and 7). CG TTH, M = 5.05 seconds (SD = 4.07) and Mdn = 3.5 seconds; EG TTH M = 2.52
seconds (SD = 1.54) and Mdn = 2 seconds. One member (4.76%) of the experimental group
only offered help. Five members (22.73%) of the control group neither helped nor offered help,
5The survey had a space for participants to write in their gender identity of choice; the indicated
other percentage is comprised of all participants who entered anything besides male or female.
6 Captain America, Spider-Man, Superman, and Wonder Woman
THE INFLUENCE OF SUPERHERO COMICS ON ALTRUISM !12
Discussion
Survey
Helping Attitudes Scale. As expected, there was a significant difference in the self-
assessed level of altruism in the CR and NCR. The results, however, are the opposite of this
hypothesis, with the mean score of the CR being significantly lower than that of the NCR. As
such, H1 must be rejected. This result corresponds to Nelson and Norton’s (2005) finding that
participants primed by a notoriously altruistic superhero, Superman, were actually less likely
than the control group to help a stranger in a hypothetical scenario. However, they also found
that participants primed with the general category of superheroes were more likely than control
to help in the scenario. While our studies were exploring different effects—they were looking at
maintenance—one would expect similar results. The discrepancy may be explained by their use
It is not surprising this portion of study found both groups, on average, think of
themselves as altruistic. Only 1 member of each group scored in the “not helpful” range, and
another member of the NCR as “neutral.” Humans have an innate desire to be seen as good by
others, and this often colors responses to self-report surveys of this nature (Navarro-González,
Lorenzo-Seva, & Vigil-Colet, 2016). We tend to think the best of ourselves, even when there is
evidence of the contrary. For example, a participant might answer with a 4 or 5 to the statement,
“If possible, I would return lost money to the rightful owner,” knowing every time they’ve found
That said, it is interesting that although both groups overwhelmingly rated themselves as
being helpful the CR was more modest about their proclivity to help others. The “helpful” scores
of the CR are almost evenly split between the 61-80 and 81-100 point ranges, at 48.45% and
THE INFLUENCE OF SUPERHERO COMICS ON ALTRUISM !13
50.52%, respectively. The “helpful” scores of the NCR, on the other hand, are much more
concentrated in the 81-100 point range (62.04%) than the 61-80 point range (37.04%). A
possible explanation for this, which might also explain the lower overall average score, might be
that the CR are more self-aware of perceived moral shortcomings and this tempers the desire to
be perceived positively. It may also be that, while long-time readers identify with the overall
they do not integrate the personality traits of specific characters into their own. A more simple
explanation could be that superheroes are but one of a collection of positive forces for moral
development in a child’s life, and don’t have a significantly measurable effect on their own.
between the overall opinions of the CR and NCR, so H2 is not rejected. I did expect a more
strongly negative opinion of others’ level of altruism, especially from the CR. It is normal to rate
desirable traits as less-common in others than in one’s self (Fisher, 1993). Additionally, while the
CR’s opinion was lower—a difference of 3.61 points—I expected more negativity due to
consistent exposure to the idea that most people are inherently weak and need a hero to show
the way. In retrospect, this storytelling idea is consistent in most forms of popular fiction, so the
Experiment
As hypothesized, the EG was both more likely to help (95.24% vs. 72.73%), and quicker
to do so (2.52 seconds vs. 5.05 seconds). This indicates that, contrary to what many parents
have said over the years, comic books probably aren’t bad for you and may make you a more
altruistic person7. Picking up pens in an office likely doesn’t indicate a proclivity to grander
heroism, but that there is a positive effect is promising for further research. My finding
7 Amusingly, one member of the CG, after being debriefed, said, “After you spilled the pens, my
first thought was, ‘I should move my computer because you might spill your drink, too.’”
THE INFLUENCE OF SUPERHERO COMICS ON ALTRUISM !14
corresponds with Nelson and Norton’s (2015) that participants were more likely to volunteer and
representative of the general population. It is a common belief that women are more helpful than
men, and there is some evidence this is true, at least to an extent (Schmitt, 2016)—though at
least one study found the opposite to be the case (Eagly & Crowly, 1986). I found no significant
2.07; female n = 38, M = 4 seconds, SD = 3.43). However, to authoritatively state comic reading
makes someone more likely to be helpful requires replication with a proportional sample group.
Conclusion
Superheroes are an important, widely engaged-with aspect of our popular culture, and
their effects on their audience should be explored. I remain confident people who grow up
reading superhero comics are more likely than average to be altruistic; however, this study was
not the best way of looking for that difference. The weaknesses of self-report surveys for this
and similar traits prevent a fully-accurate evaluation. A better method for evaluating this question
might be to survey those familiar with the participants regarding the participants altruistic
behaviors and/or prosocial beliefs, rather than the participants themselves. Another possibility
It would be worthwhile to replicate the experimental portion of the study with a more well-
structured sample group, to obtain more generalizable results. It would also be interesting to
apply this experimental design to aspects other than helpfulness, such as resilience,
References
Argo, J. J., Zhu, R., & Dahl, D. W. (2008). Fact or fiction: An investigation of empathy
Berman, E. (2016, Nov. 4). Doctor Strange and what we’ve learned from superhero movies in
Block, A. B. (2014, Nov. 13). Which superhero earns $1.3 billion a year? The Hollywood
Box Office Mojo (n.d.). 2016 worldwide grosses. Box Office Mojo. Retrieved Dec. 2, 2016 from
http://boxofficemojo.com
Calvert, S. L., Strouse, G. A., Murray, K. J. (2006). Empathy for adolescents’ role model
selection and learning of DVD content. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 27,
444-455. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2006.06.005
Eagly, A. H. & Crowley, M. (1986). Gender and helping behavior: A meta-analytic review of the
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.100.3.283
Farsides, T., Pettman, D., & Tourle, L. (2013). Inspiring altruism: Reflecting on the personal
Finger, B. (1940). Introducing Robin, the boy wonder. Detective Comics [Digital Edition], 1(38).
Fisher, R. J. (1993). Social desirability bias and the validity of indirect questioning. Journal of
Fradkin, C., Weschenfelder, G. V., & Yunes, M. A. (2016). Shared adversities of children and
comic superheroes as resources for promoting resilience. Child Abuse & Neglect, 51,
Genter, R. (2007). “With great power comes great responsibility”: Cold War culture and the birth
10.1111/j.1540-5931.2007.00480.x
Gover, M. & Conway, P. (1999). To borrow and bestow: Identification as the acquisition of
Greitemeyer, T., Osswald, S., & Brauer, M. (2010). Playing prosocial video games increases
Harvey, E. (2011, Aug. 1). ‘Retromania’: Why is pop culture addicted to its own past? [Review of
the book Retromania: Pop culture’s addiction to its own past by S. Reynolds]. The Atlantic
Howe, S. (2012). Marvel Comics: The untold story. New York, NY: Harper Books.
Johnson, D.R. (2012). Transportation into a story increases empathy, prosocial behavior, and
perceptual bias toward fearful expressions. Personality and Individual Differences, 52,
150-155. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.10.005
Karniol, R., Galili, L., Shtilerman, D., Naim, R., Stern, K., Manjoch, H., & Silverman, R. (2011).
Why Superman can wait: Cognitive self-transformation in the delay of gratification paradigm.
10.1080/15374416.2011.546040
Kelly, J., Mahnke, D., & Bermejo, L. (2001). What’s so funny about truth, justice, and the
Langley, T. (2012). Batman and psychology: A dark and stormy knight [iBooks Edition].
Lee, S. & Ditko, S. (1962). Spider-Man! Amazing Fantasy [Digital Edition], 1(15), 1-11.
Link, A. (2013). The secret of Supergirl’s success. The Journal of Popular Culture, 46(6),
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03057240701325381
McCrary, J. H. (1999). Children’s heroes and heroines: Developing values manifested artwork.
Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Mid-South Educational Association, Point
Melnick, S. (2002). Fictional characters in books as positive role models for adolescent females.
Morrison, G. (2012). Supergods: What masked vigilantes, miraculous mutants, and a sun god
from Smallville can teach us about being human. New York, NY: Spiegel & Grau.
Navarro-González, D., Lorenzo-Seva, U., & Vigil-Colet, A. (2016). How response bias affects
10.7334/psicothema2016.113
Nelson, L.D. & Norton, M.I. (2005). From student to superhero: Situational primes shape future
10.1016/j.jesp.2004.08.003
Nickell, G. (1998). The helping attitudes scale. Paper presented at 106th Annual Convention of
Norton, B. (2003). The motivating power of comic books: Insights from Archie comic readers.
Peter, H. G. (1948). When treachery wore a green shirt. Sensation Comics [Digital Edition],
1(81).
THE INFLUENCE OF SUPERHERO COMICS ON ALTRUISM !18
Ress, S. (2010). Bridging the generation gap: Little Orphan Annie in the Great Depression. The
Ruben, L. (2012). Superheroes on the Couch: Exploring our limits. The Journal of Popular
Schmitt, D. P. (2016, Mar. 10). Are men more helpful, altruistic, or chivalrous than women?
Siegel, J. & Shuster, J. (1938). Superman. Action Comics [Digital Edition], 1(1).
Singh, M. & Lu, M. (2003). Exploring the function of heroes and heroines in children’s literature
from around the world. ERIC Digest, 183. Retrieved from http://eric.ed.gov
Szalai, G. (2016, Nov. 2). DC Universe movies heading to TNT, TBS. The Hollywood Reporter.
Wilson, G. W. & Alphona, A. (2014). All mankind. Ms. Marvel [Digital Edition], 3(2).
Wilson, G. W. & Alphona, A. (2015). Last days part two. Ms. Marvel, 3(17).
Wolf-Meyer, M. (2003). The world Ozymandias made: Utopias in the superhero comic,
subculture, and the conservation of difference. The Journal of Popular Culture, 36(3), 497–
Table 1
Helping Attitudes Scale Score Interpretation
Table 2
Philosophies of Human Nature Scoring
Response 1 2 3 4 5 6
Point value -3 -2 -1 1 2 3
Table 3
Philosophies of Human Nature Score Interpretation
Table 4
Survey Scales ANOVA Results, Childhood Reader
Note. Scale One = Helping Attitudes Scale; Scale Two = Philosophies of Human Nature Scale
THE INFLUENCE OF SUPERHERO COMICS ON ALTRUISM !20
Table 5
Mean, Median, and Score Ranges for Scales
Scale 1 Scale 2
Note. Scale One = Helping Attitudes Scale; Scale Two = Philosophies of Human Nature Scale;
CR = Childhood readers; NCR = Non-childhood readers
Table 6
Experiment Group Statistics
Table 7
Experiment Group Independent Samples T-Test Results
Equal var. 13.48 0.001 2.67 41 0.01 2.52 0.95 0.61 4.43
assumed
Equal var. not 2.71 27.12 0.01 2.52 0.93 0.62 4.43
assumed
THE INFLUENCE OF SUPERHERO COMICS ON ALTRUISM !21
Appendix: Survey
Using the scale below, indicate your level of agreement or disagreement in the space next to
each statement. This is not a test, so there are no right or wrong answers. Please answer as
honestly as possible.
1 2 3 4 5
Using the scale below, indicate your level of agreement or disagreement in the space next to
each statement. This is not a test, so there are no right or wrong answers. Please answer as
honestly as possible.
1 2 3 4 5 6
1. Most people try to apply the Golden Rule even in today’s complex society.
2. The typical person is sincerely concerned about the problems of others.
3. Most people will act as “Good Samaritans” if given the opportunity.
4. “Do unto others as you would have them do unto you” is a motto most people follow.
5. It’s only a rare person who would risk their own life and limb to help someone else.
6. Most people would stop and help a person whose car is disabled.
7. Most people with a fallout shelter would let their neighbors stay in it during a nuclear
attack.
8. People are usually out for their own good.
9. It’s pathetic to see an unselfish person in today’s world because so many take
advantage of them.
10. People pretend to care more about one another than they really do.
11. Most people do not hesitate to go out of their way to help someone in trouble.
12. Most people exaggerate their troubles in order to get sympathy.
13. Most people inwardly dislike putting themselves out to help other people.
14. The average person is conceited.
THE INFLUENCE OF SUPERHERO COMICS ON ALTRUISM !24
Once you’ve completed the survey, please return it to the researcher. You will be given a
debriefing document with an explanation of the research being conducted; at that point your
participation will be concluded.