Joint Effects of Emotion and Color On Memory: Brief Report
Joint Effects of Emotion and Color On Memory: Brief Report
Joint Effects of Emotion and Color On Memory: Brief Report
BRIEF REPORT
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
University of Munich
Numerous studies have shown that memory is enhanced for emotionally negative and positive information relative to neutral information. We examined whether emotion-induced memory enhancement is
influenced by low-level perceptual attributes such as color. Because in everyday life red is often used as
a warning signal, whereas green signals security, we hypothesized that red might enhance memory for
negative information and green memory for positive information. To capture the signaling function of
colors, we measured memory for words standing out from the context by color, and manipulated the color
and emotional significance of the outstanding words. Making words outstanding by color strongly
enhanced memory, replicating the well-known von Restorff effect. Furthermore, memory for colored
words was further increased by emotional significance, replicating the memory-enhancing effect of
emotion. Most intriguingly, the effects of emotion on memory additionally depended on color type. Red
strongly increased memory for negative words, whereas green strongly increased memory for positive
words. These findings provide the first evidence that emotion-induced memory enhancement is influenced by color and demonstrate that different colors can have different functions in human memory.
Keywords: emotion, color, memory, von Restorff effect, isolation effect
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
376
Figure 1. Example of one experimental trial. (A) Participants were presented sequences of 12 words containing
11 neutral black words and 1 red, green, or blue colored word that was either emotionally neutral, negative, or
positive. (B) Memory performance as a function of serial position and color of the outstanding word for neutral
(left panel), negative (middle panel), and positive words (right panel). Note that the outstanding word was
actually randomly presented at serial position 5, 6, 7, or 8 (shaded in gray); the black baselines show memory
performance for black neutral words presented at the same serial positions.
von Restorff effect. Moreover, we expected that emotional significance of the perceptually outstanding words would further increase the von Restorff effect. Such a combined effect of color and
emotional significance would demonstrate that emotional salience
can enhance memory beyond the memory-enhancing effect of
perceptual salience. However, if red and green differentially influence memory for negative and positive information, then there
should be an interaction between the effects of color and emotional
salience. Negative words should be remembered best when colored
red, whereas positive words should be remembered best when
colored green.
Method
Participants
Forty-eight undergraduate students (42 females, mean age
22.6 years, SD 4.2) at a German university participated for
course credit. All participants were tested individually and were
not red green colorblind.
The colored word of a list was randomly presented at the 5th, 6th,
7th, or 8th serial position, and the mean memory performance for
black words across these positions was used as a baseline for the
colored words. The remaining three lists contained only black
words to include an additional control condition. Memory performance for black words presented at the 5th, 6th, 7th, or 8th serial
position did not differ between lists containing one colored word
and lists containing only black words, t(47) 0.89, p .376.
All words were five- to six-letter German nouns. The black words
were randomly drawn from a pool of 333 neutral words (taken
from the CELEX database using WordGen v1.0 software toolbox;
Duyck, Desmet, Verbeke, & Brysbaert, 2004). The colored words
were randomly drawn from a pool of 27 words that differed in
emotional valence (see the Appendix). Nine of the colored words
were negative, nine were positive, and nine were neutral. The three
types of colored words were matched on word length and word
frequency (Google Ngram Viewer; Michel et al., 2011).
Three of the words of an emotional type were presented in red,
three in green, and three in blue. The assignment of colors to these
words was counterbalanced across participants. The colorimetric
properties of the three colors were chosen using a spectrometer
(i1Pro, X-rite Inc., Grandville, Michigan) according to the LCh
color model. In order to account for possible confounding effects
of lightness and chroma, red, green, and blue hues were selected
that were equal on the parameters L and C (red: LCh[47.0, 63.1,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Results
Emotional Ratings
As indicated by the ratings of the participants, the three emotional types of colored items varied significantly in their emotional
ratings. Compared with neutral words (M 3.17), negative words
were rated much more unpleasant (M 1.31), t(47) 32.18, p
.001, d 4.85,1 and positive words much more pleasant (M
4.66), t(47) 33.27, p .001, d 5.03. This difference in
valence was confirmed by the ratings of the independent judges.
Compared with neutral words (M 3.91), negative words were
rated much more unpleasant (M 1.32), t(7) 25.64, p .001,
d 9.38, and positive words much more pleasant (M 6.44),
t(7) 16.79, p .001, d 6.00. The arousal ratings of the
independent judges revealed that both negative words (M 5.68)
and positive words (M 6.03) were rated as more arousing than
neutral words (M 2.72), t(7) 6.84, p .001, d 2.45, and
t(7) 9.67, p .001, d 3.92, respectively. Arousal did not
differ between negative and positive words, t(7) 1.52, p
.172, d 0.60.
377
and both negative and positive words were better remembered than
neutral words, t(47) 5.84, p .001, d 0.84, and t(47) 5.54,
p .001, d 0.80, respectively. However, these main effects
were qualified by a significant Color Type Emotional Valence
interaction, F(4, 188) 5.38, p .001, p2 .10. Whereas
memory for neutral words was not influenced by color, F(2, 94)
1.41, p .25, memory for negative words was boosted by red
color, compared with green color, t(47) 4.08, p .001, d
0.59, and blue color, t(47) 3.16, p .003, d 0.46. By contrast,
memory for positive words was boosted by green color, compared
with red color, t(47) 2.68, p .010, d 0.39, and blue color,
t(47) 3.44, p .001, d 0.50.
Discussion
The present findings demonstrate that emotion-induced memory
enhancement can be influenced by color. We employed a typical
von Restorff paradigm in which participants memory for words
standing out from the context by color was examined. Replicating
the von Restorff effect, we found that memory for colored words
was strongly enhanced. Furthermore, in line with previous findings
documenting emotion-induced memory enhancement, our results
showed that memory for colored words was further increased by
emotional significance. Both negative and positive words were
better remembered than neutral words. Finally, most intriguingly,
our findings demonstrate that the memory-enhancing effect of
emotional significance is differentially influenced by different
colors. Red strongly enhanced memory for negative words,
whereas green strongly enhanced memory for positive words.
This pattern of results mirrors the conventional use of red and
green in everyday life to signal that a situation is potentially
threatening or beneficial (Travis, 1991). Accordingly, based on
societal learning, red and green might function as implicit cues
alerting the perceiver to potential dangers or benefits, which might,
in turn, intensify the impact of negative, respectively positive,
information (Bower, 1981). Indeed, recent findings demonstrate
that viewing red can have effects that are typically observed in
threatening situations, such as a decreased heart rate variability
(Elliot, Payen, Brisswalter, Cury, & Thayer, 2011) or an enhanced
force and velocity of motor output (Elliot & Aarts, 2011). As most
of the recent studies on the effects of red on psychological functioning have used green only sometimes as a control color, conclusive evidence concerning the effects of green is lacking. However, a few studies on physiological reactions to different colors
suggest that green is associated with pleasant, low-arousal affective states (e.g., Jacobs & Hustmyer, 1974; Wilson, 1966), although these studiesin contrast to recent studies on the effects of
red did not control for confounding effects of lightness or
chroma.
The idea that colors having specific emotional connotations can
influence emotional processing is also supported by previous studies on emotional valence judgments. For instance, it has been
shown that valence judgments are speeded up when negative
words are presented in black, and positive words in white, a pattern
reflecting the tendency of people to view dark colors as negative
and light colors as positive (Meier, Robinson, & Clore, 2004;
1
To account for repeated measures, all reported effect sizes were calculated using Morris and DeShons (2002) Equation 8.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
378
References
Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36,
129 148. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.36.2.129
Depue, B. E., Banich, M. T., & Curran, T. (2006). Suppression of emotional and nonemotional content in memory. Psychological Science, 17,
441 447. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01725.x
Duyck, W., Desmet, T., Verbeke, L. P. C., & Brysbaert, M. (2004).
WordGen: A tool for word selection and nonword generation in Dutch,
English, German, and French. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments
& Computers, 36, 488 499. doi:10.3758/BF03195595
Elliot, A. J., & Aarts, H. (2011). Perception of the color red enhances the
force and velocity of motor output. Emotion, 11, 445 449. doi:10.1037/
a0022599
Elliot, A. J., Maier, M. A., Moller, A. C., Friedman, R., & Meinhardt, J.
(2007). Color and psychological functioning: The effect of red on
performance attainment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,
136, 154 168. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.136.1.154
Elliot, A. J., & Niesta, D. (2008). Romantic red: Red enhances mens
attraction to women. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95,
1150 1164. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.95.5.1150
Elliot, A. J., Payen, V., Brisswalter, J., Cury, F., & Thayer, J. F. (2011). A
subtle threat cue, heart rate variability, and cognitive performance.
Psychophysiology, 48, 1340 1345. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011
.01216.x
Hamann, S. (2001). Cognitive and neural mechanisms of emotional memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 394 400. doi:10.1016/S13646613(00)01707-1
Hunt, R. R. (1995). The subtlety of distinctiveness: What von Restorff
really did. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 105112. doi:10.3758/
BF03214414
Jacobs, K. W., & Hustmyer, F. E. (1974). Effects of four psychological
primary colors on gsr, heart rate, and respiration rate. Perceptual and
Motor Skills, 38, 763766.
Khan, S. A., Levine, W. J., Dobson, S. D., & Kralik, J. D. (2011). Red
signals dominance in male rhesus macaques. Psychological Science, 22,
10011003. doi:10.1177/0956797611415543
Meier, B. P., Robinson, M. D., & Clore, G. L. (2004). Why good guys wear
white. Psychological Science, 15, 82 87. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004
.01502002.x
Michel, J. B., Shen, Y. K., Aiden, A. P., Veres, A., Gray, M. K., The
Google Books Team, . . . Aiden, E. L. (2011). Quantitative analysis of
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
culture using millions of digitized books. Science, 331, 176 182. doi:
10.1126/science.1199644
Moller, A. C., Elliot, A. J., & Maier, M. A. (2009). Basic hue-meaning
associations. Emotion, 9, 898 902. doi:10.1037/a0017811
Morris, S. B., & DeShon, R. P. (2002). Combining effect size estimates in
meta-analysis with repeated measures and independent-group designs.
Psychological Methods, 7, 105125. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.7.1.105
Murray, B. D., Muscatell, K. A., & Kensinger, E. A. (2011). Effects of
emotion and age on performance during a think/no-think memory task.
Psychology and Aging, 26, 940 955. doi:10.1037/a0023214
Okubo, M., & Ishikawa, K. (2011). Automatic semantic association between
emotional valence and brightness in the right hemisphere. Cognition and
Emotion, 25, 12731280. doi:10.1080/02699931.2010.541658
379
Appendix
English Translation of the Original German Emotional Words
Neutral
Negative
Positive
Circle
Eagle
Fabric
Foil
Kiosk
Pen
Sign
Table
Crisis
Fear
Jail
Misery
Nazi
Tumor
War
Weapon
Friend
Fun
Holiday
Kisses
Laugh
Luck
Party
Winner
Note. In German language, the word length of all words ranged from five to six letters.