Spanish-Influenced English: Typical Phonological Patterns in The English Language Learner

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 16

Spanish-influenced English: Typical phonological patterns in the

English language learner


Brenda K. Gorman
and
Ellen Stubbe Kester
This course is offered by Bilinguistics, an ASHA Approved CE Provider.
Content Area: Basic Communication Processes
Instructional Level: Basic
Continuing Education Units: .1 (1 hour)


Objectives:
Participants will be able to:
Describe basic principles of the Competition Model and its application to second language
acquisition.
Identify similarities and differences between the phonological systems of Spanish and English.
Recognize common articulation patterns of sequential bilingual (Spanish/English) children
while they are acquiring English
Differentiate between normal articulation patterns and signs of speech impairments



Page 2
Introduction
According to the National Clearinghouse for English Language Acquisition & Language
Instruction Educational Programs (Kindler, 2002), more than 1 in 10 children in elementary school
were classified as Limited English Proficient (LEP) during the 2000-2001 school year. Spanish
was the primary language of 79% of students with limited English proficiency. With the shortage
of bilingual professionals, increasing numbers of monolingual SLPs are realizing the need to learn
more about bilingual speech and language development to enhance their ability to provide equitable
and appropriate services to their bilingual students. In response to this need, the first purpose of
this paper is to review the current data about the phonological development of children who are
bilingual in Spanish and English. Because there are so little data to date, the second purpose of this
paper is to present a conceptual framework of bilingual development that will help SLPs
distinguish between typical and atypical phonological patterns of Spanish-speaking children
learning English.
While normative information on the phonological development of monolingual English
speakers is readily available, there are less data from monolingual Spanish speakers, and minimal
published data from bilingual (Spanish/English) children. One reason information is lacking about
bilingual speech development is that no two bilinguals are alike. There is a great deal of variation
in the ages at which children learn each language, the amount of exposure to each language, and the
contexts in which they learn each language (Iglesias & Goldstein, 1998). The vast number of
variables involved in bilingual speech acquisition makes it difficult to identify consistent patterns of
acquisition in bilinguals. One important finding, however, is that bilingual childrens speech


Page 3
development differs from both monolingual English and monolingual Spanish-speakers
development (Gildersleeve, Davis, & Stubbe, 1996). Thus, it is critical that identification of a
bilingual child as speech impaired should not be based entirely on monolingual English or
monolingual Spanish norms.
More specific patterns of bilingual phonological development have been identified by
Goldstein (2001). With a small sample of children of Puerto Rican descent, Goldstein (2001)
found that by four years of age, the only sounds this group of normally developing bilingual
children produced with less than 90% accuracy were fricatives and affricates. Phonological
processes that were common to this group of children included cluster reduction, final consonant
deletion, and unstressed syllable deletion. While this developmental information informs us
about the phonological development of a group of bilingual children of Puerto Rican descent, it is
unknown how generalizeable these data are to bilingual children from other regions and other
Spanish dialects.
Until more information about typical bilingual phonological development becomes
available, we can use the knowledge we do have to create a model that will help us predict
additional patterns. In this paper, we will present information about the phonological systems of
English and Spanish as well as a model for how these systems may interact in bilingual
development. An important tool for distinguishing between typical and atypical phonological
patterns of English language learners is a solid framework for understanding the processes
involved in learning two languages. We present the Competition Model as a useful tool for
understanding typical bilingual speech and language development. The Competition Model was


Page 4
proposed by Bates and MacWhinney (1982, 1987, 1989) to explain how individuals learn
language. In this model, the rules of language are learned through attention to specific cues.
Different cues are salient in different languages (MacWhinney, 1997). The cues that bilingual
children learn earliest are those that occur most frequently and consistently and are most
perceptually salient. Word order is commonly cited in the literature as an example of a language-
specific cue. To illustrate, consider the sentence Juan left. Subject-verb word order is the only
option for producing this sentence in English. Subject-verb-object word order as in Juan left
Susie is more common than the reverse, Susie was left by Juan. Consequently, an English
language learner can use word order cues to infer which word is the subject, verb, and object. In
contrast, word order is not as reliable a cue in Spanish. To distinguish between the subject and
object in Juan dej a Susie, which can also be produced as A Susie la dej Juan, a Spanish
language learner must attend to clitics and prepositional cues.
Although the Competition Model is generally used to explain the development of
syntactic and lexical aspects of language, the model can also be used to explain the phonological
patterns of children exposed to two languages. Many elementary school students who are
classified as limited English proficient are sequential language learners, meaning that they began
learning a first language (L1) at home before learning a second language (L2) in school. English
language learners may initially use cues from L1 when expressing themselves in L2. Using
terminology from the Competition Model, this pattern is called forward cue transfer and it occurs
when language learners have not yet figured out the cue system of the second language. Children
may not yet have learned L2 cues, so they do the next best thing, which is to use the L1 cues they


Page 5
already know. For example, in Spanish, children learn that s-clusters only occur in word initial
position when preceded by e, as in escuela (school). Before children learn that s-clusters can
occur in word initial position in English, as in the word school, children might use forward
transfer of the L1 cue (the es rule they know from Spanish) to pronounce eschool in English.
There are other types of cue transfer as well. Backward transfer occurs when children use
a strategy learned from L2 when using L1. This typically occurs as children gain proficiency in
L2. Differentiation is another pattern of language development that describes how bilinguals use
separate strategies for each language that are identical to those used by monolinguals of each
language. Children are more likely to demonstrate differentiation when they have high level of
proficiency in both languages. Finally, amalgamation refers to the application of a single set of
strategies to both languages. This is most common in communities with a large population of
bilinguals in which code-switching is a prevalent mode of communication. For the purposes of
this paper, we will focus on forward transfer because it is the most common type of transfer for
sequential bilinguals, who make up most of the bilingual elementary school population.

Spanish and English Phonological Systems
Spanish and English share a similar alphabet, but when the phonologies of the two
languages are compared, many differences are revealed. These differences will influence the
speech of Spanish-speaking children learning English. We can think of this influence in terms of
the Competition Model. Before English language learners have internalized the phonological
system in English, they may transfer what they know about Spanish to English. Knowledge of


Page 6
these differences will help the practitioner understand why some sounds in English are difficult for
English language learners to perceive and produce. Even the practitioner who does not speak
Spanish can become familiar with normal speech patterns for English language learners.
The alphabets of Spanish (according to the Real Academia Espaola) and English are
presented in the following table.
Table 1: Spanish and English alphabets

Some dictionaries also classify the trilled rr (also known as the rolled r) as a single letter.
The letters k and w do not occur in words native to the Spanish language, but may occur in
words adopted into Spanish such as karate, washatera, and sandwich. Three Spanish letters
that are not found in English are the ch, ll, and . The ch is considered a single letter in
Spanish and it represents the same sound as the two-letter combination ch in English. The ll
represents a sound that a monolingual English speaker may perceive as similar to the /j/ (y) in
English. The , represented by the phonetic symbol /n /, may be perceived as similar to the ny
sound in onion.
Vowel Systems
There are major differences between the vocalic phonemes in Spanish and English. In
Spanish, there are five vowels that consistently represent the single tense vowel sounds /a/, /e/,
Spanish a b c ch d e f g h i j k l ll m n o p q r s t u v w x y z
English a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r s t u v w x y z


Page 7
/i/, /o/, and /u/. In contrast, English vowels represent many tense and lax vowel sounds including
/a/, /d/, //, /J/, /e/, /c/, /i/, /I/, /o/, /o/, /u/, and //. English language learners may use forward
transfer by substituting tense vowels from L1 for lax vowels in English. For example, a student
might pronounce the word man with the tense /a/ as in mahn instead of the lax vowel //.
The following chart illustrates several examples of this forward transfer of Spanish vowels.
Table 2: Examples of Spanish to English forward transfer of vowels

Consonant Systems
The next chart shows the consonantal phonemes of Spanish and English by manner of
production. This list includes the primary phonemes that exist in all dialects of Spanish.
Depending on the dialect of Spanish, additional allophones of these sounds exist. However, we
will focus on the most common Spanish phonemes to discuss typical speech patterns that may
occur when speakers use forward transfer of the Spanish phonological system to communicate in
Spanish phonemes English phonemes Result
/a/
//
hat may sound like hot
/e/
/c/
get may sound like gate
/i/ /I/
hit may sound like heat
this may sound like these
/o/ /J/
//
ball may sound like bowl
bun may sound like bone
/u/
/o/
put may sound like poot


Page 8
English.
Table 3: Spanish and English consonantal phonemes

Spanish and English share several consonantal phonemes including the stop sounds /p/, /t/,
/k/, /b/, /d/, /g/, the nasals /m/, /n/, //, the fricatives /s/, /f/, the affricate /tj/ (ch), the liquid /l/, the
glide /w/, /j/, and the glottal /h/. Despite these common phonetic symbols, it is important to note
that there are differences in voicing, aspiration, and precise place of articulation for many of these
sounds that result in acoustic differences. We will now look at some of these differences in more
detail.
Stops
One prevalent difference between Spanish and English stop consonants is that Spanish stops
are less plosive, meaning that they are produced with less air pressure than their English pairs. This
is particularly noticeable with the Spanish voiceless consonants /p/, /t/, and /k/, which monolingual
Spanish English
Voiceless Voiced Voiceless Voiced
Stops /p/ /t/ /k/ /b/ /d/ /g/ /p/ /t/ /k/ /b/ /d/ /g/
Nasals
/m/ /n/ / /

/m/ /n/ //
Fricatives /s/ /f/ /x/
/} /s/ /f/ /b/ /j/ /z/ /v/ // //
Affricates
/tj/

/tj/ /d/
Liquids /l/ /l/ /r/
Tap/Trill /r/ /rr/
Glides /w/ /j/ /w/ /j/
Glottal /h/


Page 9
English speakers sometimes perceive as their voiced counterparts (/b/, /d/, and /g/). Another
difference is in voice onset time, which refers to the duration of vocal fold vibration before the
release of air pressure. When stop consonants occur in word initial position, voiceless stops in
Spanish are produced with a relatively short voice onset compared to English. In contrast, English
voiceless stops are produced with long voice onset time, and voiced stops are produced with short
voice onset time. This difference can also lead to the perception of voiced rather than voiceless
sounds. Another difference is the exact place of articulation. While the English phonemes /t/ and
/d/ are produced on the alveolar ridge above the teeth, the Spanish /t/ and /d/ are produced with the
tip of the tongue placed on the back of the teeth, giving these sounds a dentalized quality (Whitley,
1986).
As a result of differences in voice onset time and place of articulation, if an English
language learner uses forward transfer of sound production from Spanish into English, a
monolingual English listener may have difficulty distinguishing the students production of
voiceless /p/, /t/, and /k/ sounds from their voiced pairs /b/, /d/, and /g/. A childs production of
words such as bear and pear or time and dime, for example, may sound very similar.
Nasals
The nasal sounds /m/ and /n/ are produced in the same manner in Spanish and English. As
singletons, these sounds do not generally cause difficulty for Spanish speakers learning English.
However, when these sounds appear in clusters, such as the word sometimes, it is not unusual for
an English language learner to substitute the /m/ with the /n/ sound (Merino, 1992), or vice versa,
particularly when such a substitution facilitates coarticulation. Because the // sound is not one of


Page 10
the primary phonemes in Spanish, although it may occur as an allophone, particularly in Caribbean
dialects of Spanish (Goldstein, 2001), an English language learner may substitute it for the next
closest sound, which is /n/. Consequently, the word long may be pronounced as lon.
Fricatives
As you can see from the Table 3, the fricative class presents several phonetic contrasts
between Spanish and English. Though there are some similarities in the two language systems (e.g.
both contain the phonemes /s/ and /f/), there are many differences. There are two Spanish
phonemes that do not occur in English. The phoneme /x/ represents a voiceless sound produced
with velar frication. The phoneme // represents a voiced bilabial fricative. Because the English
fricatives /b/, /j/, /z/, /v/, //, and // are not primary phonemes in Spanish (except /b/ in Spain) and
are considered later developing sounds for English speakers, it would not be uncommon for a
person who is learning English to have difficulty producing these sounds.
Forward transfer may again be used to substitute Spanish phonemes for their similar English
counterparts. For example, the next closest Spanish phonemes to the English voiceless /b/ and
voiced // are voiceless /t/ and the voiced /d/, respectively. As a result, an English language learner
may pronounce the words think as tink and cloth as clot. Similarly, this may be
produced as dis. Voiced /z/ is often substituted by its voiceless pair /s/, so the word zipper may
be produced as sipper. Even though Spanish includes the orthographic v, both the letters b
and v represent the same phoneme in most dialects of Spanish. As a result, a Spanish speaker
may use forward transfer of the phoneme /b/ or //, thereby producing very as bery. Voiced /v/


Page 11
in word final position may be substituted with /f/, and so the word leave may sound like leaf.
Finally, the voiceless /j/ sound is not a primary phoneme in Spanish. The closest Spanish phoneme
is /tj/. A word like shop might be produced as chop.
Affricates
Now that you are becoming familiar with patterns of forward transfer, you may foresee that
a Spanish speaker might substitute the voiced phoneme /d/ with the /tj/ sound. This may often be
the case. However, if you listen carefully or envision the coarticulatory movements that are
required to produce a word like jump, you will notice that the glide /j/ is a coarticulatory product
of the phoneme /d/. Spanish speakers often perceive these English phonemes as allophones. As a
result, a Spanish speaker may also substitute the glide /j/ for the English /d/ sound and say yust
for just. This is a common pattern of forward transfer and is not indicative of a speech disorder.
Liquids
The written letters l and r exist in both languages. For a child of the appropriate
developmental age, the /l/ should not typically cause difficulty for the English language learner.
The /r/, on the other hand, is produced quite differently between the languages, as a rhotic or
retroflexed /r/ in English, and as a tap or trill in Spanish. A monolingual English speaker may
perceive the Spanish tap /r/ to sound like the English tap in butter, or perhaps like an English /d/.
An example of forward transfer of the Spanish tap /r/ for the English rhotic /r/ is boding for
boring.


Page 12
Glides
The bilabial glide /w/ and palatal glide /j/ occur in both Spanish and English. A listener
may hear subtle differences in the way an English language learner produces these sounds in
English. The /w/ sound may be produced with slight velar constriction. The phoneme /j/ may be
produced with varying degrees of palatalization. Also, it would not be atypical to hear the /j/ sound
pronounced more like /d/, which Spanish speakers may consider to be allophones, as mentioned
earlier. For example, yesterday could sound like jesterday.
Glottal
The written letter h is silent in Spanish. The open glottal phoneme /h/ exists in some
dialects of Spanish. For speakers for whom /h/ is not a common phoneme, the next most similar
sound is the velar fricative /x/. As a result, a child may produce a word such as horse with more
initial velar constriction than a mainstream English speaker.
Phonotactic Constraints
There are also differences in phonotactic rules about which phonemes can occur in which
positions in syllables and words and in what combinations. For example, whereas most consonants
can occur in word final position in English words, the only consonantal phonemes that occur in
word final position in Spanish are /d/, /n/, /s/, /x/, /l/, and the tap /r/ (which represent the written
letters d, n, s, z, j, l, and r). Another phonotactic difference is that consonant clusters are common
and can occur in any word position in English. In Spanish, however, consonant clusters are much
less frequent and are subject to more constraints. For example, initial /s/ clusters can only occur in


Page 13
Spanish if preceded by the letter e. As a result, English language learners may use forward
transfer and add a schwa to words beginning with /s/. For example, they may pronounce store as
estore. Further, consonant clusters cannot occur in word final position in Spanish. Children may
thus drop the final consonant in consonant clusters when producing words in English. For example,
they may reduce the final cluster in test to tes, or in sound to soun. As a result of such
differences in phonotactic constraints, the fact that a child can produce a particular sound in
Spanish does not necessarily mean the child will automatically produce that sound correctly in
English (Merino, 1992).
Summary and Conclusions
Sequential language learners are those who have acquired a first language (L1) before
learning a second language (L2). We have discussed the Competition Model in order to provide a
framework for understanding sequential language learners phonological development. Forward
cue transfer is a common process in sequential bilinguals that involves using the cues learned in
their L1 and applying that knowledge to L2. Now that you are familiar with this conceptual
framework of bilingual speech and language development, as well as the basic features of the
Spanish and English phonological systems, you will be better equipped to recognize when a childs
speech errors in English may be attributed to normal forward cue transfer from Spanish. Note that
in addition to speech, you may also observe these patterns of phonological transfer in childrens
early spelling, since children often use a phoneme-to-grapheme or sounding out strategy when they
write. As we discussed earlier, bilingual learners phonological development may differ from
English or Spanish monolingual development. Thus, monolingual norms should not be used alone,


Page 14
and skills must be assessed in both languages with consideration of the interaction between L1 and
L2.
Because of phonemic differences among various dialects of Spanish, it also will be
important to know what dialect(s) the child is exposed to in the home and community. Parents, of
course, are an excellent source for more insight into the childs speech skills and language history.
The clinician who needs assistance communicating with the parents can often find another family
member, neighbor, or teacher who can help translate. It is important to find out when, from whom,
and in what contexts the child began learning each language, if the parents and similar peers have
trouble understanding the childs speech, and what articulation errors the child produces in the
home language. If intelligibility is indeed a concern, if the childs speech patterns in both
languages are not developmentally appropriate, and if speech errors cannot be clearly explained by
cue transfer, it is likely that speech services are warranted.

References
Bates, E. & MacWhinney, B. (1982). Functionalist approaches to grammar. In E. Wanner & L.
Gleitman (Eds.), Language acquisition: The state of the art. New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Bates, E. & MacWhinney, B. (1987). Competition, variation and language learning. In B.
MacWhinney (Ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bates, E. & MacWhinney, B. (1989). Functionalism and the competition model. In B. MacWhinney
& E. Bates (Eds.), The cross-linguistic study of sentence processing (pp. 3-73). New York:


Page 15
Cambridge University Press.
Gildersleeve, C., Davis, B., & Stubbe, E. (1996, November). When monolingual rules dont apply:
Speech development in a bilingual environment. Paper presented at the American Speech-
Language-Hearing Association Convention, Seattle, WA.
Goldstein, B., & Washington, P.S. (2001). An initial investigation of phonological patterns in
typically developing 4-year old Spanish-English bilingual children. Language, Speech &
Hearing Services in the Schools, 32(3), 153-164.
Goldstein, B. (2001). Transcription of Spanish and Spanish-influenced English. Communication
Disorders Quarterly, 23(1), 54-60.
Iglesias, A. & Goldstein, B. (1998). Language and dialectal variations. In J. Bernthal & N. Bankson
(Eds.), Articulation and phonological disorders (4
th
ed., pp. 148-171), Needham Heights,
MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Kindler, A. (2002). Survey of the states' limited English proficient students and available
educational programs and services, 2000-2001 summary report. National Clearinghouse for
English Language Acquisition & Language Instruction Educational Programs.
MacWhinney B. (1997). Second language acquisition and the Competition Model. In A.M.B. de
Groot & J. Kroll (Eds.), Tutorials in bilingualism: Psychological perspectives (pp. 169-
199). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Merino, B. (1992). Acquisition of syntactic and phonological features in Spanish. In H.W. Langdon
& L. Cheng (Eds.), Hispanic children and adults with communication disorders (pp. 57-98).


Page 16
Gaithersburg, MD: Aspen Publishers.
Whitley, M.S. (Ed.). (1986). Spanish/English contrasts: A course in Spanish linguistics.
Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.

You might also like