Attachment Theory and Fathers
Attachment Theory and Fathers
Attachment Theory and Fathers
282
40 years. Attachment theory has a long history (Ainsworth, 1967; Bowlby, 1969) that has
carefully examined and described the evolving parentchild relationships in early childhood
with a focus on mother as the primary attachment figure. Research into the fathers role as
attachment figures also began in the 1970s and
has gone through distinct stages of development (Bretherton, 2011). This review focuses
on the accumulation of knowledge and insights
about fathers and attachment from both research
and practice literature. Mikulincer and Shaver
(2012), in their recent review of attachment theory and the interface with family theory and
research, open the door for family researchers
and practitioners to further explore the dyadic
concepts from attachment theory as a way to
enrich our understanding of family relationships. This review can be seen as an extension
of the marriage of attachment theory and family research to focus specifically on fathers as
important attachment figures within diverse family systems.
The review of literature has incorporated
a variety of previous reviews of attachment
research on fathers (Bretherton, 2011; Hoffman,
2011; Lamb & Lewis, 2010), meta-analyses
(Fox, Kimmerly, & Schafer, 1991; Lucassen
et al., 2011; Van IJzendoorn & DeWolff, 1997),
longitudinal work on attachment (e.g., Grossmann, Grossmann, & Waters, 2005; Sroufe,
Egeland, Carlson, & Collins, 2005). In addition, I completed a search of the databases
in Academic Search Premier using the terms
father and attachment for the years 20002014.
A recent book on the scholarship of fathers and
283
rooted in the function of protection of infants
from danger. Mothers were the primary focus
for this developing relationship. Attachment is
about our first relationships that are dynamic yet
have enduring characteristics that profoundly
influence later development. The dynamic and
complex nature of parentchild relationships
makes it difficult to capture and measure this
evolving relationship in a precise manner.
Bowlby, in the development of attachment
theory, brings together different perspectives
from psychoanalytic theory, learning theory, and
cognitive psychology with an ethological perspective to describe and explain the developing
bond between mother and infant (Karen, 1998).
Attachment theory stems from ethology and the
notion that an infant is biologically wired or
programmed to seek proximity with caregivers
to get their needs met. The biological function
of attachment behaviors is to ensure survival.
From an evolutionary, ethological perspective
the infant is equipped with signaling capabilities
to gain parent attention as a means to getting
their needs met and ensuring survival. The
infant communicates distress through crying;
seeks proximity for security; and initiates social
engagement through smiling, cooing, and gazing. The parent also is equipped with instinctual
resources to attend to and respond to child cues
indicating a need for care. Bowlby describes how
the attachment relationship provides a secure
base for the child to explore and a safe haven to
return to in times of distress. Reciprocity of the
motherinfant relationship was a key factor in
Bowlbys description of attachment. For infants
the goal of their attachment behaviors was to
reduce stress and create a sense of security
through contact with their caregiver (mother).
For the caregiver, the goals were to regulate the
infants arousal and to respond to their bids for
attention and security (Gonzalez, Atkinson, &
Fleming, 2009).
Attachment Categories
Mary Ainsworth (1967) was the first to create
a research protocol to define and assess the
attachment relationship. It was based on an
observational procedure that identified patterns
of child responses to parenting behaviors that
led to different classifications. She described
these patterns as indicative of secure or insecure
attachment relationship patterns. The original
basis for the categories was careful observation
284
of mothers and infants in their home environments. Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, and Wall
(1978) developed the Strange Situation Protocol (SSP) as a research tool for assessing
parentchild relationship patterns. The original tool led to classification of parentchild
relationship pattern into three distinct categories: secure, insecure-avoidant, and
insecure-anxious/ambivalent. In the secure
category the child explores the environment
and checks back with parent as secure base.
The child will engage with a stranger when the
parent is nearby, cries when the parent leaves,
and is happy to reunite with parent. Parents tend
to be sensitive and responsive in their interactions with their child. The insecure-avoidant
relationship category is characterized by the
child showing little interest in adults and tending to avoid or ignore the parent when the
stranger is present. The child lacks affective
expression and doesnt interact with adults
or the parent when he or she returns. In the
insecure-anxious/resistant category the child
expresses distress when the stranger is present,
even if the parent is nearby. The child expresses
distress when parent leaves but when parent
returns is often resistant to reuniting. Disorganized attachment was added as a fourth
category when several children did not fit into
the three identified classifications (Main &
Solomon, 1990). This classification represents
a lack of organized behavioral strategies for
managing stresses in the SSP. These children
may experience their caregivers as frightening
or frightened.
285
responses that reflect value for attachment
relationships. These responses are related to
SSPs with children that are secure. A second
category, Dismissing, represents responses
for which the adult caregivers parents are
idealized but no specific details are shared.
Caregivers in this category were related to
SSP attachment that was avoidant. A third
category, Preoccupied, represents responses
that are unreflective, vacillating, and confusing
accounts of a conflicted childhood. Caregivers
in this category were related to SSPs that were
classified as ambivalent. A final category, Unresolved/Disorganized, represents someone
who may fit into the first through third categories but has lapses in the interview that include
references to traumatic events. Parents in this
category were related to disorganized SSPs.
Children may see these parents as frightening
or frightened (dangerous or helpless). The AAI
has been used to describe and assess intergenerational transmission of attachment patterns.
Summary of the Importance of Attachment
Theory
The significance of attachment theory to
long-term development is best described through
the results of longitudinal studies that have been
conducted to track child outcomes related to
different early attachment categories (e.g.,
Grossmann et al., 2005; Sroufe et al., 2005).
These studies track children and the impacts of
attachment on development and how these may
change over time. The results emphasize the
importance of early attachment and the development of internal working models and how
these persist as states of mind in adulthood.
The pathways that have been created by different attachment qualities are influential but not
deterministic and the results suggest that mothers and fathers have unique influences on child
personality development that emerge at different
stages of development (Grossmann et al., 2002).
Historical Review of Attachment
Research With Fathers
The focus on fathers using attachment theory
has led to different research questions over
4 decades that reflect attempts to apply the
original constructs and methods of study to
fatherchild relationships. The history of fathers
and attachment theory has mirrored in many
286
ways the evolving concerns about the changing role of fathers, the importance of father
involvement in childrens lives, and fathers
influence on child development. Attachment
theory has also generated controversy about
generalizations to fatherchild relationships
(Grossmann et al., 2002; Paquette & Bigras,
2010) based on the application of methods
designed from motherinfant observations
and the limited explanatory power in regard
to understanding the developing fatherchild
relationship. Pleck (2007) also questions the
usefulness of attachment theory to describing
the influence of fathers on child development.
Although the major emphasis in attachment
research on fathers has been on the empirical
study of fatherchild attachment system, the
change in family systems and the social-political
context have influenced the types of questions
that researchers have asked and how their
results have been interpreted. The gender politics related to attachment theory and how this
has affected our views of attachment are critical
to making sense of the research agendas that
have emerged and the implications of research
for practice. The influence of gender politics
around valuing the role and importance of
fathers involvement in childrens lives will be a
thread that is woven into the historical review.
Phase 1: Fathers as Attachment Figures
Bretherton (2011) describes four different
phases of research and different focal questions in explaining how attachment theory
has been applied to understanding fathers and
fatherchild relationships. In the first phase starting in the 1960s, Ainsworth (1967) noted that
fathers and other adults were attachment figures
in some of the families that she observed. A
previous study by Schaffer and Emerson (1964)
had also described a similar pattern, with some
fathers emerging as primary attachment figures.
Bretherton notes that in both of these studies
fathers were selected by children because of
their responsiveness versus time spent in caretaking. Bowlby (1969) recognized that fathers
could also be important attachment partners but
saw them as supplementary playmate attachment figures rather than primary attachment
figures. Children would seek out mothers when
they needed comfort and fathers when they were
looking for a playmate. The research question
that came from these observations was, Can
fathers serve as primary and/or equal attachment partners? Originally, attachment theory
research focused on the importance of mothers
to child development and mental health, and
this fit well with the late 1940s and early 1950s
social and family environments. In both the
United States and the United Kingdom mothers
were moving out of the workforce into a primary
role of child care in the home as men returned
from World War II to take over jobs outside
the home. Attachment theory and the study of
problems with orphans from the war affirmed
the importance of mothers as consistent caregivers in the lives of young children. Fathers or
others may fulfill this role if mothers were not
available, but this was seen as an exception to
the typical attachment patterns.
Phase 2: Mother Hierarchy and Father Role
In Phase 2 (1960s and 1970s) Bretherton (2011)
describes the early work of Michael Lamb, who
began to ask new questions about the role of
fathers in attachment through a series of observational studies. Lamb (1976b) was interested in
addressing the hierarchy hypothesis that mothers
were the primary attachment figure and fathers
form supplementary attachments in the shadows
of the primary relationship. Lamb found that
infants demonstrated similar levels of approach
and contact with both mothers and fathers and
more affiliative behaviors (smiling, vocalizing,
and showing toys) to fathers than mothers. When
a stranger was introduced, infants shifted their
attention to the mothers. This child response to
stress illustrated the hierarchy of attachment to
mothers as preferred partners during times of
stress but affirmed that fathers could also be partners in secure-attachment relationships.
The changing family structures of the late
1960s and 1970s and the movement of women
into the workforce led to some new questions
about attachment and the role of fathers and
child care. By the mid-1970s there was clear
evidence that children did form early attachments with their fathers (Lamb & Lewis, 2010).
During this phase, Lamb (1977b) noted some
differences in fathers and mothers behavior
toward infants and that toddlers showed more
affiliative behaviors toward fathers (Lamb,
1977a). Bretherton (2011) notes that Lamb
suggested that motherinfant and fatherinfant
relationships may be experienced in different
ways by infants (mothers are a source of comfort
287
may both have independent attachment relationship patterns with their children based on their
own early experiences. This reinforces an ethic
of gender equality even though mothers still
spend more time with children (Pleck, 2010).
Phase 4: Father Attachment in the Family
Context
Bretherton (2011) describes longitudinal studies
in Phase 4 (1990s2000s) that begin to address
the question, are the developmental outcomes
of father and mother attachments different even
if both are secure? Study results (Grossmann
et al., 2005) demonstrate the complexity of the
relationships between attachment relationships
with mothers and fathers and prediction of child
behavior at different stages. Grossmann et al.s
(2002) results identify sensitivity in mothers during interaction with infants and fathers sensitivity during play in interaction with toddlers to
be the most salient predictors of important child
outcomes assessed at different ages (age 610
and age 16). This confirms the notion that different pathways (play vs. caregiving) and processes (sensitivity to distress and synchrony in
exploratory play) have different outcomes for
children.
In a meta-analysis of SSPs with motherchild
and fatherchild, DeWolff and van IJzendoorn
(1997) identified a large number of cases (38%)
in which the attachment category for mothers
and fathers was not concordant. These findings
led to calls to focus on family perspectives
to understand attachment relationships (Belsky, 1996; Cowan, 1997) that would include
fatherchild, motherchild, and fathermother
relationships (McHale, 2007).
The results of attachment research during
this time occur concurrently with increasing
family structure diversity that included a greater
focus on programs for responsible fatherhood
as well as an increase in nonmarital births and
single-mother families. Longitudinal studies
have included traditional family structures
that reflect more gender stereotypical roles in
their impacts on children. The mixed results of
research during this time may be related to the
changing expectations for father involvement,
with greater involvement of fathers as a new
norm (Pleck, 2010) but fathers increasingly were
not present in the home (Blankenhorn, 1995).
Are the longitudinal research results about
fathers attachment primarily through their role
288
as a playmate still relevant to understanding
the diverse experiences of fathers in changing
family structures in 2014? Bretherton (2011)
offers suggestions about how to move research
on attachment forward to address new questions
related to diverse family systems. A key question beyond how mothers and fathers are both a
secure base for exploration and a safe haven for
emotional regulation is how mothers and fathers
value and support each others contributions,
whether similar or different (Bretherton, 2011,
p. 21). This issue of attending to the coparenting
relationship is not new (Cowan, 1997) but has
continued to gain attention (McHale, 2007) and
begun to enter into research on fatherhood practice (Cowan, Cowan, Pruett, Pruett, & Wong,
2009). The call for a family system view of
attachment acknowledges that family context
is essential for understanding the fatherchild
attachment relationship and different pathways
and processes that may help explain not only
that the relationship does exist but, more important, how it happens in different family contexts.
Which factors support processes that lead to
secure attachment relationships and which factors function as risks for insecure attachment
relationships?
Toward a Family Model of Attachment
The call for a family perspective of attachment
comes from a number of sources (e.g., Belsky,
1996; Cowan, 1997). Fathers relationships
with children have been viewed as influenced more by outside factors (Belsky, 1996;
Doherty, Kouneski, & Erickson, 1998; Lundy,
2002; Madigan, Benoit, & Boucher, 2011;
Schoppe-Sullivan et al., 2006). The fatherchild
attachment relationship is a two-way reciprocal relationship that is sensitive to a variety
of different factors in the father, the child,
the fathermother relationship, and the family
system. Most of the reviews of attachment
research about fathers end with the realization
that a complex set of factors interact and change
over time and in different contexts to influence
the fatherchild relationship. Parke (2002)
calls for a multilevel and dynamic approach
to understand the fatherchild relationship.
Lamb and Lewis (2011) note in their review of
parentchild relationships that current research
has begun to use multilevel, structural equations
and hierarchical modeling techniques to reflect
this understanding. They also caution that while
289
with their children. Coparenting patterns emerge
early in the first year of life with a new baby
(McHale, 2007), which suggests that interventions to support father attachment should begin
shortly after birth. Palm and Joyce (1994), in a
qualitative study with fathers of young children,
found that some fathers expressed competition
with mothers for childrens attention as a barrier
even if the mother was not playing a gatekeeper
role to limit access to the baby. This new triadic relationship may become a barrier for some
fathers that blocks their entry into the new family
system if they perceive that children favor their
mothers to meet their needs. Fathers may feel
excluded from the dyadic bubble, and lose confidence in their ability to meet their childs needs
for comfort and emotional regulation. Marital
conflict is another family-level factor that has
been identified (Van IJzendoorn, Schuengel, &
Bakermans-Kranenburg, 1999). The research
literature tends to ignore the residence status
of fathers, which also is a major factor that
keeps fathers from regular interaction and can
lessen their influence on the stability of the
family system. The influence of multiple family
systems on fathers reflects the complexity of
even the simplest family form of mother, father,
and child. The motherfather relationship and
the emerging triadic family interactions serve
as contexts that support fatherchild attachment
relationships and create a sense of family as a
larger secure base and safe haven or encourage
detachment.
Cultural Context
The role of cultural and community context is
also important but the specific research base
around attachment theory is limited. Feldman
(2012b) does describe some specific culturally
defined roles for fathers that may change the
trajectory of the attachment relationship based
on how roles of fathers, especially with young
children, are defined. Belsky (1996) identifies
workfamily harmony that influences fathers
attachment patterns. In a related area, family
income (Nievar & Becker, 2008) has been
described as a factor that influences stress that
may limit parent sensitivity, affect parents
emotional regulation, and affect parents ability
to engage in enjoyable synchronous interaction
and serve as a source of comfort. Cultural factors
and larger community context are one area in
which the confluence of attachment theory
290
291
a family systems impact through the contribution to the family system as a co-provider
and protector. This adds stability and sense of
safety (safe haven) to the family environment.
This combination of influences supports secure
attachment processes for mothers, fathers, and
the family unit.
This conceptualization uses attachment theory to help define important goal areas for
fathers and a focus for parent education activities. It also accommodates the idea that fathers
may have different pathways to attachment and
at the same time should be aware of the roles that
temperament and child gender play as mediators
of attachment processes. Fathers pathways can
include both sensitivity in caregiving and stimulating play to support exploration. Fathers also
learn that a positive coparenting relationship
plays an important role in developing a secure
fatherchild relationship and a secure family
attachment system.
The Circle of Security parenting program
(Cooper et al, 2005) is a useful model for parent
education and was developed primarily with
mothers in clinical settings. I was recently
trained in this model and have had the opportunity to use it with groups of incarcerated fathers
over the past 2 years. The model depicts attachment theory concepts in the Circle of Security
diagram (Cooper et al., 2005), which gives
equal attention to the secure base that supports
exploration and autonomy and the safe haven
that offers comfort and assistance to a child in
distress to help regulate his or her emotions.
This model makes explicit the specific roles and
behaviors that parents can practice to support
secure attachment in young children. It also provides parents with insight into their own state of
mind (Powell et al., 2007) by introducing them
to the concept of shark music, which alerts them
to recognize times when they feel challenged or
unable to meet their childs needs. Palm (2013)
adapted the Circle of Security program and
integrated it into a program for incarcerated
fathers. The program evaluation, using father
reports of changes, demonstrated significant
changes related to attachment behaviors, including understanding the importance of being
with a child and understanding shark music as
a concept that may influence interactions with
their child.
A recent study by Benzies, Magill-Evans,
Harrison, Gleri, and Kimak (2006) evaluating a brief video-feedback and home-visiting
292
program with fathers demonstrated a positive
effect on fathers interactional behavior. Fathers
were more sensitive to infant cues and better at
fostering childrens cognitive growth. Feldman
(2012a) also recommended video feedback
as an effective technique for increasing both
sensitivity and synchrony for parents. Video
feedback appears to be a tool for increasing
fathers awareness of infant cues and offering
opportunities to reflect on when and why synchrony is occurring in their interactions with
their children.
My work as a practitioner over more than 30
years in a variety of settings (e.g., schools, Head
Start programs, hospitals, correctional facilities)
has focused on trying to facilitate mens quest to
develop a close relationship with their children
ages 0 to 5. Attachment theory and the concept
of secure attachment have been useful in discerning pathways to the types of relationships
fathers want to develop with their children. This
work has focused on some of the differences that
fathers bring to the parentchild relationship
(Fagan & Palm, 2004; Palm, 1997). These differences have been based on innate dispositions
(Rossi, 1984) reinforced by male socialization processes. These include differences in
sensitivity to visual, nonverbal, and olfactory
cues that might explain some of the different
research findings related to gender differences in
sensitivity of mothers versus fathers and attachment patterns (DeWolff & Van IJzendoorn,
1997). The field of infant mental health has
used attachment theory to more clearly define
a role for fathers in promoting socio-emotional
development and emotional regulation. The
Circle of Security model of attachment has
brought clarity to working with fathers and to
identifying some of fathers unique struggles
with the attachment process. In my experience,
fathers seem to struggle more with being a safe
haven when children are in distress. It is easy for
fathers to miscue and give the message to children that they need to figure it out on their own.
This is related to a strong male socialization
message of asking for help being a sign of weakness. Being a secure base to support and even
enhance or stretch a child in exploration appears
to be a role that fits with male socialization and
the traditional role of fathers to be risk taker,
protector, and a connection with the outside
world.
As a scholar and a consumer of fathering
research over the past 30 years, I believe that
293
for several years (e.g., Shonkoff & Phillips,
2000). The research on fathers and hormones
(Naber, Poslawsky, van IJzendoorn, van Engeland, & Bakermans-Kranenburg, 2013) links the
introduction of the hormone oxytocin to fathers
increased sensitivity to toddlers in a play situation. Feldman (2012a) reports that oxytocin is
related in different ways to mothers and fathers
at 6 months. For fathers oxytocin is related
to stimulatory touch, encouragement of exploration, and high positive arousal. For mothers, oxytocin is related to affectionate parenting behaviors. Feldman (2012b) distinguishes
synchronous behavior in fathers as being jerky,
with high arousal and more random contour than
mothers more rhythmic contour. This connection of biochemical indicators to behavioral patterns describes mothers and fathers such that
hormones play a role in supporting parenting
sensitivity, but behaviors in fathers and mothers
are different, leading through different pathways
to secure attachment.
Hughes and Baylin (2012) have translated
parentchild relationships (stress, emotional
regulation, and early social emotional development) into brain-based explanations to guide
practice. They have identified five domains of
parenting that are mapped onto brain activity to describe how the attachment process
functions in our heads. This model provides
new understanding of how different parenting
processes work to support effective parenting
and lead to secure attachment relations. The
five systems are (a) the parental approach system, or the ability to feel comfortable with the
childs bids for attention; (b) the parental reward
system, or the ability to experience interacting
with ones child as pleasurable, satisfying, and
rewarding; (c) the child-reading system, or the
ability to understand and read the childs emotional cues (sensitivity to cues); (d) the parental
meaning-making system, or the ability to create
a coherent personal narrative (reflect on own
state of mind); and (e) the parental executive
system, or the ability to regulate internal state
(emotions), monitor parentchild connection
(synchrony), and engage in repair as needed.
These are helpful because they describe specific
skill areas to focus on in order to support secure
fatherchild attachment relationships. These
concepts provide new insights into fatherchild
attachment dynamics that are mirrored in brain
activity and can help practitioners set specific
goals.
294
295
Gonzalez, A., Atkinson, L., & Fleming, A. S. (2009).
Attachment and comparative psychobiology of
mothering. In M. De Haan & M. E. Gunnar (Eds.).
Handbook of developmental social neuroscience
(pp. 225245). New York, NY: Guilford.
Grossmann, K., Grossmann, K. E., Fremmer-Bombik,
E., Kindler, H., Scheuerer-Englisch, H., & Zimmermann, P. (2002). The uniqueness of the
childfather attachment relationship: Fathers sensitive and challenging play as a pivotal variable in
a 16-year longitudinal study. Social Development,
11, 307331.
Grossmann, K. E., Grossman, K., & Waters, E.
(2005). Attachment from infancy to adulthood:
The major longitudinal studies. New York, NY:
Guilford Press.
Hazen, N. L., McFarland, L., Jacobvitz, D., &
Boyd-Soisson, E. (2011). Fathers frightening
behaviours and sensitivity with infants: Relations with fathers attachment representations,
fatherinfant attachment, and childrens later outcomes. In L. A. Newland, H. S. Freeman, & D. D.
Coyl (Eds.), Emerging topics on father attachment
(pp. 5068). New York, NY: Routledge
Hoffman, J. (2011) Father factors: What social
science research tells us about fathers and
how to work with them. Father Involvement Research Alliance at the University of
Guelph, Canada. Retrieved from http://www.
fira.ca/cms/documents/211/FatherFactorsFinal.pdf
Hughes, D. A., & Baylin, J. (2012). Brain-based parenting: The neuroscience of caregiving for healthy
attachment. New York, NY: Norton.
Isabella, R. A., & Belsky, J. (1991). Interactional synchrony and the origins of infantmother attachment: A replication study. Child Development, 62,
373384.
Karen, R. (1998). Becoming attached. New York, NY:
Oxford University Press.
Lamb, M. E. (1976a). The role of the father in child
development. New York, NY: Wiley.
Lamb, M. E. (1976b). Twelve-month olds and their
parents: Interactions in a laboratory playroom.
Developmental Psychology, 12, 237244.
Lamb, M. E. (1977a). The development of
motherinfant and fatherinfant interactions in the
second year of life. Developmental Psychology,
13, 637648.
Lamb, M. E. (1977b). Motherinfant and
fatherinfant interaction in the first year of
life. Child Development, 48, 167181.
Lamb, M. E. (1978). Qualitative aspects of mother
infant and fatherinfant attachments in the second
year of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 1,
265275.
Lamb, M. E. (2012). A wasted opportunity to engage
with the literature on the implications of attachment research for family court professionals. Family Court Review, 50(3), 481485.
296
Lamb, M. E., & Lewis, C. (2010). The development
and significance of fatherchild relationships in
two parent families. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The role
of the father in child development (5th ed., pp.
94153). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Lamb, M. E., & Lewis, C. (2011). The role of
parentchild relationships in child development. In
M. H. Bornstein & M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Developmental science (6th ed., pp. 469518). New York,
NY: Psychology Press.
Lucassen, N., Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Volling, B.
L., Thamer, A., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.
J., Verhulst, F. C., & Tiemeier, H. (2011). The
association between paternal sensitivity and
infantfather attachment security: A meta-analysis
of three decades of research. Journal of Family
Psychology, 25(6), 986992.
Lundy, B. L. (2002). Paternal socio-psychological
factors and infant attachment: The mediating role
of synchrony in fatherinfant interactions. Infant
Behavior and Development, 25, 221236.
Lundy, B. L. (2003) Fatherinfant and motherinfant
face-to-face
interactions:
Difference
in
mind-related comments and infant attachment?
Infant Behavior and Development, 26, 200212.
Madigan, S., Benoit, D., & Boucher, C. (2011).
Exploration of the links among fathers unresolved
states of mind with respect to attachment, atypical
paternal behavior, and disorganized infantfather
attachment. Infant Mental Health Journal, 32(3),
286304.
Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1990). Procedure for
identifying infants as disorganized/disoriented
during the Ainsworth strange situation. In M.
T. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, & M. E. Cummings
(Eds.), Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research, and intervention (pp. 121160).
Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Main, M., & Weston, D. R. (1981). The quality of
the toddlers relationship to mother and father,
related to conflict behavior and the readiness to
establish new relationships. Child Development,
52, 932940.
Mayes, L. C., Magidson, J., Lejuez, C. W., &
Nicholls, S. S. (2009). Social relationships as primary rewards: The neurobiology of attachment. In
M. De Haan & M. R. Gunnar (Eds.), Handbook of
developmental social neuroscience (pp. 342377).
New York, NY: Guilford Press.
McBride, B. A., & Lutz, M. M. (2004) Intervention:
Changing the nature and extent of father involvement. In M. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in
child development (4th ed., pp. 446475). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
McHale, J. (2007). Charting the bumpy road of coparenthood. Washington, DC: Zero to Three Press.
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2012). Adult attachment orientations and relationship processes. Journal of Family Theory & Review, 4, 250274.
297
Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., Carlson, E. A., & Collins,
W. A. (2005). The development of the person. New
York, NY: Guilford.
Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1995). Adult attachment
representations, parental responsiveness and
infant attachment: A meta-analysis on the predictive validity of the Adult Attachment Interview.
Psychological Bulletin, 117, 117.
Van IJzendoorn, M. H., & DeWolff, M. (1997). In
search of the absent fathers: Meta-analysis of
infantfather behavior. Child Development, 68,
604609.
Van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Kroonenberg, P. M.
(1988). Cross-cultural patterns of attachment:
A meta-analysis of the strange situation. Child
Development, 59(1), 147156.
Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Schuengel, C., &
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (1999). Disorganized attachment in early childhood: Meta-analysis
of precursors, concomitants, and sequelae. Development and Psychopathology, 11, 225249.
Wong, M. S., Mangelsdorf, S. C., Brown, G. L., Neff,
C., & Schoppe-Sullivan, S. J. (2009). Parental
beliefs, infant temperament, and marital quality:
Associations with infantmother and infantfather
attachment. Journal of Family Psychology, 23,
828838. doi:10.1037/a0016491