Aims of Critical Discourse Analysis Van Dijk 1995
Aims of Critical Discourse Analysis Van Dijk 1995
Aims of Critical Discourse Analysis Van Dijk 1995
1(1995), 17-27
A i m s of Critical D i s c o u r s e Analysis
Teun A. van Dijk
CDA-studies (may) pay attention to all levels and dimensions of discourse, viz
those of grammar (phonology, syntax, semantics), style, rhetoric, schematic
organization, speech acts, pragmatic strategies, and those of interaction, amonj
others.
Much work in CDA deals with the discursively enacted or legitimated structures
and strategies of dominance and resistance in social relationships of class, gender,
ethnicity, race, sexual orientation, language, religion, age, nationality or world-region.
Much work in C D A is about the underlying ideologies that play a role in the
reproduction of or resistance against dominance or inequality.
This attempt to uncover the discursive means of mental control and social
influence implies a critical and oppositional stance against the powerful and the
elites, and especially those who abuse of their power.
criteria, which are not merely descriptive of critical studies but also more or less
programmatic: This is what C D A studies ideally should try to realize. However, these
criteria provide the major traits of an approach that distinguishes it fairly well from other
-19work on discourse: CDA is essentially dealing with an oppositional study of the structures
and strategies of elite discourse and their cognitive and social conditions and consequences,
as well as with the discourses of resistance against such domination. In that respect, it goes
beyond the usual methodological criteria of observational, descriptive and explanatory
adequacy. Adding the criterion of critical adequacy presupposes social norms and values
and introduces a social or political ethics (what we find wrong or right) within the
scholarly enterprise as such. It is not surprising that such a view is often seen as "political"
(biased) and hence as "unscientific" ("subjective") by scholars who think that their
"objective" uncritical work does not imply a stance and hence a sociopolitical position,
viz., a conservative one that serves to sustain the status quo. Critical Discourse Analysis,
thus, emphasizes the fact that the scholarly enterprise is part and parcel of social and
political life, and that therefore also the theories, methods, issues and data-selection in
discourse studies are always political. Unlike other, implicitly political studies of
discourse, CDA explicitly formulates its (oppositional) stance.
How to do Critical Discourse Analysis?
As much of the scholarly aftermath of the student-revolution of 1968 has shown,
lofty ideals do not necessarily lead to good scholarship. This is also true for CDA, and
it is therefore crucial to examine in detail how critical studies of text and talk may
effectively realize the criteria and the tasks spelled out above. At a practical level, for
instance, students typically will want to know "how to do CDA," which requires us to
formulate proposals for successful research strategies. Theoretically and descriptively we
need to explore which structures and strategies of text and talk to attend to in order to discover patterns of elite dominance or manipulation "in" texts. Or conversely, focussing
on major social and political problems and issues such as sexism and racism, we need to
detail how such forms of inequality are expressed, enacted, legitimated and reproduced by
text and talk.
In sum, adequate CDA requires good theories of the role of discourse in the
enactment and reproduction of social dominance and resistance. More than theories that
merely claim descriptive or explanatory adequacy, however, successful CDA must be
effective: Its conclusions, recommendations and other practical interventions must work.
These are pretty tough criteria, and we should not have the illusion that they will be met
soon. In that respect, CDA is not only a scholarly practice, but also a scholarly program
of research.
-21forms of control and access, e.g., when politicians, journalists, professors, managers or
judges prevent others from legitimate forms of text or talk (censorship), or when they
engage in talk or text that otherwise limits the freedom or rights of other participants, as
when police officers threaten immigrant defendants with expulsion, or when sexist
professors threaten female students with a bad grade if they do not consent in sexual
favors.
Such patterns of control also extend to the more detailed structures of text and
context. Thus context control may consist of control of "calling" a communicative event,
making an appointment or setting the agenda. It may involve decisions about time and
location, and who may participate in the event and in what role. Similarly, discursive
control may apply to all levels and dimensions of text and talk, such as language variants,
genres, topics, grammar, lexical style, rhetorical figures, overall organization, local and
global coherence, speech acts, turn taking, politeness forms, and so on. Thus, in a
parliamentary hearing, in the courtroom, in a police interrogation, during an oral exam
or a tax audit, the institutional power holders may oblige participants to use a specific
language variant, to answer questions, to provide information, and to use a polite style.
In all these institutional communicative events such power may be abused to censor,
intimidate or otherwise limit the freedom of less powerful participants. Detailed
contextual and textual analysis is necessary to pinpoint the sometimes subtle strategies of
such forms of discursive dominance.
Mind Control
Preferential access to, and control over discourse and its properties are forms of
the direct enactment of social or institutional power. They allow specific social actors to
engage in (verbal) action that is prohibited to others, or they may force or oblige others
to engage in discourse or to use discourse properties as desired by the powerful actor,
thereby limiting the freedom (and hence the power) of the less powerful.
Discourse, however, is not limited to verbal action, but also involves meaning,
interpretation and understanding. This means that preferential access to public discourse
or control over its properties (e.g., specific, preferred topics) may also affect the minds of
others. That is, powerful social actors not only control communicative action, but
indirectly also the minds of recipients. We know that these processes of "influence" are
exceedingly complex: The fate of "effect research" in mass communication, or the
fuzziness of critical notions such as "preferred meanings" or "manufacturing consent", all
show that controlling the mind through text and talk is not a straightforward process.
Research on attention, interpretation, comprehension, memory storage, and other aspects
-22of information processing defining "reception", has shown that these are a function of
properties of the text as well as of properties of the context, and especially of the previous
knowledge, attitudes or ideologies of recipients.
Yet, we all k n o w that news reports, political propaganda, advertising, religious
sermons, corporate directives or scholarly articles somehow influence the "minds" of
readers and hearers: They convey knowledge, affect opinions or change attitudes. We also
know that recipients, in a specific context and given their extant knowledge and beliefs,
may disregard, reject, dis-believe, or otherwise mentally act in opposition to the intentions
of powerful speakers or writers. They may have relative freedom to interpret and use
discourses as they please and in their own best interests, as is also the case for media
messages.
Despite such freedom, however, there are many constraints. These may be a
function of the power, as well as the status or credibility, of the speaker/writer, as well
as a function of the properties of text or talk: People may be lied to, manipulated,
persuaded or otherwise influenced against their best interests, or in the interests of the
powerful speaker/writer. People may lack alternative sources of information, or they
may lack the knowledge of rules and strategies of grammar or discourse, they may not
have sufficient knowledge to detect lies and manipulation, or strong counter-opinions or
counter-ideologies to argue against and reject influential text and talk.
In sum, powerful speakers may control at least some parts of the minds of
recipients. C D A studies the ways in which such influence and control of the mind is
socially or morally illegitimate, e.g., when powerful speakers self-servingly control the
minds of others in a way that is in the interest of the powerful. Since action is based on
mental models of actors, models which in turn embody social knowledge and attitudes,
influencing such models or the beliefs on which they are based may be an effective way
to (indirectly) control the actions of other people.
For instance, if European politicians and media attribute major social problems
(such as unemployment or a housing shortage) to immigration and immigrants, they may
thus influence the beliefs of large segments of the majority, and thus indirectly the models
that underlie racist stories or discriminatory action of dominant group members, as well
as other expressions of prejudice and resentment that may again be used by the politicians
to legitimate political decisions on immigration restrictions.
Again, all this is more or less known in the cognitive and social psychology of
language, discourse and communication.
properties of speakers and especially of discourses are the preferred and most effective
means of such forms of mind control. To be sure, political propagandists, advertisers,
-23journalists or professors have enough practical experiences to have some idea about what
kind of messages will have what kinds of effects. They know how to effectively change
the knowledge and opinions of recipients, and what kind of social actions will typically
result from such mind control. However, such intuitive, practical knowledge is largely
implicit, and few theories of communicative influence or discourse processing spell out
in detail under which precise textual or contextual conditions, and for what recipients,
specific forms of mind control will occur.
Besides spelling out the detailed forms of discourse control and access as described
above, CDA needs to focus on the morally illegitimate forms of discursive mind control
by the powerful. It needs to specify how powerful speakers control properties of text and
talk in such a way that they are able to monitor the mind of recipients in their own
interests. For instance, by emphasizing specific topics at the expense of others (e.g.,
through headlines or summaries) or by preventing others to address other topics, they
may influence the overall (mental) model structures that are involved in discourse
comprehension. In the example mentioned above, defining immigration as a problem or
as a threat (e.g. as an "invasion of refugees") instead of as an economic and cultural
contribution to the country or city, is one of the persuasive ways dominant speakers are
able to mold the models of (white) citizens, if these have no alternative representations of
immigration. In the same way, immigrants and their actions may be described by negative
lexical terms or rhetorical figures. Conversely, negative actions of the elites (or of "our
own group") may be de-emphasized topically, stylistically or rhetorically, whereas "our"
positive actions and properties may be textually emphasized in interactional strategies of
face keeping and positive self-presentation.
At all levels of text and talk we may thus postulate structures that preferentially
affect the structure and content of mental models, as well as the more general and abstract
forms of knowledge, beliefs, opinions, attitudes or ideologies that are shared by groups
of recipients. For instance, strategic generalizations in discourse ("this always happens like
that"; "they are all the same," etc.) may influence the generalization of ad hoc situation
models to more abstract group attitudes and prejudices. Specific forms of local coherence
may facilitate attributing negative properties to THEM (e.g. "lacking motivation") and
positive ones to US (e.g., employment programs) in explanations of minority
unemployment. Generally, rhetorical figures such as hyperboles and metaphors may
similarly be used to emphasize OUR good properties and THEIR bad ones, whereas
figures of mitigation may de-emphasize OUR negative properties and THEIR good ones.
Narrative structures may be deployed to detail and structure models of events in specific
ways, and generally to enhance credibility, for instance as forms of "personal experience
-25Bibliographical Note
There as yet no general, book-length introductions to Critical Discourse Analysis.
Against the combined background of Marxist and Neo-Marxist approaches to the links
between language and social class, of Frankfurt School Critical Theory, of critical
sociolinguistics, British Cultural Studies and functional linguistics, among other directions
of research too vast to be bibliographically detailed here, one of the first collections of
critical linguistics is that of British and Australian linguists Towler, Hodge, Kress &
Trew (1979)(see also *Fowler, 1991; *Hodge & Kress, 1988; *Kress & Hodge, 1993), most
of whom work in the functional framework of Halliday's systemic grammar and
semiotics. Similarly in Britain, and within a more neo-marxist tradition, *Fairclough
(1989, 1992) explores the relations between language and power. *Chilton (1985, 1988)
specifically focused on critical studies of political discourse, e.g., about so-called "Nukespeak" (for other more or less critical studies of political discourse, see also *Geis, 1987;
*Seidel, 1988; *Wilson, 1990). More recently, such critical linguistics tends to be
subsumed under the broader labels of Critical Discourse Analysis or Social Semiotics. In
a continental tradition of critical sociolinguistics and text linguistics, Wodak and her
associates focus on institutional power, male dominance, racism and anti-Semitism, among
many other critical topics (among much other work, see, e.g., *Wodak, 1989; *Wodak,
et al., 1987; *Wodak, et al., 1990). My own critical work in Amsterdam focuses on racism
and on the media, and more recently on the relations between discourse and ideology (see,
e.g., ""van Dijk, 1987, 1988, 1991, 1993). A recent issue of Discourse & Society publishes
several papers of these critical linguists and discourse analysts. Both in North America
and in Europe, many papers and some book collections have focused on the study of
language and power (*Kedar, 1987; *Kramarae, Schulz, & O'Barr, 1984; *Lakoff, 1990;
*Ng & Bradac, 1993), but not always within a discourse-analystic framework. Similarly,
also work on professional language and discourse often takes a critical position (*Fisher
& Todd, 1986). Mainstream conversation analysis has as yet avoided taking critical
positions, but new developments point to an increasing interest in linking the study of
talk with social structure (*Boden & Zimmerman, 1991; *Drew & Heritage, 1992).
Critical language and discourse studies would have been unthinkable without the
powerful example of feminist work in the study of gender, language use and discourse
(among many other studies, see, e.g., "'Bull & Swan, 1992; *Lakoff, 1975; T h o m e ,
Kramarae & Henley, 1983; *Fisher & Todd, 1988). For further critical studies on
discourse, see also the contribitions in Discourse & Society. "Whereas the references given
here are all in English, there is of course a rich tradition of critical studies in Germany,
-26France, Italy, Spain and Latin America, among other countries, which however cannot
be detailed here, except with reference to the work of Techeux (1975; 1982) who inspired
much work on language and ideology; of Foucault (e.g., Toucault, 1980) who is often
cited in work on discourse and power, and the work of Habermas (e.g., *Habermas 1991)
on communication and its influence on critical pragmatics, among many others.
References
Boden, Deirdre and Don H. Zimmerman, (eds.) 1991. Talk and Social Structure: Studies
in Ethnomethodology and Conversation Analysis. Cambridge: Polity Press.
Bull, Tove and Toril Swan, (eds.) 1992. Language, sex and society. Special issue of
International Journal of the Sociology of language, 94.
Chilton, Paul, (ed.) 1985. Language and the Nuclear Arms Debate: Nukespeak Today.
London: Pinter.
.1988. Orwellian Language and the Media. London: Pluto Press.
Drew, Paul and John Heritage, (eds.) 1992. Talk at Work: Interaction in Institutional
Settings. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Fairclough, Norman L. 1989. Language and Power. London: Longman.
. 1992. Discourse and Social Change. Oxford: Polity Press.
Fisher, Sue and Alexandra Dundas Todd, (eds.) 1986. Discourse and Institutional Authority:
Medicine, Education, and Law. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
. (eds.) 1988. Gender and Discourse: The Power of Talk. Norwood, NJ: Ablex
Publishing Company.
Foucault, Michel. 1980. Power/Knowledge: Selected Interviews and Other Writings, 19721977. New York: Pantheon.
Fowler, Roger. 1991. Language in the News: Discourse and Ideology in the Press. London:
Routledge.
, Bob Hodg, Gunther Kress, and T. Trew. 1979. Language and Control. London:
Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Geis, Michael L. 1987. The Language of Politics. New York: Springer.
Hodge, Robert and Gunther Kress. 1988. Social Semiotics. London: Polity Press.
Kedar, Leah, (ed.) 1987. Power Through Discourse. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Kramarae, Cheris, Muriel Schulz and William M. O'Barr. (eds.) 1984. Language and
Power. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Kress, Gunther and Bob Hodge. 1993. Language as Ideology. Second Edition. London: