Posture and Movement in Healthy Preterm Infants in Supine Position in and Outside The Nest
Posture and Movement in Healthy Preterm Infants in Supine Position in and Outside The Nest
Posture and Movement in Healthy Preterm Infants in Supine Position in and Outside The Nest
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Objective: To evaluate whether lying in a nest affects the posture and spontaneous movements of healthy
preterm infants.
Method: 10 healthy preterm infants underwent serial video recording in the supine position, when lying in a
nest and outside it, at three ages: 3033 weeks postmenstrual age (PMA) (early preterm), 3436 weeks PMA
(late preterm) and 3740 weeks PMA (term). The nest was shell-shaped, made by putting two rolled blankets
in a form of an oval. Posture was assessed both before and after general movements by scoring the
predominant postural pattern. Movements towards and across the midline, elegant wrist movements, abrupt
hand and/or limb movements, rolling to side, and frozen postures of the arms and legs were assessed during
four general movements. All data relating to motor and postural items were normalised into frequencies of
events per minute because the general movements varied in duration.
Results: When lying in the nest, the infants more often displayed a flexed posture with shoulder adduction and
elbow, and hip and knee flexion, and the head was frequently in the midline. The nest was also associated
with an increase in elegant wrist movements and movements towards and across the midline and a reduction
in abrupt movements and frozen postures of the limbs. The nest did not affect the occurrence of asymmetrical
tonic neck posture.
Conclusions: A nest promotes a flexed posture of the limbs with adduction of shoulders, facilitates elegant
wrist movements and movements towards and across the midline and reduces abrupt movements and frozen
postures of the arms and legs.
F387
Five infants were excluded from the study because at the age of
2 years they showed an atypical developmental outcome as
determined by Griffiths scales of development19 and Touwens
neurological examination.20
All the 10 infants who participated in the study needed
respiratory support. One of them needed intermittent positive
pressure ventilation (IPPV) for six days, six infants required
IPPVintermittent mandatory ventilation for one to three days
and one needed high-flow oxygen for five days. All infants were
given continuous positive airway pressure (CPAP) for a few
days. Ultrasound scans, serially performed from birth until
term age, showed normal findings in six infants and transient
increased echogenicity lasting less than four days in three
infants; one infant had GMH-IVH (germinal matrix-intraventricular haemorrhage) grade 1.21
Figure 1 Infant in the nest: lying in the nest facilitates arm and hand
movement during which various parts of the body are touched, and
promotes flexion of arms and legs, and adduction of shoulders and hips.
Parental/guardian informed consent was obtained for publication of this
figure.
Method
All infants underwent three video recordings, each lasting an
hour. The recordings were done at 3033 weeks postmenstrual
age (PMA) (early preterm age), 3436 weeks PMA (late
preterm age) and 3740 weeks PMA (term age). The first video
recording was done when the infants clinical condition was
reasonably stable and respiratory support was no longer
necessary. The video recordings were done in a standardised
orderthat is, half an hour in the nest and half an hour outside
the nest, in an incubator or in a cot, in supine position, naked or
in a nappy, completely free to move.22 One infant missed one
early preterm age video recording.
The nest is oval-shaped and made of cloth (we use two
rolled-up blankets), in which the infant lies when inside the
incubator (fig 1). The infant is placed in the nest to facilitate a
semiflexed and adducted position of the shoulders and hips and
to reduce environmental stimuli. The postural support provided
by the nest is in accordance with the strategy of individualised
developmental care in the NICU.23 We placed a small roll under
the infants neck to help the proper alignment of the neck and
trunk. This was done mainly to provide an optimal position for
breathing.
Our analysis of the videos focused on motor behaviour during
activity periodsthat is periods with repeated general movements and other non-sporadic movement patterns.16 General
movements are gross movements involving the whole body,
lasting from a few seconds to several minutes.22 Four general
movements were randomly selected from each video. The
movement analysis focused on motor behaviour immediately
before, during and immediately after the four general movements. We took care that the posture after one general
movement did not overlap with the posture prior to another
general movement.
We analysed posture at rest immediately before a general
movement started and immediately after this general movement ended. The postural items analysed are given in appendix
A and were designed for the present study. A posture was
considered to be a posture (and no longer a movement) when it
lasted for at least 10 s. When evaluating the position of the
head, we used a simple dichotomy: midline position versus
rotated to either the right or the left side (2070). On the basis
of the four postures before and after a general movement, the
predominant postural pattern before and after general movements was determined. If two patterns were observed equally
often, the more flexed, adducted or midline pattern was
scoredthat is, for the elbows, hips and knees the flexed
posture, for the shoulders the adducted posture, for the head
the midline position, and absence rather than presence of the
asymmetrical tonic neck posture. We also evaluated the four
general movements. In the analysis of spontaneous motility we
focused on the items listed in appendix B.24
www.archdischild.com
F388
Term age
(3740 weeks)
RESULTS
Lying in a nest had a clear effect on the infants postural
behaviour before and after a general movement (table 1).
Before a general movement, an infants shoulders were more
adducted and limbs somewhat more flexed in the nest than
outside the nest. After a general movement, lying in a nest was
particularly associated with an increase in limb flexion, and at
term age, also with an increase in shoulder adduction and head
in midline position. Interestingly, the asymmetrical tonic neck
posture was never a predominant posture in these relatively
healthy preterm infants.
Lying in a nest also had a substantial effect on spontaneous
motor behaviour. Throughout the early and late preterm and
term periods lying in a nest increased the number of
www.archdischild.com
DISCUSSION
The present study has shown that placing an infant in a nest
facilitates a flexed and adducted posture, promotes elegant
wrist movements and movements towards or crossing the
midline, and it reduces abrupt movements and frozen postures.
The strength of the present study was the standardised and
detailed assessment of the effect of lying in a nest in relatively
healthy preterm infants. Each infant acted as its own control.
There were some weaknesses of this explorative study. First,
the infants, for the rest of their stay in hospital, were placed in
the nest. This meant that when they were taken out of the nest
to be studied, they experienced an abrupt interruption of the
environment and posture to which they had got used to.
However, we could not overcome this limitation because of the
strong opinion of the NICU nursing staff regarding the
beneficial effect of the nest. A firm belief in a method, despite
F389
N
N
N
N
N
N
Figure 3 Upper panel: effect of lying in a nest on occurrence of frozen
postures of arms and legs. ARM-FR-FL, arms in frozen flexion and fisting;
LEGS-EXT, legs in frozen extension. Lower panel: effect of lying in a nest on
the occurrence of abrupt movements. ABD-EXT, abrupt abduction
extension movements of the arms; Open hand, abrupt opening of hands
and fingers; Startle, abrupt abductionextension of the four limbs; Rolling,
abrupt rolling to side. *p,0.05, **p,0.01 (Wilcoxons signed rank test).
during the early preterm age, the nest did not affect head
position, but from late preterm onwards it did;
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We are grateful to Quinta Bergman and Manuela Corradini for their
help in data collection.
.......................
Authors affiliations
F Ferrari, N Bertoncelli, C Gallo, M F Roversi, M P Guerra, A Ranzi,
Department of Pediatrics and Neonatology of Modena University, Modena
University Hospital, Modena, Italy
M Hadders-Algra, Department of NeurologyDevelopmental Neurology,
University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, the Netherlands
Parental/guardian informed consent was obtained for publication of fig 1.
Competing interests: None.
www.archdischild.com
F390
REFERENCES
1 Casaer P. Postural behaviour in newborn infants. Clinics in Developmental
Medicine, no. 72. London: Heinemann Medical, 1979.
2 Kernell D. The final common pathway in postural controldevelopmental
perspective. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1998;22:47984.
3 De Groot L. Posture and motility in preterm infants. Dev Med Child Neurol
2000;42:658.
4 Ververs IA, Van Gelder-Hasker MR, De Vries JI, et al. Prenatal development of
arm posture. Early Hum Dev 1998;51:6170.
5 Als H, Lawhon G, Duffy HF, et al. Individualized developmental care for the very
low-birth-weight preterm infant: medical and neurofunctional effects. JAMA
1994;272:8538.
6 Dubowitz LMS, Dubowitz V, Mercuri E. The neurological assessment of the
preterm and full-term newborn infant, 2nd edn. Clinics in Developmental
Medicine, no. 148. Cambridge: MacKeith Press, 1999.
7 Ferrari F, Roversi MF. Fattori di rischio pre, peri e neonatali e la care del neonato
pretermine in Terapia Intensiva Neonatale. Riabilitazione Oggi 1998;8:714.
8 Cuttini M, Maraschini A, Greisen G, et al. Developmental care for preterm
neonates: a survey of practices in European neonatal units. Book of Abstracts,
European Society Paediatric Research, October 2006, Barcelona.
9 Fleisher BE, VandenBerg K, Constantinou J, et al. Individualized developmental
care for very low-birth-weight premature infants. Clin Pediatr 1995;34:5239.
10 Westrup B, Kleberg A, von Eichwald K, et al. A randomized controlled trial to
evaluate the effects of the newborn individualized developmental care and
assessment program in a Swedish setting. Pediatrics 2000;105:6672.
11 Young J. Positioning premature babies. Nursing preterm babies in intensive care.
Which position is the best? J Neonatal Nurs 1994;1:2731.
12 Prechtl HFR. Qualitative changes of spontaneous movements in fetus and preterm
infant are a marker of neurological dysfunction. Early Hum Dev 1990;23:1518.
13 Prechtl HFR. The neurological examination of the full-term newborn infant, 2nd edn.
Clinics in Developmental Medicine, no. 63. London: Heinemann Medical, 1977.
14 Ferrari F, Cioni G, Prechtl HFR. Qualitative changes of general movements in
preterm infants with brain lesions. Early Hum Dev 1990;23:193231.
15 Eken T, Hultborn H, Kiehn O. Possible functions of transmitter-controlled plateau
potentials in alpha motoneurones. Prog Brain Res 1989;80:25767.
16 Hadders-Algra M, Van Eykern LA, Klip-van den Nieuwendijk AWJ, et al.
Developmental course of general movements in early infancy. II. EMG correlates.
Early Hum Dev 1992;28:23152.
17 National Center for Health Statistics. NCHS growth charts. Mon Vital Stat Rep
1976;25(Suppl3)(HRA):76112.
18 Prechtl HFR. Neurological findings after pre- and paranatal complications. In:
Jonxis JHP, Visser HKA, Troelstra JA, eds. Aspects of Praematurity and
Dysmaturity. Leiden: Stenfert Kroese, 1968:30321.
19 Griffith R. The abilities of young children. A comprehensive system of mental
measurement for the first 8 years of life. London: Child Development Research
Centre, Young and Son, 1970.
20 Touwen BCL. Neurological development in infancy. Clinics in Developmental
Medicine, no. 58. Philadelphia: Lippincott, 1976.
21 Trounce JQ, Rutter N, Levene MI. A prospective study of the incidence of
periventricular leukomalacia and ventricular haemorrhage in the preterm
neonate. Arch Dis Child 1986;61:1196202.
22 Einspieler C, Prechtl HFR, Ferrari F, et al. The qualitative assessment of general
movements in preterm, term and young infantsreview of the methodology.
Early Hum Dev 1997;50:4760.
23 Bauer K. Interventions involving positioning and handling in the neonatal
intensive care unit: early developmental care and skin-to-skin holding. Research
on Early Developmental Care for preterm neonates. Paris: John Libbey Eurotext,
2005:5965.
24 Ferrari F, Bertoncelli N, Roversi MF, et al. Motor and postural behavior in lowrisk preterm infants from 3033 to 4654 weeks postmenstrual age: an
observational study. Prenat Neonat Med 2001;6:16683.
25 Westrup B, Bohm B, Lagercrantz H, et al. Preschool outcome in children born very
preterm and cared according to NIDCAP. Acta Paediatr 2004;93:498507.
26 Stevens B, Petryshen P, Hawkins J, et al. Developmental versus conventional
care: a comparison of clinical outcomes for very low birth weight infants.
Can J Nurs Res 1996;28:97113.
27 Brown LD, Heermann JA. The effect of developmental care on preterm infant
outcome. Appl Nurs Res 1997;10:1907.
28 Symington A, Pinelli J. Developmental care for promoting development and
preventing morbidity in preterm infants. Cochrane Database Syst Rev
2006;2:CD001814.
29 Slevin M, Murphy JFA, Daly L, et al. Retinopathy of prematurity screening, stress
related responses, the role of nesting. Br J Ophthalmol 1997;81:7624.
30 De Graaf-Peters VB, De Groot-Hornstra AH, Dirks T, et al. Specific postural
support promotes variation in motor behaviour of infants with minor neurological
dysfunction. Dev Med Child Neurol 2006;48:96672.
31 Hadders-Algra M, Klip-Van den Nieuwendijk AWJ, Martijn A, et al. Assessment
of general movements: towards a better understanding of a sensitive method to
evaluate brain function in young infants. Dev Med Child Neurol 1997;39:8898.
32 Woollacott M, Sveistrup H. The development of sensorimotor integration underlying
posture control in infants during the transition to independence stance. In:
Swinnen SP, Massion J, Heuer H et al, eds. Interlimb coordination:neural dynamical
and cognitive constraints. San Diego, CA: Academic Presss, 1994:37189.
33 Hadders-Algra M. The Neuronal Group Selection Theory: an attractive
framework to explain variation in normal motor development. Dev Med Child
Neurol 2000;42:56672.
34 Blauw-Hospers CH, Hadders-Algra M. A systematic review on the effects of early
intervention on motor development. Dev Med Child Neurol 2005;47:42132.
www.archdischild.com
APPENDIX A
Items in the assessment of posture
Item
Description
0 = adduction
1 = neutral
2 = abduction
0 = flexion
1 = semiflexion
2 = extension
0 = adduction
1 = neutral
2 = abduction
0 = flexion
1 = semiflexion
2 = extension
0 = flexion
1 = semiflexion
2 = extension
0 = flexion
1 = semiflexion
2 = extension
0 = flexion
1 = semiflexion
2 = extension
0 = flexion
1 = semiflexion
2 = extension
= absent
= present
0 = head in the midline
1 = head to side
(9) Asymmetrical
tonic neck posture
(10) Head position
020
2190
.90
030
31150
.150
020
2190
.90
030
31150
.150
080
81150
.150
060
61150
.150
080
81150
.150
060
61150
.150
Rotated ,20
Rotated 2070
APPENDIX B
Items in the assessment of details of general movements
and spontaneous movements
Movements
towards and
across the
midline
(1) Head
rotation from
side to
midline and
back
(2) Head
rotation from
side to side
(3) Hand
mouth contact
(4) Handhead
contact
(5) Gently
striking head
with open
hands
(6) Handhand
contact
(7) Hands
touching
contralateral
shoulder and
trunk
(8) Handleg
contact
(9) Footfoot
contact
Abrupt hand
and/or limb
movements
Frozen postures
Elegant wrist and
of arms and
movements
rolling to side legs
(1) Wrist
movements with
superimposed
rotations
(1) Abrupt
(1) Arms in
opening
frozen extension
of hands and
fingers
(2) Abrupt
abduction
extension of
the arms
(3) Abrupt
abduction
extension of
the four limbs
(4) Abrupt
rolling to side
(2) Arms in
frozen flexion
and fisting
(3) Legs in
frozen extension